Odden - 2013 - Chapter 5 - Interacting Processes
Odden - 2013 - Chapter 5 - Interacting Processes
5 Interacting
processes
PREVIEW
In this chapter, you will broaden your understanding of how
KEY TERMS phonological systems work by
interaction u looking at more complex patterns of phonological
ordering alternation
u seeing how complex surface patterns of alternations
result from the interaction of different but related
phonological rules
u understanding the effect of different rule orderings
on how an underlying form is changed into a
surface form
116 INTRODUCTION PHONOLOGY
The first group of examples (1a) shows that the nominative has no suffix,
and the partitive has the suffix [-a] or [-æ] (the choice depends on the
preceding vowels, determined by a vowel harmony rule according to
which a suffix vowel is front if the preceding vowel is front – the
rule skips over the vowel [i], but if there are no vowels other than [i]
preceding, the harmony rule turns the suffix vowel into a front vowel).
The second group of examples (1b) illustrates roots which have /i/ as the
underlying final vowel of the root. The nouns in the third group (1c)
illustrate a phenomenon of final vowel raising and fronting (which
we have previously seen in closely related Finnish), whereby e and ǝ
become [i] word-finally.
The essential difference between the examples of (1b) and (1c) is that the
forms in (1b) underlyingly end in the vowel /i/, and those in (1c) end in /e/
or /ǝ/. In the last set of examples (1d), the noun root underlyingly ends in
the sequence /kǝ/, which can be seen directly in kurkǝ-a. However, the final
CV of the root appears as [t ʃi] in the nominative kurt ʃi.
It would be unrevealing to posit a rule changing word-final /kǝ#/ into
[tʃi#] in one step. A problem with such a rule is that the change of a velar to
a palatal conditioned by following word-final schwa is not a process found
in other languages, and depends on a very specific conjunction of facts,
that is, not just schwa, but word-final schwa. You may not know at this
point that such a rule is not found in other languages – part of learning
about phonology is learning what processes do exist in languages, some-
thing you will have a better basis for judging by the end of this book.
What you can see right now is that such a rule treats it as a coincidence
that the underlying final schwa actually becomes [i] on the surface by an
independently necessary rule, so that much of the supposed rule applying
to /kǝ#/ is not actually specific to /kǝ#/.
This alternation makes more sense once it is decomposed into the two
constituent rules which govern it, namely final raising (independently
motivated by the data in (c)). Applying this rule alone to final /kǝ/ would
result in the sequence [ki]. However, [ki] is not an allowed CV sequence in
this language, and a process of palatalization takes place, in accordance
with the following rule:
(3) Palatalization
⎡+cons⎤ ⎡+syl ⎤
⎢ ⎥ → [ +cor ] / __ ⎢ ⎥
⎣+back⎦ ⎣-back⎦
We can thus account for the change of underlying /kurkǝ/ and /ǝɫkǝ/ to
[kurt ʃi] and [ǝɫt ʃi] by applying these two rules in a specific order, where the
118 INTRODUCTION PHONOLOGY
You should take note of two points regarding how the palatalization rule
is formalized. First, by strictly making a velar consonant become [+cor],
the result would be a velarized retroflex stop [ʈγ]: such sounds simply do
not exist in the language, in fact the [-ant] coronal sounds of the language
are all alveopalatal, and the alveopalatal stops in Votic are all affricates.
Observed [t ʃ] is the closest segment of the language to [ʈγ].
Second, we do not have direct evidence that all front vowels trigger the
change of velars, in fact we only have direct evidence that word-final [i]
triggers the change. At the same time, we do not have any direct evidence
that it matters whether the triggering vowel is word-final or not, nor do
we have any evidence that the other front vowels [y ø e æ] fail to trigger
the change. Because there is no evidence for adding restrictions to the
rule, we follow the general scientific principle of stating the rule as simply
as possible, consistent with the data.
