17 01CaballoAssessing223
17 01CaballoAssessing223
net/publication/269519251
CITATION READS
1 5,371
5 authors, including:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Pablo J. Olivares-Olivares on 11 March 2019.
Abstract
The self-reporting of social skills continues to be assessed through the
assertiveness inventories developed in the 1970s, such as the Rathus
Assertiveness Schedule (RAS), the Assertion Inventory (AI), and the College Self-
Expression Scale (CSES). The study reported here involved 421 university students
(76.5% women) and obtained the factor structures of the aforementioned
instruments, plus the new Social Skills Questionnaire (SSQ-I) (Cuestionario de
habilidades sociales, CHASO-I). The factorial solutions obtained were 6, 8, 11 and
12 factors, respectively. The reliability (Guttman split-half and Cronbach’s alpha)
of all the questionnaires was high, and the correlations between the CHASO-I
and all the other questionnaires were moderate. The sex differences found
involved the total scores of the RAS and the factors “Speaking or performing in
public/Interacting with figures in authority”, “Interacting with persons I am
attracted to”, and “Interacting with strangers”, with men being more skilled than
women, and the factor “Apologizing/Recognizing their own mistakes”, with
women being more skilled than men. The study concluded by recognizing certain
common problems affecting the self-report measures of social skills, as well as
certain advantages of the new CHASO-I.
KEY WORDS: social skills, factor structure, questionnaires, assertiveness, sex
differences.
Resumen
La evaluación de las habilidades sociales por medio de medidas de
autoinforme sigue realizándose con instrumentos desarrollados en los años 70,
como el “Inventario de asertividad de Rathus” (RAS), el “Inventario de aserción”
(AI) o la “Escala de autoexpresión universitaria” (CSES). Este estudio llevado a
cabo con 421 estudiantes universitarios (76,5% mujeres) halló las estructuras
factoriales de dichos instrumentos así como del nuevo “Cuestionario de
This study was partially supported by a grant from the European Regional Development Fund (ERDF)
and Junta de Andalucía as part of the “Fortalecimiento de la I+D+i” program of the University of
Granada.
Correspondence: Vicente E. Caballo, Faculty of Psychology, University of Granada, 18071 Granada
(Spain). E-mail: [email protected]
376 CABALLO, SALAZAR, OLIVARES, IRURTIA, OLIVARES, AND TOLEDO
Introduction
Interest in the assessment of social skills peaked in the 1970s and 1980s
(usually under the name of “assertiveness” or “assertion” assessment), and today
we still profit from the major research conducted in those years. Furthermore, the
assessment of social skills is, still today as it was then (see Caballo, 1986, 1997), a
controversial issue pending general consensus among experts in the field. Like
then, too, we may refer to the problem with a meaningful title reflecting its
difficulty: “Pandora's Box reopened? The assessment of social skills” (Curran,
1979).
If we focus on self-report measures assessing social skills, we find that most
of the questionnaires used today were developed in those years. In spite of the
time lapse, it is unusual to find relevant research describing the psychometric
properties of those measures, leaving behind a statistical “limbo”. In fact, as
opposed to other areas, we do not have a self-report assessment measure that
acts as a gold standard for comparing or validating other measures. This may be
due, at least partially, to the fact that many of the questionnaires were developed
with a clinical application in mind (e.g., assessing the effectiveness of assertiveness
or social skills training), or as an ad hoc tool for specific research, but without
considering a subsequent and wider application.
The fact is that we are currently facing several important issues in regard to
the psychometric nature of the social skills self-report measures. One such issue
involves factorial validity. There seems to be some consensus that when speaking
about social skills we are taking a series of response classes or behavioral
dimensions for granted (Caballo, 1997; Galassi & Galassi, 1977; Lazarus, 1973).
Social skills assessment questionnaires should include such dimensions. The
problem is that there is no consensus over their number and nature. As many as
14 possible different dimensions or response classes have been proposed (see
Caballo, 1997, for a review), but they are not included in any self-report
assessment measures, especially not as non-overlapping independent entities. An
analysis of the factorial structure of social skills questionnaires has found that the
number of identified factors varies according to the specific measure and the
Assessing social skills with four self-report measures 377
particular study (somewhat akin to what happens in the field of social anxiety, as
reported by Caballo, Salazar, Irurtia, Arias, & Nobre [2013], although it is much
more evident in the social skills field).
