Jaakkola 2015
Jaakkola 2015
To cite this article: Timo Jaakkola, Sami Yli-Piipari, Vassilis Barkoukis & Jarmo Liukkonen (2015):
Relationships among perceived motivational climate, motivational regulations, enjoyment, and
PA participation among Finnish physical education students, International Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology, DOI: 10.1080/1612197X.2015.1100209
Physical inactivity has been one of the leading determinants of the doubling of the worldwide
prevalence of obesity since 1980 (World Health Organization, 2013). It has been argued that
schools and physical education (PE) are the most cost-effective channels to affect public health
because of schools’ potential to reach the whole age cohort of children and adolescents (Sallis
et al., 2012). However, due to limited amount of weekly PE classes, physical educators’ resources
to engage their students in recommended daily 60 minutes of moderate-to-vigorous physical
activity (PA) (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2008) are inadequate. Thus,
the examination of the pathways through which motivational and positive PE experiences transfer
into physically active lifestyle is well warranted. In addition, the examination of the role of
motivational climate in children and adolescent PA has been suggested as one of the top ten
research questions in PE domain (Chen, 2013). Thus, evolving from the current state of affairs
and grounded in self-determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985) and achievement goal
theory (AGT; Nicholls, 1989), this study had two-fold purposes. First, we aimed to examine
how teacher-initiated motivational climate can influence student motivation and affect in PE
along with their PA participation, and whether motivational climate has a longitudinal effect
across middle school. Second, we aimed to examine the role of positive affect in explaining
the relationship between motivation in PE and out-of-school PA participation.
Self-determination theory
SDT is a prominent theory used to understand human motivation in PA settings. The theory offers
an explanatory framework for the understanding of the quality of motivation behind volitional and
satisfying behaviours (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2000; Deci, Ryan, & Guay, 2013). SDT identifies three
major types of motivational regulations existing in any life context: intrinsic motivation, extrinsic
Downloaded by [UNSW Library] at 21:18 22 April 2016
motivation and amotivation1 (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Intrinsic motivation represents engagement in
an activity for the pleasure and excitement, whereas extrinsic motivation has been typically
divided into three motivational regulations corresponding to different levels of autonomy (Deci
& Ryan, 2000). In the most autonomous regulation, which is labelled identified regulation, par-
ticipation in the activity is a result of the individual’s choice as he or she acknowledges the value
and importance of the activity for the self. Introjected regulation describes behaviours which have
been only slightly internalised and which are performed out of feelings of guilt or shame. The
least autonomous regulation of extrinsic motivation is external regulation which describes invol-
vement in an activity due to external demands and contingencies, such as obtaining rewards,
avoiding punishment, or any other activity due to external pressure. In addition, amotivation,
which reflects a lack of either intrinsic or extrinsic motivation, is a construct similar to learned
helplessness and it corresponds to an absence of reasoning and intention to carry out behaviour
(Deci & Ryan, 1985, 2012). In this study the term autonomous motivational regulations refers to
intrinsic motivation and identified regulation whereas the term less autonomous motivational
regulations refers to introjected and extrinsic regulation. SDT has been extensively studied in
PE and evidence has consistently shown that autonomous motivation is associated with positive
cognitive (e.g. concentration), affective (e.g. enjoyment), and behavioural (e.g. effort, PA partici-
pation) responses during lesson participation as compared to less autonomous motivation (see
Vallerand, 1997, 2007).
perceptions of learning-oriented climate promote mastery of skills and personal improvement and
perceptions of performance-oriented climate advance social comparison and focus on obtaining
performance attainments (Duda & Balaguer, 2007).
Perceptions of learning-oriented climate, as postulated in AGT, result in positive conse-
quences as compared to perceptions of a performance-oriented climate (Braithwaite, Spray, &
Warburton, 2011). Empirical evidence has shown that learning-oriented climate relates positively
to enjoyment (Barkoukis, Ntoumanis, & Thøgersen-Ntoumani, 2010; Liukkonen, Barkoukis,
Watt, & Jaakkola, 2010) and out-of-school PA participation (Barkoukis & Hagger, 2013),
whereas research evidence has indicated that perceptions of performance-oriented climate
display a neutral or negative effect on these outcomes (Barkoukis & Hagger, 2013). Studies
have also demonstrated that learning-oriented climate is positively associated with autonomous
motivation (Ommundsen & Kvalø, 2007; Standage, Duda, & Ntoumanis, 2003). In the same
studies performance-oriented climate has been found to be unrelated or negatively related with
less autonomous motivation but insignificantly associated with amotivation (Ommundsen &
Kvalø, 2007; Standage et al., 2003).