(5) to V to cause to V
a. koka̋ ma̋ koka̋ mja̋ ‘milk’
kokonà kokonjà ‘hit’
kolaa̋ àmbà kola̋ àmbjà ‘lap’
kota̋ la̋ kota̋ lja̋ ‘count’
kwaambatà kwaambatjà ‘go up’
kwaàðà kwaàðjà ‘govern’
kwe̋ e̋ ta̋ kwe̋ e̋ tja̋ ‘answer’
kwı̋ ı̋ mba̋ kwı̋ ı̋ mbja̋ ‘swell’
b. koβikà koβitʃà ‘arrive’
koβálokà koβálotʃà ‘fall’
kolikà kolitʃà ‘enter’
kolε̋ ὲŋgà kolε̋ ὲɲdʒà ‘aim’
Interacting processes 119
The examples in (a) illustrate the causative affix following various non-
velar consonants of the language. In (b), we see the causative of various
roots which end in k or g, where by analogy to the data in (a) we predict
the causatives /koβikjà/, /koβálokjà/, /kolε̋ ὲŋgjà/, and so on. Instead of
the expected consonant sequences kj, gj, we find instead that the velar
consonant has been replaced by an alveopalatal affricate, due to the
following rule:
Sometimes rules
(6) Palatalization affect two segments
⎡ +syl ⎤ simultaneously: this
⎡+cons⎤ ⎢ ⎥ is an example of
⎢+back⎥ ⎢ -cons ⎥ → [ +cor ] Ø
⎣ ⎦ ⎢ -back⎥ how such rules are
⎣ ⎦ formalized, stating
that the first
Examples of glide formation are also seen in (5), where the vowel /o/ in the segment becomes
infinitive prefix becomes [w] before another vowel. This process of glide [+cor] and the
formation is further illustrated in (7) and (8). In (7), you can see across all second is deleted.
of the columns that the prefix for the infinitive is /ko/, and appears
phonetically as such when it stands before another consonant. The last
three data columns show that the prefixes marking different classes of
objects are /mó/ for class 3, /mé/ for class 4, and /ké/ for class 7 (Kamba
nouns have a dozen grammatical agreement classes, analogous to gender
in some European languages).
When the verb root begins with a vowel, we would predict a sequence of
vowels such as *koasja for ‘to lose,’ in lieu of a rule modifying vowel The stem-initial
sequences. Vowel sequences are avoided in Kamba by the application of vowel in these
the rule of glide formation, according to which any nonlow vowel examples becomes
becomes a glide before another vowel. long, as a side effect
of the preceding
(8) to V to V it (cl 3) to V them (cl 4) to V it (cl 7) vowel becoming a
kwa̋ a̋ sja̋ komwa̋ a̋ sja̋ komja̋ a̋ sja̋ kotʃa̋ a̋ sja̋ ‘lose’ glide: this is known
kwa̋ a̋ ka̋ komwa̋ a̋ ka̋ komja̋ a̋ ka̋ kotʃa̋ a̋ ka̋ ‘build’ as compensatory
kwaàsà komwa̋ àsà komja̋ àsà kotʃa̋ àsà ‘carve’ lengthening
kő ő mba̋ komő ő mba̋ komjő ő mba̋ kotʃő ő mba̋ ‘mold’
kookeljà komóokeljà komjóokeljà kotʃóokeljà ‘lift’
kűűna̋ koműűna̋ komjűűna̋ kotʃűűna̋ ‘fetch’
120 INTRODUCTION PHONOLOGY
While this rule does not explicitly state that the resulting glide is [+high],
that value is predictable via structure preservation, given the fact that the
language does not have glides that are [-high].
This rule would be expected to apply to underlying forms such as
/kouna/ ‘to fetch’ and /ko-omba/ ‘to mold,’ since those forms have an
underlying sequence of a vowel /o/ followed by another vowel. Applying
that rule would result in *[kwűűna̋ ] and *[kwő ő mba̋ ], but these are not the
correct forms. We can resolve this problem once we observe that the glide
[w] never appears before the tense round vowels [u, o] (but it can appear
before the vowel [ɔ], as seen in [kwɔ̋ ɔ̋ na̋ ] ‘to see’ from /ko-ɔna/).
It does not help to restrict rule (9) so that it does not apply before /o, u/,
since the vowel /e/ does actually undergo glide formation before these
vowels (/ko-me-okelya/ becomes [komjóokeljà] ‘to lift them’ and /ko-méűna̋ /
becomes [komjűűna̋ ] ‘to fetch them’). What seems to be a restriction on
glide formation is highly specific: the tense round vowel fails to surface as
a glide only if the following vowel is o or u. Furthermore, the round vowel
does not merely fail to become a glide, it actually deletes, therefore we
can’t just rewrite (9) so that it doesn’t apply before [u, o], since that would
give *[koűna̋ ] and *[koő mba̋ ]). Two rules are required to account for these
vowel-plus-vowel combinations. A very simple solution to this problem is
to allow the most general form of the Glide Formation rule to apply,
imposing no restrictions on which vowels trigger the rule, and derive
the intermediate forms kwűűna̋ and kwő ő mba̋ . Since we have observed that
the surface sequences [wo] and [wu] are lacking in the language, we may
posit the following rule of glide deletion, which explains both why such
sequences are lacking and what happened to the expected glide in the
intermediate forms.