There are no stable factorial structures using the same questionnaire. For
instance, in the case of the Rathus Assertiveness Schedule (RAS; Rathus, 1973),
there are very different factorial solutions, varying from four (Heimberg & Harrison,
1980), through seven (Carrasco, Clemente, & Llavona, 1983), eight and nine
(Nevid & Rathus, 1979), up to 12 factors (Hull & Hull, 1978), even though Rathus
published the scale as a general measure of assertiveness. Something similar
happens with the Adult Self-Expression Scale (ASES; Gay, Hollandsworth, Jr., &
Galassi, 1975), where the authors reported a structure of 14 factors, although
LaFromboise (1983) subsequently found several factorial solutions (14, 13, and,
finally, three) in an attempt to reduce the number of items. The same may be said
regarding the Personal Relations Inventory (PRI; Lorr & More, 1980), in which the
authors proposed a 4-factor solution, although different solutions were found in
further studies that included samples from other countries, such as the nine factors
in Australia (Heaven, 1984) or the five factors in Chile (Carmona & Lorr, 1992).
The Assertion Inventory (AI; Gambrill & Richey, 1975) also varies significantly
regarding the number of factors depending on the studies: 11 factors in the
former original study, 13 factors according to Carrasco, Clemente, and Llavona
(1989), and eight in the study by Castaños, Reyes, Rivera, and Díaz (2011). The
same thing happens with the College Self-Expression Scale (CSES; Galassi, DeLo,
Galassi, & Bastien, 1974), in which four (Kipper & Jaffe, 1978), six (Galassi &
Galassi, 1979), 11 (Caballo & Buela, 1988), and even 14 (Henderson & Furnham,
1983) factors have been found.
The inconsistency of the factorial structures of the self-report measures of
social skills may be due to a variety of reasons. One could be that too many factors
are often extracted and retained, in spite of having very few items (one or two), as
was the case, for example, with the AI. Another reason is that there are no
consistent standards for retaining specific items for each factor, that is, no attempt
is made to retain only those items with loadings above a particular score on a
factor (e.g., .35 or more) or, less frequently, to discard those items with high
loadings on two or more factors. Thus, in the specific case of the Wolpe-Lazarus
Assertiveness Scale (WLAS; Wolpe & Lazarus, 1966), while Henderson and
Furnham (1983) used .40 as the loading criterion to include an item in a factor,
Hersen et al. (1979) used .25, and Kogan et al. (1996) used .30. In the case of the
CSES, Kipper and Jaffe (1978) used .25, while Galassi and Galassi (1979) used .35
as the loading criterion.
A further problem with social skills questionnaires is that identical names are
often given to two separate factors in spite of their different composition. For
instance, Hersen et al. (1979) analyzed the factorial structure of the WLAS
separating males from females, and although the solution in both samples
revealed 10 factors, the first factor (which explained the highest variance
percentage in both samples), called “General Expressivity”, contained different
items. The items in the male sample were 12, 14, 16, 17, 28, and 29; and 5, 11,
16 and 28 in the female sample. This first factor shared only two items in these
378 CABALLO, SALAZAR, OLIVARES, IRURTIA, OLIVARES, AND TOLEDO
samples, but was given the same name in both cases. Additionally, three out of
the six items that formed the factor in the males sample loaded also on another
factor. The situation seems to deteriorate when analyzing a combined male and
female sample with the same questionnaire (WLAS), as in the work by Henderson
and Furnham (1983), where that “same” factor has a very different composition
to the one found in the work by Hersen et al. (1979), including items 12, 15, 21
and 22. In sum, although both studies found the same factorial solution, the items
included in each factor do not match, and the variance explained by the main
factors differs significantly. This can also be seen with the AI in the research by
Gambrill and Richey (1975), and Henderson and Furnham (1983), in which the
solution was 11 factors in each one, but only three factors have a major
concordance, some concordance in four, and none at all in five of the factors.