Downloaded by [UNSW Library] at 21:18 22 April 2016
Enjoyment
Based on the conceptualisation of Scanlan and Simons (1992), we operationalised enjoyment as a
positive affective response to a certain activity that corresponds to states experienced during PE
participation and described as “enjoy”, “happy”, “like”, and “fun”. In this sense, enjoyment
differs from the SDT postulated intrinsic motivation and it should be viewed as a broader and
more inclusive construct (Fairclough, 2003; Scanlan & Lewthwaite, 1986). Whereas intrinsic
motivation refers to internal locus of causality in participation (participation is due to totally
intrinsic reasons, such as stimulation, pleasure, excitement), enjoyment corresponds to experi-
enced affective outcome of the participation (enjoyment, happiness).
Previous studies have shown that students’ perceptions of learning-oriented climate have a
direct positive association with PE enjoyment (perceptions of performance-oriented climate have
a negative relation) (Barkoukis, Hagger, Labropoulos, & Tsorbatzoudis, 2010). In addition, stu-
dents’ enjoyment in PE has been found to be an important affective variable linked with increased
PA participation (Garcia Bengoechea, Sabiston, Ahmed, & Farnous, 2010; Papaioannou, Bebetsos,
Theodorakis, Christodoulidis, & Kouli, 2006; Wang, Chatzisarantis, Spray, & Biddle, 2002) and it
has been found to mediate the effectiveness of a school-based PA intervention (Dishman et al.,
2005). In fact, previous studies have suggested enjoyment in PE to be a missing link between motiv-
ation in school PE and PA participation (e.g. Wallhead & Buckworth, 2004). The basic premise of
this assumption is that if school PE increases students’ positive affect, such as enjoyment, these
affective outcomes will transfer into motivation to adopt a physically active lifestyle in other con-
texts (Wallhead & Buckworth, 2004). This assumption is based on theory and empirical findings
suggesting that the experience of enjoyment is a critical factor in determining one’s motivation
for and continued participation in exercise settings (Kremer, Trew, & Ogle, 1997).
This study
The general purpose of the study was to examine how students’ perception of teacher-initiated
motivational climate can influence students’ motivational regulations and enjoyment in PE
along with their PA participation. This study tested Vallerand’s (2007; see also Deci & Ryan,
2012) model in which motivational regulations are hypothesised to mediate the relationship
between students’ perception of social environment (learning-oriented climate should have a posi-
tive relationship; performance-oriented climate should have a negative relationship) and
4 T. Jaakkola et al.
enjoyment. Empirical evidence has partly supported this postulation, showing a positive relation-
ship between PE students’ perceptions of learning-oriented climate, enjoyment, and PA partici-
pation whereas the relationship between performance-oriented climate and the outcomes has
been mixed (Barkoukis & Hagger, 2013). A systematic review of SDT-related studies in exercise
and PA domains (Teixeira, Carraça, Markland, Silva, & Ryan, 2012) concluded that both intrinsic
and extrinsic motivation can lead to positive outcomes in PA participation, but only intrinsic
motivation leads to long-term PA participation and maintenance because it is more rewarding
and enjoyable. In addition, by applying a longitudinal cross-lagged approach we were able to
test whether learning-oriented climate and autonomous motivational regulations in comparison
to performance-oriented climate and less autonomous motivational regulations have a more
lasting effect on PA participation across middle school years. By applying a longitudinal
approach, we are able to take into account such processes as they unfold during middle school
years, as well as to estimate relations taking into account the temporal ordering of these processes,
an important prerequisite to causality.
Second, to enhance adolescents’ participation in daily 60 minutes of moderate-to-vigorous PA
Downloaded by [UNSW Library] at 21:18 22 April 2016
(U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2008), the examination of pathways through
which positive motivational and affective experiences in PE transfer into increased out-of-school
PA participation has gained continued interest (Chen, 2013). Although learning-oriented PE
climate, autonomous motivational regulations, and enjoyment have found to be related positively
to PA participation (Braithwaite et al., 2011), there is a lack of studies examining the pathways
through which teacher-initiated motivational climate in PE can impact students’ PA participation.
It is important to recognise that PE motivation and overall PA motivation are conceptually different
hence they occur in different contexts. In other words, a student may have high PE motivation but
low motivation to be physically active during leisure-time. Thus, this study was to examine the argu-
ment suggesting that PE enjoyment is a “missing link” to explain the transfer of PE motivation on PA
participation (Wallhead & Buckworth, 2004). This study views PE enjoyment as a possible mediator
of the effect of motivational climate and motivational regulations in PE on PA participation.
We tested the following research hypotheses.