Glide Formation first creates a glide, and some of the glides so created are
then deleted by (10).
Interacting processes 121
You will also note that a nasal consonant always agrees in place of
articulation with the following consonant, due to the following rule.
The data considered so far have not given clear evidence as to what the
underlying place of articulation of the first-singular subject prefix is, since
that nasal always assimilates to the following consonant. To determine that
the prefix is indeed /n/, we turn to the form of stems which underlyingly
begin with a vowel, where there is no assimilation. In the imperative,
where no prefix precedes the stem, the glide [ j] is inserted before the initial
vowel. (The data in (17) include examples of underlying initial /j/, which is
generally retained, showing that there cannot be a rule of j-deletion.) When
the third-plural prefix /βa/ precedes the stem, the resulting vowel sequence
is simplified to a single nonhigh vowel. No rules apply to the first-singular
prefix, which we can see surfaces as [n] before all vowels.
The reason why ordering does not matter is that the voicing rule does
not refer to the place of articulation of the nasal, and the assimilation
rule does not refer to the voicing of the following consonant. Thus infor-
mation provided by one rule cannot change whether the other rule applies.
⎡+voice⎤
C→ ⎢ ⎥ / [+nas] __
⎣-cont ⎦
The examples in (a) show the effect of rules of voicing and consonant
hardening, applying as expected to /t/ and /r/. However, the examples in
(b) show the deletion of underlying /l/ after a nasal. These examples contrast
with the first set of examples in (17), where the root also begins with
underlying /l/: the difference between the two sets of verbs is that in the
second set, where /l/ deletes, the following consonant is a nasal, whereas in
the first set where /l/ does not delete, the next consonant is not a nasal.
The significance of the examples in (20a) is that although underlying /t/,
/l/, and /r/ all become [d] after a nasal, the deletion of an underlying
consonant in the environment N_VN only affects underlying /l/. Since
the voicing-hardening rule (19) neutralizes the distinction between
the three consonants after a nasal but in fact /l/ acts differently from /t/
and /r/ in the context N_VN, we can conclude that there is a prior rule
deleting /l/ – but not /t/ or /r/ – in this context.
(21) l-deletion
[+lat] ! Ø / [+nasal] _ V0 [+nasal]
This rule clearly must apply before the hardening rule changes /l/ into [d]
after a nasal, since otherwise there would be no way to restrict this rule to
applying only to underlying /l/. When (19) applies, underlying /n-liinda/
would become n-diinda, but /n-riina/ would also become n-diina. Once that
has happened, there would be no way to predict the actual pronunciations
[niinda] versus [ndiina].
On the other hand, if you were to apply the l-deletion rule first, the rule
could apply in the case of /n-liinda/ to give [niinda], but would not apply to
/n-riina/ because that form does not have an l: thus by ordering the rules so
that l-deletion comes first, the distinction between /l/, which deletes, and
/r/, which does not delete, is preserved.
ɲa βaɲa ɲa ‘defecate’
ɲaaɲa βaɲaaɲa ɲaaɲa ‘chew’
ɲwa βaɲwa ɲwa ‘drink’
In the case of mala ‘I finish,’ the underlying form would be /n-mala/ which
would undergo the place assimilation rule (14), resulting in *mmala.
According to the data available to us, there are no sequences of nasals in
the language, so it is reasonable to posit the following rule.
The underlying form of fuma ‘I spread’ is /n-fuma/ since the prefix for 1sg
is /n-/ and the root is /fuma/, and this contains a sequence nasal plus
voiceless fricative. Our data indicate that this sequence does not appear
anywhere in the language, so we may presume that such sequences are
eliminated by a rule of nasal deletion. The formulation in (25) accounts for
the deletion facts of (24).
⎡+cont ⎤
[+nasal ] → Ø /__ ⎢-voice⎥
⎣ ⎦
There can be an important connection between how rules are formulated
and how they are ordered. In the analysis presented here, we posited the
rules Nasal Deletion (25) and Post-Nasal Voicing-Hardening (19), repeated
here, where Nasal Deletion applies first.