Recently developed questionnaires share the same problems. For instance, in
the case of the Social Skills Inventory (Inventário de Habilidades Sociais, IHS; Del
Prette, Del Prete, & Barreto, 1998) the same factorial solution was proposed (five
factors) in two studies (Del Prette et al., 1998; Olaz, Medrano, Greco, & Del Prette,
2009), but there was no coincidence of the items in each factor. Only in one factor
did most items coincide, with partial coincidence in three of the factors (fewer
than half of the items), while the fifth factor is completely different in both studies.
Considering the problems mentioned in this review, we decided to analyze
the factorial structure of three assertiveness self-report measures that are widely
used around the world today, namely, the RAS, the AI, and the CSES, as well as
the Social Skills Questionnaire (SSQ-I) (Cuestionario de habilidades sociales-I,
CHASO-I), a new measure recently developed by our team to evaluate social skills.
Method
Participants
The sample consisted of 421 participants with a mean age of 22.54 years
(SD= 6.75), being made up of 322 females (M= 22.25 years, SD= 6.01) and 99
males (M= 23.49, SD= 8.70). They were mostly university students (93.82%). Of
these, 246 were Psychology students, and 149 were students in other subjects. As
regards the rest of the sample (6.18%), one was a psychologist, seven were
workers with other university degrees, four were workers without university
studies, two were pre-university students, and eight were undefined (e.g., jobless,
retired, etc.) There were no data on four of the participants.
However, not all the subjects completed all the questionnaires. The entire
sample answered the Social Skills Questionnaire (SSQ-I) (Cuestionario de
habilidades sociales, CHASO-I) and the Rathus Assertiveness Schedule (RAS), but
the participants only partially completed the Assertion Inventory (AI) and the
College Self-Expression Scale (CSES). In fact, the AI was answered by 228 subjects
with a mean age of 21.62 years (SD= 7.00), of whom 170 were females (M=
21.26 years, SD= 5.91) and 50 were males (M= 22.67 years, SD= 9.48), while the
CSES was completed by 117 subjects, with a mean age of 21.53 years (SD= 6.82),
Assessing social skills with four self-report measures 379
and consisted of 88 females (M= 21.08 years, SD= 5.27) and 29 males (M= 22.93
years, SD= 10.19).
Instruments
from 1 (“Always do it”) to 5 (“Never do it”); the higher the score, the lower
the assertiveness. The authors of the questionnaire found a test-retest
reliability of .81 for the Response Probability subscale (Gambrill & Richey,
1975). Reports on this subscale in Spanish samples have found reasonable
test-retest reliability (r = .84) and internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = .88)
(Carrasco et al., 1989; Casas-Anguera et al., 2014).
College Self-Expression Scale (CSES; Galassi, Delo, Galassi, & Bastien, 1974).
This scale contains 50 items using a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (“Almost
always or always”) to 4 (“Never or rarely”). Twenty-nine items are worded so
that they require reverse scoring. Scores on the 50 items are summed to yield
a total score, with higher total scores indicating more assertion. Its
psychometric properties have been considered as appropriate. The test-retest
reliability found varies between .89 and .90 (Galassi et al., 1974). The test-
retest reliability found with a Spanish university sample was .87, and the
internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) was .89 (Caballo & Buela, 1988).
Procedure
Statistical analysis
Results
The EFA identified 12 factors with eigenvalues higher than 1.00 as the best
factor solution for the SSQ-I, explaining 48.05% of the cumulative variance. Table
1 shows these 12 factors, each including two of the most representative items of
the corresponding factor fulfilling the following criteria: 1) a high loading on the
Assessing social skills with four self-report measures 381
factor (and always higher than .40), and 2) helping to operationally define the
factor. Furthermore, the eigenvalues and explained variance of each factor are
included.