H1a: We hypothesised that perceptions of learning-oriented climate relate to higher intrinsic motiv-
ation and identified regulation, lower amotivation, higher enjoyment, and higher PA participation
across both grade levels (Barkoukis & Hagger, 2013; Standage et al., 2003). In contrast, perceived
performance-oriented climate was assumed to relate to maladaptive pattern, such as lower levels of
intrinsic motivation and identified regulation, higher amotivation and lower enjoyment, and lower
levels of PA participation.
H1b: We hypothesised that learning-oriented climate at Grade 7 predicts not only learning-oriented
climate but also intrinsic motivation and identified regulation at Grade 9. In contrast, we hypothesised
that performance-oriented climate at Grade 7 predicts not only performance-oriented climate but also
introjected and extrinsic regulation and amotivation at Grade 9 (Barkoukis & Hagger, 2013).
H2: We hypothesised that enjoyment is a significant mediator between motivational regulations and
PA participation. Specifically, we hypothesised that enjoyment mediates between motivation in PE
and PA participation, intrinsic motivation and identified regulation relating to higher PE enjoyment
and PA participation (Wallhead & Buckworth, 2004).
Method
Participants
A total of 540 adolescents (277 boys, 263 girls; 95% Caucasian) enrolled in five urban middle
schools were examined during Grade 7 (7th grade fall) and Grade 9 (9th grade fall). At the begin-
ning of the study, the median age of the students was 13 (age range 12–14). The study comprised
International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 5
nine Caucasian PE teachers (Mage 45.3 [7.18]), with an average of 18 years of teaching experi-
ence. There were no remarkable differences in PE facilities and equipment between study
schools. The research protocol was approved by the ethical committee of the local university.
Children and teachers were recruited through a direct contact with schools in consultation with
the school principal. All the middle school children were invited to participate and parental con-
sents and child assents were obtained. Participation was completely voluntary and participants
had the right to withdraw from the study at any time. All the measurements were carried out
by a trained team of researchers and took place during PE lessons.
Research context
This study was conducted in Finland, the northernmost country of the European Union. PE is
mandated as a part of the Finland’s comprehensive school curriculum (detailed description of
the Finland’s PE curriculum can be found in Yli-Piipari, 2014). The Finnish National Board of
Education designs the core curriculum goals and content for PE at all school levels. Health is a
Downloaded by [UNSW Library] at 21:18 22 April 2016
critical aspect of the Finnish PE curriculum supported by continuing attention to the skills and
knowledge associated with lifelong engagement in PA. Additionally, Finnish PE aims are more
related to cooperation, socialisation, and team effort than physiology, competitions, and perform-
ance outcomes. In middle school (grades 7–9), students are taught by a specialist (five-year master
degree in PE pedagogy) PE teacher and they have seven obligatory PE units (a unit is 38 hours),
that is two or three 45-minute PE lessons weekly. In addition, students have a possibility to select
a maximum of three PE units from a set of elective units that are developed according to the local
school curriculum. In Finland, PE is typically organised around multi-activity programmes in a
series of units (Heikinaro-Johansson & Telama, 2005) and it has been found to be similar to
that in other Western countries (Annerstedt, 2008) and in the USA (Yli-Piipari, 2014).
Measures
A pilot study (N = 230) to test reliability and validity of the scales and to reduce the number of
factor indicators was conducted prior to this study. A detailed description of the pilot study can be
provided by the first author on request. To avoid survey fatigue, the number of factor indicators
of the original scales was reduced (task climate 6 → 4; ego climate 6 → 4; intrinsic motivation
12 → 9; and identified, introjected, external regulation, and amotivation 4 → 3).
Motivational climate
Perceptions of motivational climate in PE were measured using the Finnish version of Motivation
Climate in Physical Education Questionnaire (MCPEQ; Soini, Liukkonen, Watt, Yli-Piipari, &
Jaakkola, 2014), which consists of two subscales representing learning- and performance-oriented
climates. The individual item stem used in the measure was “In my PE class … ”. Both the learn-
ing-oriented dimension (e.g. “It is important for students to try their best in PE lessons”) and the
performance-oriented dimension (e.g. “It is important for students to succeed better than others in
PE lessons”) consisted of four items. Responses were indicated on a five-point Likert scale
ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (5). Factorial validity and internal consist-
ency of the MCPEQ have been found to be satisfactory (Soini, Liukkonen, Watt, et al., 2014).
Motivational regulations
Motivational regulations in PE were assessed with the Finnish version of the Physical Education
Motivation Scale (PEMS; Jaakkola, 2002), which is a modified version of the Sport Motivation
6 T. Jaakkola et al.
Scale (Pelletier, Fortier, Vallerand, Tuson, & Briere, 1995). The Physical Education Motivation
Scale consists of five subscales comprising nine items of intrinsic motivation and three items
of identified, introjected, and external regulation, and amotivation. Each item was rated on a
five-point Likert scale (1 = does not correspond at all … 5 = corresponds exactly). The scale
used in this study had the individual item stem, “I’m currently participating in physical education,
because … ”. Construct validity and internal consistency of the PEMS have been found to be sat-
isfactory (Jaakkola, 2002).