⎡+voice⎤
C→ ⎢ ⎥ / [+nas]
⎣-cont ⎦
126 INTRODUCTION PHONOLOGY
⎡+ cont⎤
[ +nasal ] → Ø/__ ⎢- voice⎥
⎣ ⎦
⎡ +syl ⎤
⎡αant ⎤ ⎢ ⎥
[+nas] → ⎢βcor ⎥ / ⎢ αant⎥
⎣ ⎦
⎣ βcor ⎦
⎢ ⎥
(21) l-Deletion
⎡+voice⎤
C→ ⎢ ⎥ / [+nas]
⎣-cont ⎦
[ +nas] → Ø/ [ +nas ]
Despite some similarity in these processes, which involve a common
environment of nasal-plus-consonant, there is no reasonable way to state
these processes as one rule.
Interacting processes 127
5.1.4 Matuumbi
The following data from Matuumbi illustrate the different surface real-
izations of the noun-class prefixes (nouns are assigned lexically or syntac-
tically to different classes, conventionally numbered between 1 and 21).
You should be able to discern and formalize the rule that applies in
these data, and order those rules correctly. What rule applies in the
following data?
The examples in (27) illustrate three rules. First, there is an optional rule When a rule is
applying in both subsets of (27) which deletes u after m, hence in these optional, that fact
words, the prefix /mu/ can be pronounced in two ways, one with u and one is indicated by
without u. You should formalize the optional vowel deletion rule illus- writing “(optional)”
trated by these data. to the right of the
An independent rule assimilates a nasal to the place of articulation of rule – see chapter 2.
the following consonant (we saw this rule in previous Matuumbi data in
chapters 2 and 4). This rule applies in both subsets of examples, and is the
only other rule besides deletion of u involved in the first subset. The third
rule applies in the second subset of examples, and explains the change in
the initial consonant of the stem. This rule only applies to a glide preceded
by a nasal which is separated by a morpheme boundary, notated in
rules as “+.”
How do you explain the following examples of nouns, which also have
singulars in class 7 and plurals in class 8, given that the class prefixes in
these examples are underlyingly /ki-/ and /i-/?
In order to see what these data show, we must first understand the mor-
phological structure of these words, a step which leads us to realize that the
pronunciation of certain morphemes changes, depending on their phonetic
context. Verbs in Lamba are composed of a root of the form CV(C)C, an
optional derivational affix marking passive, neuter, applied or reciprocal,
and a word-final suffix -a which marks the form as being a verb. The
underlying forms of the passive and reciprocal suffixes are clearly -w- and
-an-, since they exhibit no phonetic variations. The neuter and applied suf-
fixes appear phonetically as -ik- and -ek-, -il- and -el-. The choice of vowel in the
suffix is determined by the vowel which precedes the suffix: if the verb root
contains the vowel i, u, or a the suffix has the vowel i, and if the root contains
the vowel e or o the suffix has the vowel e. The group of vowels i, u, a is not
a natural phonetic class, so it is implausible that the suffixes are underlyingly
-el- and -ek- with -il- and -ik- being derived by a rule. The class of vowels e, o is
the phonetic class of mid vowels; it is thus evident that this language has a
vowel harmony rule which assimilates underlying high vowels (in the
suffixes /il/ and /ik/) to mid vowels when they are preceded by mid vowels.
b. Fricative Palatalization
⎡+syl ⎤
⎡+cont⎤ ⎢ ⎥
⎢+cor ⎥ → [- ant] / ⎢+high⎥
⎣ ⎦ ⎢
⎣-back⎥
⎦
Thus, applying the rules in this order gives the wrong results: this order
cannot be correct.
On the other hand, if we apply the processes in the other order, with
height harmony applying before palatalization, then the correct form is
generated.
We would assume that the underlying forms of the prefixes are /at/ and
/ap/, and that there is a rule which voices obstruents before voiced
obstruents.