Table 1
Exploratory factor analysis of the Social Skills Questionnaire (SSQ-I) and loadings of two
representative items for each factor (N= 421)
The 12 factors and the items loading on each one of them were the
following: Factor 1: Speaking or performing in public/Interacting with people in
Assessing social skills with four self-report measures 383
authority (items 3, 26, 31, 35, 37, 49, 62, 67, 69, 78, 94, 102); Factor 2:
Expressing positive feelings (items 9, 18, 56, 64, 81, 83, 89, 92, 96, 99, 101, 105,
113); Factor 3: Refusing requests (items 8, 21, 65, 100, 106, 111); Factor 4:
Interacting with persons I am attracted to (items 5, 13, 22, 30, 36, 50, 79); Factor
5: Dancing or singing in public (items 19, 71); Factor 6: Disclosing information
about myself to close persons (items 45, 54, 107, 116); Factor 7: Asking an
attendant or a stranger for something (items 43, 46, 80); Factor 8: Expressing
annoyance, disgust, or displeasure (items 4, 12, 14, 23, 24, 32, 40, 86, 90); Factor
9: Expressing different opinions/Clarifying opinions (items 7, 75, 87, 91, 104);
Factor 10: Apologizing/Recognizing my own mistakes (items 2, 15, 17, 20, 48, 93,
115, 118); Factor 11: Interacting with strangers (items 16, 34, 47, 57, 84, 103,
110, 114); Factor 12: Thanking for congratulations/Arguing with salespeople
(items 38, 82). The remaining items did not load above .40 on any factor (except
for three items loading on two factors and remaining outside the factorial
solution).
The questionnaire’s internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) was .97 and the
split-half reliability (Guttman) was .96.
The EFA identified six factors with eigenvalues higher than 1.00 as the best
factor solution for the RAS, explaining 44.79% of the cumulative variance. Table 2
shows these six factors, each including two of the most representative items of the
corresponding factor fulfilling the same criteria as with the SSQ-I. Furthermore, the
eigenvalues and explained variance of each factor are included.
Table 2
Exploratory factor analysis of the Rathus Assertiveness Schedule (RAS; Rathus, 1973) and
loadings of two representative items for each factor (N= 421)
The six factors and the items loading on each one of them were the
following: Factor 1: Interacting with commercial firms (items 12, 13, 14, 16, 17);
Factor 2: Expressing annoyance or displeasure (items 3, 25, 27, 28). Factor 3:
Defending my own position (items 6, 7, 8, 22); Factor 4: Interacting with others in
an extroverted way (items 1, 2, 10, 11, 18, 30); Factor 5: Expressing feelings
openly (items 20, 21, 24, 26); Factor 6: Refusing requests (items 4, 5, 15, 23).
Items 9, 19 and 20 did not load above .40 on any factor, or did so on two or more
factors.
The internal consistency of the questionnaire (Cronbach’s alpha) was .82 and
the split-half reliability (Guttman) was .80.
The EFA identified eight factors with eigenvalues higher than 1.00 as the best
factor solution for the AI (subscale of Response Probability), explaining 52.34% of
the cumulative variance. Table 3 shows these eight factors, each including two of
the most representative items of the corresponding factor fulfilling the same
criteria as with the SSQ-I. Furthermore, the eigenvalues and explained variance of
each factor are included.
The eight factors and the items loading on each one of them were the
following: Factor 1: Expressing annoyance, disgust or displeasure (items 13, 28,
35, 39, 40); Factor 2: Making requests to potential employers (items 9, 17, 18, 33);
Factor 3: Giving and receiving compliments, requests and asking personal
questions (items 2, 3, 12, 16, 21, 29, 30, 37); Factor 4: Resisting undesirable
propositions (items 10, 25, 27, 31, 32, 34); Factor 5: Resisting consumer
propositions (items 4, 6, 23); Factor 6: Refusing requests involving my own
Assessing social skills with four self-report measures 385
Table 3
Exploratory factor analysis of the subscale “Response probability” of the Assertion Inventory
(AI; Gambrill & Richey, 1975) and loadings of two representative items for each factor (n=
228)
Factor 6. Refusing requests involving my own property and asking for it to be returned
(eigenvalue: 1.38; explained variance: 3.45%)
11. Turn down a request to
.08 .27 .05 .06 .06 .70 -.07 -.03 .33
borrow money
1. Turn down a request to
-.04 -.12 .05 .02 .49 .53 -.00 .16 .24
borrow your car
Factor 7. Apologizing/Recognizing my own mistakes (eigenvalue: 1.32; explained variance:
3.29%)
5. Apologize when you are
-.14 .02 .22 .19 .17 -.12 .65 .16 .32
at fault
7. Admit fear and request
.19 .26 .33 .07 .14 .05 .47 -.24 .45
consideration
Factor 8. Expressing different opinions/Clarifying opinions (eigenvalue: 1.27; explained
variance: 3.17%)
38. Continue to converse
with someone who .20 .13 .19 .20 .15 -.01 .16 .55 .48
disagrees with you
26. Express an opinion that
differs from that of the
.32 -.01 .25 .27 .10 .06 .15 .51 .53
person with whom you are
talking
Note: AI-PR= Assertion Inventory-Response probability; ri-t= item-total correlations.