Enjoyment
The Finnish version of the Sport Enjoyment Scale (Scanlan, Carpenter, Schmidt, Simons, &
Keeler, 1993) was used to assess enjoyment in PE lessons (Soini, Liukkonen, Jaakkola, Leskinen,
& Rantanen, 2007). The scale measures one dimension of enjoyment with four items. The items
of the original version were modified to measure enjoyment during PE lessons (example item, “I
have fun during PE lessons”). Participants responded on a five-point Likert scale with a range
Downloaded by [UNSW Library] at 21:18 22 April 2016
from 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much). Construct validity and internal consistency of the Sport
Enjoyment Scale have been found to be satisfactory (Soini, Liukkonen, Jaakkola, et al., 2007).
Physical activity
To assess adolescents’ self-report PA participation, the Health Behavior in School-aged Children
Research Protocol was used (Currie, Samdal, Boyce, & Smith, 2002). The scale was to measure
how many days in the week students are meeting a 60-minute moderate-to-vigorous PA rec-
ommendation (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2008). The scale consisted of
two items and was rated on an eight-point response scale (0–7 days of the week). The introduction
for the scale was:
In the next two questions PA means all activities which raise your heart rate or momentarily gets you
out of breath, for ex-ample, in doing exercise, playing with your friends, going to school, or in school
PE. PA also includes for example jogging, intensive walking, roller skating, cycling, dancing, skating,
skiing, soccer, basketball and baseball.
The items were: (a) “Think about your typical week. How many days did you exercise for at
least 60 min. during which you got out of breath” and (b) “Think about your last 7 days. How
many days did you exercise for at least 60 min during which you get out of breath?” A sum
scale of PA participation was formulated by adding the response scores for the two items to
assess students’ self-report participation in moderate-to-vigorous PA. Prochaska, Sallis, and
Long (2001) reported adequate factorial validity and reliability of these two PA participation
within adolescence population.
Statistical analyses
Preliminary analyses were initiated by calculating descriptive statistics, such as means, standard
deviations, and Pearson’s correlation coefficients of the research variables. Missing data com-
prised 1.7% of all cases and were handled under the assumption that the data were missing at
random (Collins, Schafer, & Kam, 2001). To test hypothesis H1a, a structural equation model
(SEM) was conducted separately for Grades 7 (T0) and 9 (T1). A SEM encompasses two com-
ponents: a measurement model (essentially the confirmatory factor analysis; CFA) and a structural
model. A measurement model is a multivariate regression model that describes the relationship
between a set of observed (factor indicators) and latent (factors) variables. A structural model
International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 7
allows the simultaneous estimation of several dependent latent constructs. To test hypothesis H1b,
Time 1 variables were regressed by estimating regression coefficients from the subsequent Time 0
variables (i.e. PAT0 to PAT1). In addition, cross-lagged paths from the most proximal and theory-
supported determinants (T0) to outcome variables (T1) were estimated [paths from learning- and
performance-oriented climate (T0) to intrinsic, identified, introjected, and extrinsic regulations
and amotivation (T1); from intrinsic, identified, introjected, and extrinsic regulations along
with amotivation (T0) to enjoyment (T1); from enjoyment (T0) to PA participation (T1)]. To
test the second hypothesis (H2), the role of enjoyment as a possible mediator was tested by
using the MODEL INDIRECT command.
All analyses were performed using the Mplus statistical package (Version 6.1; Muthén &
Muthén, 1998–2013). A COMPLEX option was used to correct a possible nonindependence of
the observations based on students being nested within their PE classes (Asparouhov, 2005). A
SEM fits the data well when the p value associated with the chi-square test is non-significant.
Additionally, if the values of the Bentler comparative fit index (CFI) and Tucker-Lewis index
(TLI) are above .95 and the values of the Root Mean Squared Error of Approximation
Downloaded by [UNSW Library] at 21:18 22 April 2016
(RMSEA) are below .06, a good fit between the hypothesised model and the observed data
exists (Hu & Bentler, 1999). To determine the statistical significance of possible mediation or
indirect effects, bootstrapped asymmetric confidence intervals were calculated based on 5000
bootstrapped samples (i.e. Hayes, 2009). A mediation or indirect effect is supported if the confi-
dence interval (CI) does not contain 0, which suggests that the independent variable significantly
influences the mediator, which in turn influences the dependent variable.