The alternative hypothesis would be that the prefixes are underlyingly /ad/
and /ab/. However, there is no natural context for describing the process of
devoicing. Although devoicing of voiced obstruents before voiceless
obstruents is quite natural, assuming that the prefixes have underlying
voiced obstruents would also require the consonant to be devoiced before
vowels and sonorant consonants, in order to account for the supposed
derivations /ad-eiti/ ! [ateiti], /ab-eiti/ ! [apeiti], /ad-neʃti/ ! [atneʃti], and
/ab-mo:ki:ti/ ! [apmo:ki:ti]. But there is clearly no rule prohibiting voiced
obstruents before vowels and sonorants in this language (in fact, no
language has ever been attested with a rule of consonant devoicing where
the conditioning environment is a following vowel). On the basis of this
reasoning, we conclude that the prefixes have underlying voiceless
consonants.
When the initial consonant of the root is an alveolar stop, the vowel [i]
appears after the prefix /at/, and similarly when the initial consonant is a
bilabial stop, [i] is inserted after the consonant of /ap/.
Given just the voicing assimilation rule, you would expect forms such as
*[adduoti], *[abberti] by analogy to [adbekti] and [abdauʒi:ti]. Lithuanian
does not allow sequences of identical consonants, so to prevent such a
result, an epenthetic vowel is inserted between homorganic obstruent
stops (ones having the same values for the place of articulation features).
(38) Epenthesis
⎡ +syl ⎤
⎢ ⎥
⎡-cont
-son
⎤ ⎡--cont
son
⎤
Ø → ⎢ +high⎥ /
⎢αant ⎥ ⎢αant ⎥
⎣ -back⎥
⎢ ⎦ ⎣βcor ⎦ ⎣βcor ⎦
The ordering of these rules is important: epenthesis (38) must apply before
voicing assimilation, since otherwise the prefix consonant would assimi-
late the voicing of the root-initial consonant and would then be separated
from that consonant by the epenthetic vowel. The result of applying the
voicing assimilation rule first would be to create [adduoti], [abberti], and
then this would undergo vowel epenthesis to give incorrect *[adiduoti],
*[abiberti]. If, on the other hand, epenthesis is the first rule applied, then
underlying /at-duoti/ becomes [atiduoti] and /ap-berti/ becomes [apiberti].
Epenthesis eliminates the underlying cluster of obstruents, preventing
the voicing rule from applying.
The underlying forms of the subject prefixes are /N/ (which stands for a
nasal consonant, whose exact place of articulation cannot be determined),
/o/, /a/, /to/, /lo/, and /ba/. There is a vowel harmony process assimilating the
closed vowel /o/ to the open vowel [ɔ] when the following syllable contains
either of the open vowels [ε] or [ɔ], and the prefix for first-singular subject
assimilates in place of articulation to the following consonant.
The examples in (41) show how the subject prefixes are realized if the
verb root begins with a vowel.
When the first-singular subject prefix stands before the root, it has the
shape [ndʒ], which we will treat as being the result of insertion of [dʒ]
between the prefix and a vowel-initial root. (We might also assume the
134 INTRODUCTION PHONOLOGY
The vowel /a/ deletes before another vowel, as shown by the third-singular
and third-plural forms /a-εna/ ! [εna] and /ba-εna/ ! [bεna].
The prefixes /o/, /to/, and /lo/ undergo a process of glide formation where /o/
becomes [w] before a vowel.
Turning /l/ into an
affricate seems (44) Glide formation
strange from a [+round] ! [–syl] / _ V
functional
perspective, but is In the case of /to/ and /lo/ a further process affricates these consonants
explained by the before a glide.
fact that l was
originally /d/, so (45) Affrication
this rule comes ⎡-syl ⎤
historically from the [+cor] → [+del.rel] / ⎢+high⎥
⎣ ⎦
more natural
change /t, d/! This affrication process must apply after glide formation, since it applies
[ts, dz]/ _ i. to a sequence of consonant plus glide that is created by the application of
glide formation from an underlying consonant-plus-vowel sequence.
The final set of examples illustrates verb roots which underlyingly
begin with the consonant /b/. As these data show, when underlying /b/ is
preceded by a vowel, it is deleted.
Thus, surface [oina] derives from /obina/ and [baina] derives from /babina/,
via the following rule.
glide formation, those rules do not in fact apply. In other words, in this
case the grammar must contain some kind of explicit statement
regarding the interaction of these processes, such as an explicit
ordering of the rules, which guarantees that the output of b-deletion
does not undergo glide formation or a-deletion. By ordering the
b-deletion rule so that it applies after the glide formation and vowel
truncation rules, we explain why those two rules fail to apply, just
in case the consonant b is deleted intervocalically. The ordering where
b-deletion precedes vowel truncation and glide formation, illustrated in
(48b), results in ungrammatical forms, which shows that that ordering
of the rules is incorrect. (“NA” means that the rule cannot apply,
because the conditions called for in the rule are not satisfied in
the string.)