The EFA identified 11 factors with eigenvalues higher than 1.00 as the best
factor solution for the CSES, explaining 56.86% of the cumulative variance. Table
4 shows these 11 factors, with each one including two of the most representative
items of the corresponding factor fulfilling the same criteria as with the SSQ-I.
Furthermore, the eigenvalues and explained variance of each factor are included.
The 11 factors and the items loading on each one of them were the
following: Factor 1: Expressing annoyance, disgust, or displeasure (items 6, 9, 13,
18, 23, 28, 30, 32, 38, 41, 47, 48); Factor 2: Expressing positive feelings (items 10,
20, 36, 49); Factor 3. Speaking in public or in front of the class (items 21, 33, 43,
50); Factor 4: Defending personal preferences before my parents (items 29, 46);
Factor 5: Giving compliments to friends (items 25, 26, 31); Factor 6: Interacting
with the opposite sex (items 22, 39, 44); Factor 7: Defending my own rights before
friends/roommates (items 11, 16, 27, 34, 45); Factor 8: Being careful not to hurt
other people’s feelings (items 5, 7, 12, 24); Factor 9: Making requests to close
people (items 2, 8, 37, 40); Factor 10: Defending my consumer rights (items 1, 15,
35); Factor 11: Defending my own decisions before my parents (item 4). Items 3,
14, 17, 19 and 42 did not load above .40 on any factor, or did so on two or more
factors.
The questionnaire’s internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) was .88 and the
split-half reliability (Guttman) was .90.
Assessing social skills with four self-report measures 387
Table 4
Exploratory factor analysis of the College Self-Expression Scale (CSES; Galassi et al., 1974)
and loadings of two representative items for each factor (n= 117)
correlation of .77, as well as (at least partially) in the RAS (r= .54). We did not find
this factor in the AI.
Table 5
Correlations between the Social Skills Questionnaire-I (SSQ-I) and its factors, and the overall
score of the social skills questionnaires used in this study
CHASO-I factor 2, “Expressing positive feelings” was also a factor in the other
three questionnaires cited here with a high correlation with the CSES (r= .65), and
a moderate one with the AI (r= -.56) and the RAS (r= .42). We should remember
that the negative correlation with the AI is due to the fact that the higher the
score in the AI, the lower the assertiveness or response probability.
The relationships between the rest of the CHASO-I factors and similar factors
in the other three measures are usually moderate, generally between .40 and .60,
although some CHASO-I factors do not pair with similar factors in all the other
questionnaires. On the other hand, the correlation among similar factors in the
three traditional social skills questionnaires also ranged from .39 to .59, except the
relationship between the RAS factor 2, “Expressing annoyance or displeasure”,
and factor 10 of the CSES, “Defending my consumer rights”, which had a high
correlation (r= .73).
Assessing social skills with four self-report measures 391
Table 6
Relationships between the factors of the Social Skills Questionnaire-I and similar factors
from the other three social skills questionnaires
We also calculated the differences between men and women in the factors
and in the total score of the CHASO-I and the other three social skills
questionnaires (RAS, AI, and CSES). There are few differences, and they are found
only in some of the CHASO-I factors, such as “Speaking or performing in
public/Interacting with people in authority”, “Interacting with persons I am
attracted to”, “Interacting with strangers” (with men scoring higher than women
in all of them) or “Apologizing/Recognizing my own mistakes” (with women
scoring higher than men). There were no sex differences in the total score for
three of the questionnaires. Only in the RAS total score were there statistically
significant differences (although with a low effect size, d= 0.43) between men and
women (with men scoring higher than women) (see table 7). Due to space limitations,
the sign of U has not been included.