Results
Preliminary analyses
Table 1 presents Pearson’s correlation coefficients, means, standard deviations, Cronbach’s
alphas, and the values of the Pearson’s first coefficient of skewness for all the variables
studied. Cronbach’s alpha values showed acceptable internal consistency of the scales (α > .70)
and Pearson’s first coefficient of skewness indicated that the distribution of the data was in accep-
table limits. Perceptions of learning-oriented climate had a moderate-to-strong correlation with
autonomous motivational regulations and enjoyment, a weak-to-moderate correlation with less
autonomous motivational regulations and participation in PA, and a negative correlation with
amotivation. In addition, perceptions of performance-oriented climate had a positive and weak-
to-moderate correlation with autonomous motivational regulations and a moderate correlation
with less autonomous motivational regulations and amotivation, and no relation with enjoyment
and participation in PA. Enjoyment in PE had a positive and moderate relationship with partici-
pation in PA.
Variable list 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
T. Jaakkola et al.
1 Learning- –
oriented (T0)
2 Learning- .39** –
oriented (T1)
3 Performance- .19* .08 –
Downloaded by [UNSW Library] at 21:18 22 April 2016
oriented (T0)
4 Performance- .06 .02 .32** –
oriented (T1)
5 Intrinsic (T0) .85** .33** .19* .06 –
6 Intrinsic (T1) .46** .72** .15 .19* .46** –
7 Identified (T0) .85** .33** .23** .07 .73** .40** –
8 Identified (T1) .44** .58** .18* .29** .38** .50** .46** –
9 Introjected (T0) .66** .26** .30** .10 .56** .31** .57** .31** –
10 Introjected (T1) .41** .67** .21** .32** .36** .56** .36** .51** .43** –
11 Extrinsic (T0) .55** .22** .45** .15* .48** .27** .49** .30** .43** .28** –
12 Extrinsic (T1) .26** .22** .30** .52** .23** .28** .23** .30** .21** .33** .41** –
13 Amotivation −.33** −.13* .23** .08 −.27** −.13* −.26** −.12* −.16* −.10 −.08 −.01 –
(T0)
14 Amotivation −.16** −.31** .11* .27** −.13* −.18* −.13* −.12* −.08 −.13* −.04 .07 .25** –
(T1)
15 Enjoyment (T0) .68** .27** .09 .03 .66** .33** .69** .33** .42** .27** .39** .17* −.46** −.16* –
16 Enjoyment (T1) .51** .64** .11 .10 .47** .68** .46** .52** .37** .61** .28** .20** −.25** −.43** .48** –
17 Physical .26** .10 .04 .01 .26** .13* .27** .13* .16* .11 .15* .07 −.18* −.06 .39** .18* –
activity (T0)
18 Physical .24** .19* .04 .03 .22** .22** .23** .18* .16* .19** .13* .08 −.14* −.13* .28** .32** .47** –
activity (T1)
M 3.80b 3.52 2.99 3.09 3.23 3.24 3.08 3.11 3.44b 3.37 2.67 2.87b 2.18 2.40b 3.75b 3.53 3.96b 3.64
SD .78 .80 .89 .82 .87 .79 .88 .84 .88 .79 .89 .84 .95 .97 .93 1.05 1.99 1.76
Α .80 .85 .86 .86 .92 .93 .73 .80 .69 .70 .71 .78 .77 .83 .93 .94 .90a .92a
Skewness −.49 .18 .08 −.03 −.32 −.25 −.08 −.15 −.16 .01 .08 −.20 .11 −.48 −.65 −.28 −.28 .08
a
Instead of Cronbach’s alpha values, Pearson’s correlation coefficient between two items are presented.
b
t test value higher in statistically significant level of p < .001 (between tests at T0 and T1).
*p < .05.
**p < .001.
International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 9
and amotivation (T0: βam = .28; T1: βam = .28). In addition, amotivation was negatively associated
with enjoyment (T0: βenj = −.23; T1: β = −.32), enjoyment related positively to PA participation
(T0: βpa = .38; T1: β = .31), and intrinsic motivation had positive but indirect relationship with PA
participation via enjoyment (β = .18). The sizes of the effects were weak to strong explaining the
10–70% of the variances of the dependent variables: R2: intrinsic motivation = .45(.05)/.47(.06);
identified regulation = .33(.06)/32(.05); introjected regulation = .26(.05)/.41(.05); external regu-
lation = .26(.05)/.27(.05); amotivation = .18(.04)/.15(.04); enjoyment = .64(.05)/.70(.05); PA
= .15(.03)/10(.03) (presented as T0/T1) [the standard errors are presented in the parentheses].