The precise rules which you postulate will depend on what you assume to
be the underlying form of the passive suffix, since there are two plausible
underlying forms for the suffix, based on the data above. The phonological
alternations seen in the following examples are relevant to deciding what
the underlying form of the passive suffix is (and therefore exactly how
Interacting processes 137
The second process, syncope, deletes a short vowel from the first syllable
of a stem when preceded by a CV prefix and followed by CV.
Exercises
1 Kerewe
What two tone rules are motivated by the following data? Explain what order the
rules apply in. Vowels have no accent with L tone: treat H tones as [+H] and
L tones as [−H].
2 Mbunga
Account for the phonological alternations in the following data. Note that there
are two roots for ‘beat,’ ‘cut,’ ‘rub,’ also there are derivational relations indicated
with suffixes (-el-, -il-, -is-, -es-, etc.) which you need not account for: except for
the difference between final -a and final -i which mark different tenses, you do
not need to be concerned with possible suffixes and alternations caused by
suffixes.
140 INTRODUCTION PHONOLOGY
3 Polish
What phonological rules are motivated by the following examples, and what
order do those rules apply in?
4 Logoori
Account for the vowel alternations in the following data. Tone may be ignored.
‘they just V’d’ ‘they just V’d for’ ‘they will V ‘they will V for
(rem. fut)’ (rem. fut)’
váakátáanga váakátáángɪra varakátáangɛ varakátáángɪrɪ ‘start’
váakávʊ́ njaanja váakávʊ́ njaanjɪra varakávʊ́ njaanjɛ varakávʊ́ njaanjɪrɪ ‘break’
váakázáázama váakázáázamɪra varakázáázamɛ varakázáázamɪrɪ ‘taste’
váakavʊrʊganja váakavʊrʊganjɪra varakavʊrʊganjɛ varakavʊrʊganjɪrɪ ‘stir’
váakaganaganja váakaganaganjɪra varakaganaganjɛ varakaganaganjɪrɪ ‘think’
váakarʊʊnga váakarʊʊngɪra varakarʊʊngɪ varakarʊʊngɪrɪ ‘season’
váakarɪɪnda váakarɪɪndɪra varakarɪɪndɪ varakarɪɪndɪrɪ ‘guard’
váakátʊ́ ma váakátʊ́ mɪra varakátʊ́ mɪ varakátʊ́ mɪrɪ ‘send’
váakásúunga váakásúúngɪra varakásúungɪ varakásúúngɪrɪ ‘hang up’
váakatuuma váakatuumɪra varakatuumɪ varakatuumɪrɪ ‘jump’
váakávɪń a váakávɪń ɪra varakávɪń ɪ varakávɪń ɪrɪ ‘dance’
váakavisa váakavisɪra varakavisɪ varakavisɪrɪ ‘hide’
váakávɪt́ a váakávɪt́ ɪra varakávɪt́ ɪ varakávɪt́ ɪrɪ ‘pass’
váakágámʊra váakágámʊrɪra varakágámʊrɪ varakágámʊrɪrɪ ‘catch’
váakahɪɪrɪɪta váakahɪɪrɪɪtɪra varakahɪɪrɪɪtɪ varakahɪɪrɪɪtɪrɪ ‘snore’
váakadiginja váakadiginjɪra varakadiginjɪ varakadiginjɪrɪ ‘tickle’
váakáhákiza váakáhákizɪra varakáhákizɪ varakáhákizɪrɪ ‘scorch’
váakavariza váakavarizɪra varakavarizɪ varakavarizɪrɪ ‘count’
váakáfʊ́ njɪɪriza váakáfʊ́ njɪɪrizɪra varakáfʊ́ njɪɪrizɪ varakáfʊ́ njɪɪrizɪrɪ ‘smell’
váakamɪnɪɪka váakamɪnɪɪkɪra varakamɪnɪɪkɪ varakamɪnɪɪkɪrɪ ‘be ill’
váakagarʊkiza váakagarʊkizɪra varakagarʊkizɪ varakagarʊkizɪrɪ ‘reverse’
váakarʊʊngikiza váakarʊʊngikizɪra varakarʊʊngikizɪ varakarʊʊngikizɪrɪ ‘straighten’
váakátʊ́ ŋaminja váakátʊ́ ŋaminjɪra varakátʊ́ ŋaminjɪ varakátʊ́ ŋaminjɪrɪ ‘invert’