392 CABALLO, SALAZAR, OLIVARES, IRURTIA, OLIVARES, AND TOLEDO
Table 7
Differences between men and women in the four self-report measures of social skills
Factors and total scores for the various measures of Men Women
t p d
social skills M (SD) M (SD)
Social Skills Questionaire (SSQ-I)
F1. Speaking or performing in public/Interacting 37.70 34.90
2.77 .006 0.33
with people in authority (8.38) (8.64)
49.02 50.53
F2. Expressing positive feelings 1.59 .112 0.18
(8.40) (8.00)
21.00 20.55
F3. Refusing requests 0.88 .380 0.10
(4.28) (4.39)
21.67 17.89
F4. Interacting with persons I am attracted to 5.19 .000 0.58
(6.86) (6.05)
5.31 5.74
F5. Dancing or singing in public 1.95 .051 0.22
(1.92) (1.89)
F6. Disclosing information about myself to close 13.99 13.96
0.08 .938 0.01
persons (3.02) (3.03)
11.40 11.54
F7. Asking an attendant or a stranger for something 0.52 .606 0.06
(2.58) (2.32)
30.55 29.61
F8. Expressing annoyance, disgust, or displeasure 1.35 .177 0.16
(5.99) (5.98)
19.61 19.23
F9. Expressing different opinions/Clarifying opinions 1.03 .304 0.12
(3.46) (3.12)
31.80 33.13
F10. Apologizing/Recognizing my own mistakes 2.34 .020 0.25
(5.76) (4.58)
27.07 25.47
F11. Interacting with strangers 2.18 .030 0.26
(6.02) (6.34)
F12. Thanking for congratulations/Arguing with 6.50 6.52
0.11 .905 0.01
salespeople (1.74) (1.61)
273.94 270.00
SSQ-I total score (12 factors) 0.83 .405 0.10
(39.90) (37.20)
Rathus Assertiveness Schedule (RAS)
3.50 1.42
F1. Interacting with commercial firms 2.82 .005 0.33
(6.10) (6.44)
-0.35 -1.83
F2. Expressing annoyance or displeasure 2.34 .020 0.27
(5.37) (5.49)
0.89 -1.41
F4. Interacting with others in an extroverted way 3.02 .003 0.35
(6.81) (6.44)
-0.77 -2.04
F6. Refusing requests 2.33 .020 0.27
(4.85) (4.62)
10.08 1.43
RAS total score 3.35 .001 0.43
(18.39) (22.01)
Assertion Inventory (AI)
99.40 101.78
IA-RP total score 0.76 .449 0.11
(24.47) (17.69)
College Self-Expression Scale (CSES)
9.48 7.28
F3. Speaking in public or in front of the class 3.04 .003 0.66
(3.20) (3.42)
7.65 6.42
F6. Interacting with the opposite sex 2.13 .035 0.48
(2.27) (2.81)
131.00 127.63
CSES total score 0.74 .460 0.16
(19.61) (21.27)
Assessing social skills with four self-report measures 393
Discussion
References
Beck, J. G., & Heimberg, R. G. (1983). Self-report assessment of assertive behavior: A critical
analysis. Behavior Modification, 7, 451 487.
Burkhart, B. R., Green, S. B., & Harrison, W. H. (1979). Measurement of assertive behavior:
Construct and predictive validity of self-report, role-playing and in-vivo measures.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 35, 376-383.
Caballo. V. E. (1986). Evaluación de las habilidades sociales. In R. Fernández Ballesteros, & J.
A. Carrobles (Eds.), Evaluación conductual: Métodos y aplicaciones (3th ed.) (pp. 553-
595). Madrid: Pirámide.
Caballo, V. E. (1993). Relationships among some behavioral and self-report measures of
social skills. Psicología Conductual, 1, 69-91.
Caballo, V. E. (1997). Manual de evaluación y entrenamiento de las habilidades sociales (2nd
ed.). Madrid: Siglo XXI.
Caballo, V. E., & Buela, G. (1988). Factor analyzing the College Self-Expression Scale with a
Spanish population. Psychological Reports, 63, 503-507.
Caballo, V. E., Salazar, I. C., Arias, B., Irurtia, M. J., Calderero, M., & CISO-A Research Team
Spain (2010). Validation of the Social Anxiety Questionnaire for Adults (SAQ-A30) with
Spanish university students: Similarities and differences among degree subjects and
regions. Behavioral Psychology/Psicología Conductual, 18, 5-34.