Figure 1. The proposed theoretical framework hypothesised to underlie human motivation and physical
activity. Note: Factor indicators have been omitted from the figure for presentation purposes. Solid
arrows represent the positive relationships, whereas dashed arrows represent the negative.
10 T. Jaakkola et al.
Table 2. Factor indicator loadings and errors along with model fit indices for both estimated models.
Regression model T(0) Regression model T(1)
Estimates
of Unstandardised Unstandardised
parameters Standardised values values Standardised values Values
Learning-oriented climate
Item 1 .63(.05) 1 .76(.03) 1
Item 2 .70(.05) 1.03(.06) .82(.03) 1.02(.05)
Item 3 .78(.03) 1.19(.08) .84(.02) 1.02(.06)
Item 4 .60(.05) .87(.06) .73(.03) .81(.05)
Performance-oriented climate
Item 5 .73(.03) 1 .73(.03) 1
Item 6 .71(.04) .94(.08) .75(.03) .98(.07)
Item 7 .85(.02) 1.12(.07) .84(.03) 1.08(.05)
Item 8 .81(.03) 1.07(.08) .81(.03) 1.05(.06)
Intrinsic
Downloaded by [UNSW Library] at 21:18 22 April 2016
(Continued)
International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 11
Cross-lagged relationships
Performance-oriented climate T0 → introjected .18(.08)** .16(.07)**
regulation T1
Performance-oriented climate T0 → extrinsic .24(.07)** .23(.07)**
regulation T1
Performance-oriented climate T0 → amotivation T1 .17(.07)** .19(.07)**
**p < .001.
climate at T0 to enjoyment at T1 via intrinsic motivation at T1 (95% asymmetric CI ranged from .02
to .15) and learning-oriented climate at T0 to enjoyment at T1 via enjoyment at T0 (95% asym-
metric CI ranged from .09 to .25) emerged.
Discussion
The aim of the study was to analyse how teacher-initiated motivational climate in PE influenced
students’ motivational regulations and affective responses in PE along with their PA participation,
and whether these relationships were sustainable across middle school levels 7 to 9. In addition,
we aimed to examine the role of positive affect in explaining the relationship between motivation
in PE and PA participation. This study extended our understanding on the effects of learning- and
performance-oriented climates on PE enjoyment and PA participation. In addition, the role of
positive affect, such as enjoyment in PE, as an indirect mediator between motivational experi-
ences in PE and overall PA participation was also studied.
Downloaded by [UNSW Library] at 21:18 22 April 2016
able and fun by the students. However, a consistent focus of the motivational climate on other-
referenced criteria may have detrimental effect on students’ motivation. Having in mind that
promotion of a learning-oriented climate does not necessarily results in decreasing perform-
ance-oriented climate (Barkoukis, Tsorbatzoudis, & Grouios, 2008), specific practices should
be employed. For instance, although competitive drills and games can be used, PE teachers
should avoid placing emphasis on winning and demonstrating competence, arrange opportu-
nities for all children to win, feel competent and successful, and follow students’ learning pace.
that PA participation is also influenced by numerous other factors (Sallis, Prochaska, & Taylor,
2000) alongside enjoyment in school PE.
Practical implications
These findings have important implications for physical educators. Most notably, our results
encourage PE instructors to imply learning-oriented teaching practices that emphasise self-
improvement and task mastery rather than performance-oriented teaching practices that focus
on competition and normative comparisons. In addition, this study supports the previous findings
(Dishman et al., 2005) and suggestions (Wallhead & Buckworth, 2004), indicating that enjoyment
in PE may be the psychological mediator that carries positive PE experiences over to increased
out-of-school PA participation. Given these findings, we believe that PE activities should (a)
match student needs, skills and preferences, (b) be perceived as exciting and challenging by
the students, and (c) offer a wide range of involvement choices. It has been argued that failure
Downloaded by [UNSW Library] at 21:18 22 April 2016
to consider these characteristics could potentially lead to a lack of enjoyment for PE (Ntoumanis,
Pensgaard, Martin, & Pipe, 2004).
Notes
1. According to SDT, extrinsic motivation includes also a fourth regulation, namely integrated regulation,
which reflects the integration of behaviour within the self. Integrated regulation is considered to be the
most autonomous type of extrinsic motivation as people “reciprocally assimilate a new identification
with their sense of who they are” (Deci & Ryan, 2008, p. 16). In the SDT tradition the measurement
of integrated regulation has been incorporated in the measures of identified regulation (Deci & Ryan,
2008).
2. Nicholls (1989) labelled the two goal orientations task and ego orientation. Recently, also the learning
and performance orientation terms have been used to describe the motivational climate dimensions.
16 T. Jaakkola et al.
References
Ames, C. (1992). The relationship of achievement goals to student motivation in classroom settings. In G.
Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in sport and exercise (pp. 161–176). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Annerstedt, C. (2008). Physical education in Scandinavia with a focus on Sweden: A comparative perspec-
tive. Physical Education & Sport Pedagogy, 13, 303–318. doi:10.1080/17408980802353354
Asparouhov, T. (2005). Sampling weights in latent variable modeling. Structural Equation Modeling, 12,
411–434. doi:10.1207/s15328007sem1203_4
Barkoukis, V., & Hagger, M.S. (2013). The trans-contextual model: Perceived learning and performance
motivational climates as analogues of perceived autonomy support. European Journal of Psychology
of Education, 28, 353–372. doi:10.1007/s10212-012-0118-5
Barkoukis, V., Hagger, M.S., Labropoulos, G., & Tsorbatzoudis, H. (2010). Extending the trans-contextual
model in physical education and leisure-time contexts: Examining the role of basic psychological need
satisfaction. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 647–670. doi:10.1348/000709910X487023
Barkoukis, V., Ntoumanis, N., & Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C. (2010). Developmental changes in achievement
motivation and affect in physical education: Growth trajectories and demographic differences.
Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 11, 83–90. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2009.04.008
Barkoukis, V., Tsorbatzoudis, H., & Grouios, G. (2008). Manipulation of motivational climate in physical
education: Effects of a seven-month intervention. European Physical Education Review, 14, 367–387.
Downloaded by [UNSW Library] at 21:18 22 April 2016
Booth, M.L., Okely, A.D., Chey, T., & Bauman, A. (2001). The reliability and validity of the physical
activity questions in the WHO health behaviour in schoolchildren (HBSC) survey: A population
study. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 35, 263–267. doi:10.1136/bjsm.35.4.263
Braithwaite, R., Spray, C.M., & Warburton, V.E. (2011). Motivational climate interventions in physical edu-
cation: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 12, 628–638. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.
2011.06.005
Carroll, B., & Loumidis, J. (2001). Children’s perceived competence and enjoyment in physical education
and physical activity outside school. European Physical Education Review, 7, 24–43.
Chen, A. (2013). Top 10 research questions related to children physical activity motivation. Research
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 84, 441–447. doi:10.1080/02701367.2013.844030
Collins, L.M., Schafer, J.L., & Kam, C-M.(2001). A comparison of inclusive and restrictive strategies in
modern missing data procedures. Psychological Methods, 6, 330–351. doi:10.1037//1082-989X.6.4.330
Currie, C., Samdal, O., Boyce, W., & Smith, B. (2002). Health behavior in school-aged children: A WHO
cross-national study. Research protocol for the 2001–2002 survey. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh.
Deci, E., & Ryan, R. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York, NY:
Plenum Press.
Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-deter-
mination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268.
Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (2008). Facilitating optimal motivation and psychological well-being across life’s
domains. Canadian Psychology, 49, 14–23. doi:10.1037/0708-5591.49.1.14
Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (2012). Self-determination theory. In P.A.M. Van Lange, A.W. Kruglanski, & E.T.
Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 416–437). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Deci, E.L., Ryan, R.M., & Guay, F. (2013). Self-determination theory and actualization of human potential.
In D. McInerney, H. Marsh, R. Craven, & F. Guay (Eds.), Theory driving research: New wave perspec-
tives on self processes and human development (pp. 109–133). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Press.
Dishman, R.K., Motl, R.W., Saunders, R., Felton, G., Ward, D.S., Dowda, M., … Pate, R. (2005). Enjoyment
mediates effects of a school-based physical-activity intervention. Medicine and Science in Sports and
Exercise, 37, 478–487. doi:10.1249/01.MSS.0000155391.62733.A7
Duda, J.L., & Balaguer, I. (2007). Coach-created motivational climate. In S. Jowett & D. Lavallee (Eds.),
Social psychology in sport (pp. 117–130). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Fairclough, S. (2003). Physical activity, perceived competence and enjoyment during secondary school
physical education. European Journal of Physical Education, 8, 5–18.
Garcia Bengoechea, E., Sabiston, C.M., Ahmed, R., & Farnous, M. (2010). Exploring links to unorganized
and organized physical activity during adolescence: The role of gender, socioeconomic status, weight
status, and enjoyment of physical education. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 81, 7–16.
Hagger, M.S., & Chatzisarantis, N.L. (2009). Integrating the theory of planned behaviour and self-determi-
nation theory in health behaviour: A meta-analysis. British Journal of Health Psychology, 14, 275–302.