váakasjɛɛna váakasjɛɛnɛra varakasjɛɛnɛ varakasjɛɛnɛrɛ ‘step’
váakárɛ́ ɛmba váakárɛ́ ɛ́ mbɛra varakárɛ́ ɛmbɛ varakárɛ́ ɛ́ mbɛrɛ ‘scold’
váakárɔ́ ɔta váakárɔ́ ɔ́ tɛra varakárɔ́ ɔtɛ varakárɔ́ ɔ́ tɛrɛ ‘dream’
váakasɛka váakasɛkɛra varakasɛkɛ varakasɛkɛrɛ ‘laugh’
váakatɛɛva váakatɛɛvɛra varakatɛɛvɛ varakatɛɛvɛrɛ ‘ask’
váakáhɛ́ ɛnza váakáhɛ́ ɛ́ nzɛra varakáhɛ́ ɛnzɛ varakáhɛ́ ɛ́ nzɛrɛ ‘seek’
váakarɔɔnda váakarɔɔndɛra varakarɔɔndɛ varakarɔɔndɛrɛ ‘follow’
váakárɛ́ ɛta váakárɛ́ ɛ́ tɛra varakárɛ́ ɛtɛ varakárɛ́ ɛ́ tɛrɛ ‘bring’
váakádɛ́ ɛka váakádɛ́ ɛ́ kɛra varakádɛ́ ɛkɛ varakádɛ́ ɛ́ kɛrɛ ‘cook’
váakámɛ́ ɲa váakámɛ́ ɲɛra varakámɛ́ ɲɛ varakámɛ́ ɲɛrɛ ‘live’
váakasɔɔma váakasɔɔmɛra varakasɔɔmɛ varakasɔɔmɛrɛ ‘read’
váakávɛ́ ga váakávɛ́ gɛra varakávɛ́ gɛ varakávɛ́ gɛrɛ ‘shave’
váakamɔrɔma váakamɔrɔmɛra varakamɔrɔmɛ varakamɔrɔmɛrɛ ‘speak’
váakarɛka váakarɛkɛra varakarɛkɛ varakarɛkɛrɛ ‘stop’
The following nouns illustrate a productive pattern for making nouns ‘tool for
V-ing with’:
ividujɪrʊ ‘pound’ ivivɛ́ gɛrɔ ‘shave’ ivikáragɪrʊ ‘cut’
ivisɔɔmɛrɔ ‘read’ ivivɪń ɪrʊ ‘dance’ ivinágɪllʊ ‘catch’
ivisɛɛmbɛllɔ ‘cultivate’ ivirʊʊngɪrʊ ‘season’
142 INTRODUCTION PHONOLOGY
5 Shona
Acute accent indicates H tone and unaccented vowels have L tone. Given the
two sets of data immediately below, what tone rule do the following data
motivate? There are alternations in the form of adjectives, e.g. kurefú, karefú,
marefú all meaning ‘long.’ Adjectives have an agreement prefix, hence ku-refú
marks the form of the adjective in one grammatical class, and so on. In some
cases, the agreement is realized purely as a change in the initial consonant of
the adjective, i.e. gúrú ~ kúrú ~ húrú, which need not be explained.
These data provide further illustration of the operation of this tone rule, which
will help you to state the conditions on the rule correctly.
6 Guerze
Account for the phonological alternations in the following data from Guerze. Be
sure that you state the order of the rules which you propose, and justify your
conclusion about ordering.
7 Catalan
Give phonological rules which account for the following data, and indicate what
ordering is necessary between these rules. For each adjective stem, state what
the underlying form of the root is. Pay attention to the difference between
surface [b, d, g] and [β, ð, γ], in terms of predictability.
8 Finnish
Propose rules which will account for the following alternations. It would be best
not to write a lot of rules which go directly from underlying forms to surface
forms in one step; instead, propose a sequence of rules whose combined effect
brings about the change in the underlying form. Pay attention to what
consonants actually exist in the language.
...................................................................................................................................
Further reading
Anderson 1974; Chomsky 1967; Goldsmith 1990b; Kiparsky 1968a; Koutsoudas, Sanders, and Noll 1974.