Caballo, V. E., Arias, B., Salazar, I. C., Calderero, M., Irurtia, M. J., & Ollendick, T. H. (2012).
A new self-report assessment measure of social phobia/anxiety in children: The Social
Anxiety Questionnaire for Children (SAQ-C24). Behavioral Psychology/Psicología
Conductual, 20, 485-503.
Caballo, V. E., Salazar, I. C., & Irurtia, M. J. (2014). Social Skills Questionnaire (SSQ-I)
(“Cuestionario de habilidades sociales”, CHASO-I). Unpublished manuscript.
Caballo, V. E., Salazar, I. C., Irurtia, M. J., Arias, B., Hofmann, S. G., & CISO-A Research
Team (2008). Social anxiety in 18 nations: Sex and age differences. Behavioral
Psychology/Psicología Conductual, 16, 163-187.
Caballo, V. E., Salazar, I. C., Irurtia, M. J., Arias, B., Hofmann, S. G., & CISO-A Research
Team (2012). The multidimensional nature and multicultural validity of a new measure
of social anxiety: The Social Anxiety Questionnaire for Adults. Behavior Therapy, 43,
313-328.
Caballo, V. E., Salazar, I. C., Irurtia, M. J., Arias, B., Hofmann, S. G., & CISO-A Research
Team (2014). Differences in social anxiety between men and women across 18
countries. Personality and Individual Differences, 64, 35-40.
Caballo, V. E., Salazar, I. C., Irurtia, M. J., Arias, B., & Nobre, L. (2013). The assessment of
social anxiety through five self-report measures, LSAS-SR, SPAI, SPIN, SPS, and SIAS: A
critical analysis of their factor structure. Behavioral Psychology/Psicología Conductual,
21, 423-448.
Caballo, V. E., Salazar, I. C., Irurtia, M. J., Olivares, P., & Olivares, J. (2014). The relationship
between social skills and social anxiety and personality styles/disorders. Behavioral
Psychology/ Psicología Conductual, 22, 401-422.
Carmona, A. E., & Lorr, M. (1992). Dimensions of assertiveness: A cross-cultural comparison
of Chilean and U.S. subjects. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 45-48.
Carrasco, I., Clemente, M., & LLavona, L. (1983). Análisis de componentes básicos del
Inventario de asertividad de Rathus. Revista Española de Terapia del Comportamiento,
1, 249-264.
Carrasco, I., Clemente, M., & Llavona, L. (1989). Análisis del Inventario de aserción de
Gambrill y Richey. Estudios de Psicología, 37, 63-74.
398 CABALLO, SALAZAR, OLIVARES, IRURTIA, OLIVARES, AND TOLEDO
Casas-Anguera, E., Prat, G., Vilamala, S., Escandell, M. J., García-Franco, M., Martín, J. R.,
López, E., & Ochoa, S. (2014). Validación de la versión española del Inventario de
asertividad Gambrill y Richey en población con diagnóstico de esquizofrenia. Anales de
Psicología, 30, 431-437.
Castaños Cervantes, S., Reyes Lagunes, I., Rivera Aragón, S., & Díaz loving, R. (2011).
Estandarización del Inventario de asertividad de Gambrill y Richey-II. Revista
Iberoamericana de Diagnóstico y Evaluación Psicológica, 29, 27-50.
Curran, J. P. (1979). Pandora's box reopened? The assessment of social skills. Journal of
Behavioral Assessment, 1, 55-71.
Del Prette, Z. A. P., Del Prette, A., & Barreto, M. C. M. (1998). Análise de um Inventário de
Habilidades Sociais (IHS) em uma amostra de universitários. Psicologia: Teoria e
Pesquisa, 14, 219-228.
Furnham, A., & Henderson, M. (1984). Assessing assertiveness: A content and correlational
analysis of five assertiveness inventories. Behavioral Assessment, 6, 79-88.
Galassi, J. P., DeLo, J. S., Galassi, M. D., & Bastien, S. (1974). The College Self-Expression
Scale: A measure of assertiveness. Behavior Therapy, 5, 164-171.