Hagger, M.S., Chatzisarantis, N.L., Culverhouse, T., & Biddle, S.J.H. (2003). The process by which per-
ceived autonomy support in physical education promotes leisure-time physical activity intentions and
International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 17
and students’ emotional experiences and effort in school physical education. Journal of Educational
Research, 103, 295–308. doi:10.1080/00220670903383044
Mellers, B., Schwartz, A., & Ritov, I. (1999). Emotion-based choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
128, 332–345.
Muthén, L.K., & Muthén, B.O. (1998–2013). Mplus user’s guide (6th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Author.
Nicholls, J. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic education. London: Harvard University Press.
Ntoumanis, N., Pensgaard, A. M., Martin, C., & Pipe, K. (2004). Anidiographic analysis of amotivation in
compulsory school physical education. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 26, 197–214.
Ommundsen, Y., & Kvalø, S. (2007). Autonomy-mastery, supportive or performance focused? Different
teacher behaviours and pupils’ outcomes in physical education. Scandinavian Journal of Educational
Research, 51, 385–413. doi:10.1080/00313830701485551
Papaioannou, A., Bebetsos, E., Theodorakis, Y., Christodoulidis, T., & Kouli, O. (2006). Causal relation-
ships of sport and exercise involvement with goal orientations, perceived competence and intrinsic
motivation in physical education: A longitudinal study. Journal of Sports Sciences, 24, 367–382.
doi:10.1080/02640410400022060
Pelletier, L., Fortier, M., Vallerand, R., Tuson, K., & Briere, N. (1995). Toward a new measure of intrinsic
motivation, extrinsic motivation and amotivation in sports: The Sport Motivation Scale (SMS). Journal
of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 17, 35–53.
Prochaska, J.J., Sallis, J.F., & Long, B. (2001). A physical activity screening measure for the use with ado-
lescents in primary care. Archives in Pediatric Adolescence Medicine, 155, 554–559.
Roberts, G.C. (2012). Motivation in sport and exercise from an achievement goal theory perspective: After
30 years, where are we? In G.C. Roberts & D. Treasure (Eds.), Advances in motivation in sport and exer-
cise (pp. 5–58). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Sallis, J.F., McKenzie, T.L., Beets, M.W., Beighle, A., Erwin, H., & Lee, S. (2012). Physical education’s role
in public health: Steps forward and backward over 20 years and HOPE for the future. Research
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 83, 125–135.
Sallis, J.F., Prochaska, J.J., & Taylor, W.C. (2000). A review of correlates of physical activity of children and
adolescents. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 32, 963–975.
Sallis, J.F., & Saelens, B.E. (2000). Assessment of physical activity by self-report: Status, limitations, and
future directions. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71, 1–14.
Scanlan, T.K., Carpenter, P.J., Schmidt, G.W., Simons, J.P., & Keeler, B. (1993). The sport commitment
model: Development for the youth-sport domain. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 15,
16–38.
Scanlan, T.K., & Lewthwaite, R. (1986). Social psychological aspects of competition for male youth sport
participants: Predictors of enjoyment: IV. Journal of Sport Psychology, 8, 25–35.
Scanlan, T.K., & Simons, J.P. (1992). The construct of sport enjoyment. In G. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in
sport and exercise (pp. 199–215). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Soini, M., Liukkonen, J., Jaakkola, T., Leskinen, E., & Rantanen, P. (2007). Motivaatioilmastojaviihtyminen-
koululiikunnassa. [Motivational climate and enjoyment of physical education in school]. Liikunta & Tiede,
44, 45–51.
18 T. Jaakkola et al.
Soini, M., Liukkonen, J., Watt, A., Yli-Piipari, S., & Jaakkola, T. (2014). Factorial validity and internal con-
sistency of the motivational climate in physical education scale. Journal of Sports Science and Medicine,
3, 137–144.
Standage, M., Duda, J.L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2003). A model of contextual motivation in physical education:
Using constructs from self-determination and achievement goal theories to predict physical activity
intentions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 97–110. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.97
Teixeira, P.J., Carraça, E.V., Markland, D., Silva, M.N., & Ryan, R.M. (2012). Exercise, physical activity,
and self-determination theory: A systematic review. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition
and Physical Activity, 9, 78.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2008). Physical activity guidelines for Americans.
Retrieved from the U.S. Department of Health website: http://www.health.gov/paguidelines/pdf/
paguide.pdf
Vallerand, R.J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. In M.P. Zanna
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 271–360). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Vallerand, R.J. (2007). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in sport and physical activity. A review and a look at
the future. In G.C. Tenenbaum & R.C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (pp. 59–83).
New York, NY: Wiley.
Vuori, M., Ojala, K., Tynjälä, J., Villberg, J., Välimaa, R., & Kannas, L. (2005). Liikunta-aktiivisuuttakos-
Downloaded by [UNSW Library] at 21:18 22 April 2016