Galassi, J. P., & Galassi, M. D. (1973). A factor analysis of a measure of assertiveness.
Unpublished manuscript, West Virginia University, USA.
Galassi, J. P., & Galassi, M. D. (1975). Relationship between assertiveness and
aggressiveness. Psychological Reports, 36, 352-354.
Galassi, J. P., & Galassi, M. D. (1977). Assessment procedures for assertive behavior. En R. E.
Alberti (Ed.), Assertiveness: Innovations, applications, issues (pp. 307-325). San Luis
Obispo, CA: Impact.
Galassi, J. P., & Galassi, M. D. (1979). A comparison of the factor structure of an assertion
scale across sex and population. Behavior Therapy, 10, 117-128.
Gambrill, E. D., & Richey, C. A. (1975). An assertion inventory for use in assessment and
research. Behavior Therapy, 6, 550-561.
Gay, M. L., Hollandsworth, J. G. Jr., & Galassi, J. P. (1975). An assertiveness inventory for
adults. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 22, 340-344.
Gritzmacher, K. K., & Tucker, R. K. (1979, May). A multivariate investigation of selected
assertiveness instruments. Paper presented at the annual convention of the
International Communication Association, Philadelphia, USA.
Heaven, P. C. L. (1984). Factor structure of the Lorr and More Assertiveness Inventory.
Personality and Individual Differences, 5, 741-742.
Heimberg, R. G., & Harrison, D. F. (1980). Use of the Rathus Assertiveness Schedule with
offenders: A question of questions. Behavior Therapy, 11, 278-281.
Henderson, M., & Furnham, A. (1983). Dimensions of assertiveness: Factor analysis of five
assertion inventories. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 14,
223-231.
Hersen, M., Bellack, A. S., Turner, S. M., Williams, M. T., Harper, K., & Watts, J. G. (1979).
Psychometric properties of the Wolpe-Lazarus Assertiveness Scale. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 17, 63-49.
Hull, D. B., & Hull, J. H. (1978). Rathus Assertiveness Schedule: Normative and factor-
analytic data. Behavior Therapy, 9, 673.
Jakubowski, P. A., & Lacks, P. B. (1975). Assessment procedures in assertion training. The
Counseling Psychologist, 5, 84-90.
Kipper, D. A., & Jaffe, Y. (1978). Dimensions of assertiveness: Factors underlying the College
Self-Expression Scale. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 46, 47-52.
Kogan, E. S., Hersen, M., Kabacoff, R. I., & Van Hasselt, V. B. (1996). Psychometric
properties of the Wolpe-Lazarus Assertiveness Scale with community-dwelling older
adults. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 17, 97-109.
Assessing social skills with four self-report measures 399
LaFromboise, T. D. (1983). The factorial validity of the Adult Self Expression Scale with
American Indians. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 43, 547-555.
Lazarus, A. A. (1973). On assertive behavior: A brief note. Behavior Therapy, 4, 697-699.
Lorr, M., & More, W. W. (1980). Four dimensions of assertiveness. Multivariate Behavioral
Research, 15, 127-138.
Nevid, J. S., & Rathus, S. A. (1979). Factor analysis of the Rathus Assertiveness Schedule
with a college population. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry,
10, 21-24.
Olaz, F. O., Medrano, L., Greco, M. E., & Del Prette, Z. A. P. (2009). Argentinean adaptation
of the Social Skills Inventory IHS-Del Prette. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 12,
756-766.
Olivares, J., Olivares-Olivares, P. J., & Macià, D. (2014). Entrenamiento en habilidades
sociales y tratamiento de adolescentes con fobia social generalizada. Behavioral
Psychology/Psicología Conductual, 22, 441-459.
Rathus, S. A. (1973). A 30-item schedule for assessing assertive behavior. Behavior Therapy,
4, 398-406.
Statsoft (2013). Statistica, v. 12 (statistical program). Tulsa, OK: Statsoft.
Stevens, J. (1986). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Vaal, J. (1975). The Rathus Assertiveness Schedule: Reliability at the junior high school level.
Behavior Therapy 6, 566-567.
Wolpe, J., & Lazarus, A. A. (1966). Behavior therapy techniques: A guide to the treatment
of neurosis. Oxford: Pergamon.