Alutor Grammar (Kibrik, Kodzasov & Muravyova)
Alutor Grammar (Kibrik, Kodzasov & Muravyova)
Language and Folklore of the Alutor People. Megumi Kurebito (ed.). Suita, Japan,
2004. 656 p.
CONTENTS:
Foreword for the English version
Editor's word
Abbreviations and symbols
Preface
Part I. Alutor Folklore
Part II. Grammar
Part III. Alutor-English Dictionary
Appendix. Samples of Conjugation
References
PART TWO
ALUTOR GRAMMAR
Pp. 203–337
CHAPTER 1. Phonology
CHAPTER 2. Morphology
CHAPTER 3. Syntax
CHAPTER 1. Phonology
In the present phonological sketch we will describe the following two main aspects of
Alutor phonology: the static aspect, that is inventories of units (segments and
suprasegmentals), and the dynamic aspect, that is phonological processes. The static
part deals with the sounds of Alutor − phones and phonemes (1.1), as well as the
syllable and rhythmic patterns (1.2). The dynamic part deals with the phonological
processes of Alutor, which are presented in the form of rewriting rules (1.3).
The approach we use here is similar to that of generative phonology (see also
[Kodzasov, Krivnova 1981]). Two levels of representing Alutor words are
distinguished: the underlying level, that is the level of phonemes (it corresponds to
the morphophonemic level in the Russian traditional phonology) and the surface
level, that is the level of phones, or systematic phonetic level (we omit some minor
details of Alutor pronunciation, irrelevant for the phonetic system). Rewriting rules
apply to the underlying representation of a word, which is a sequence of underlying
representations of the corresponding morphemes. Underlying representations of
Alutor morphemes are given in the Dictionary – see Part III. The application of
rewriting rules results in the surface representation, or the phonetic transcription.
In the present section the underlying representation of Alutor words and
morphemes is given in braces. Phonetic and practical transcriptions of words are
usually not parenthesized, but in some special cases the phonetic transcription is
given in square brackets. The application of rewriting rules is shown in round
brackets. The inventories of the surface and the underlying representations are
identical, that means we use one and the same chart of symbols for writing phones
and phonemes.
1.1. Segments
1.1.1. Vowels
1.1.1.1. Inventory
The vocalic system of Alutor consists of 11 vowels. They are: [i], [I], [u], [U], [e],
[E], [o], [O], [y], [a], [A]. The phonological status of each vowel is discussed further.
The Alutor vowels represent a typical triangle system. They are distinguished
in terms of q u a l i t y and q u a n t i t y . The aspects of quality are: t o n g u e h e i g h t
(high vs. mid vs. low), f r o n t n e s s / b a c k n e s s (front vs. central vs. back) and l i p
r o u n d i n g (rounded vs. unrounded).
The classification of vowels is given below in Table 1. The vowel [y], or schwa,
differs from all the other vowels both in length and in function. It is a supershort
vowel, while all the other vowels are of normal length (short or long – see below).
2
The phonological status of this segment and its functions are discussed in 1.1.1.2.
Besides quality, we also distinguish vowels in terms of quantity: the non-schwa
vowels can be either short or long 1 . The vowel length is often conditioned by the
position of a vowel in a word, but in some cases there is a phonological opposition
between short and long vowels. The vowel length in Alutor deserves further
discussion: the length contrast of e and o is discussed in 1.1.1.3, the length contrast of
i, u, a is discussed in 1.1.1.4.
mid e, E o, O
y
low a, A
COMMENTS on Table 1
The front and the back vowels are rather close to the central ones. The back vowels are
rounded: u is strongly rounded, while o is moderately rounded. The mid vowels stand at
approximately equal distance from the high and low vowels: they are neither open nor close. The
vowel y is a central mid vowel, but its main variant is a little bit lower than e and o. The long
vowels do not differ considerably in quality from their short counterparts.
1
Previously, long vowels were found neither in Alutor nor in any other Chukchi-
Kamchatkan language (cf. [Moll 1955; Zhukova 1968] and others).
3
often used as the syllable nucleus to create well-formed syllables and to eliminate
illicit consonant clusters.
However, in some other cases the schwa is unpredictable, because it is difficult
or even impossible to formulate more or less standard phonological rules defining
schwa epenthesis in the non-vocalized representation. In such situations the fact that a
schwa is pronounced in a given word can be explained only by the individual
property of a given morpheme. In other words, we think that in such cases y should be
considered an inherent constituent of the corresponding morpheme. Many roots and
affixes therefore contain the schwa in the underlying representation, cf. the root {ypa}
‘soup’ appearing in the following words: @ypôNa (< {ypa-Na})‘soup’, tynypatyn (<
{t-n-ypa-at-n}) ‘I cooked it’ 2 . We also treat some root-medial y that always appear
in one the same place in a morpheme as part of the corresponding underlying
representation, cf. tímkytym 3 ‘hummock’, gatímkylin ‘covered with hummocks’ (the
root {tymk}); tyHylyk ‘be ill’, gatHíllin ‘(s)he fell ill’ (the root {tHyl}), myt-jôn-myk
(< {myt-jat-myk}) ‘we (two) came’ (the prefix {myt} and the suffix {myk}). In other
words, we always include the schwa in the underlying representation in case is not
inserted by means of the standard rules listed below. On these grounds we consider
the schwa an independent phoneme of Alutor.
Thus, schwas in Alutor words can be of two different origins: some y are part of
the underlying representation (“original” y), some appear only in the surface
representation, they are epenthesized by means of standard rules (“epenthetic” y). In
the phonetic transcription we write the vowel y in all positions, as all linguists
2
We treat the glottal stop in @ypANa as a segment epethesized before a word-initial vowel (a
situation similar to German, Arabic etc.). We can certainly choose another variant for the
underlying representation – without y, but with the initial @ , that is {*@pa}, but in this case the
second word would appear as *tynApavyn (< *ty-ny-@pa-vy-n < *{t-n-@pa-v-n}), because in
syllable-final position y@ changes into A (this alternation is typical for the conjunctive − see 1.3.6).
Since it is not the word we have in reality, we choose {ypa} as the underlying representation.
3
The non-epenthetic vowels y coming from the underlying representation are underlined
here.
4
studying this language group do 4 .
4
It is worth mentioning that native speakers of Alutor are not aware of the schwa as a
segment of their pronunciation, whether predictable or not. They think some Alutor words have
only consonants; at the same time, they can easily divide such words into syllables. For instance, a
word which sounds like pyNílly ‘news’ is treated as having three syllables – |p|Nl|l| (the symbol “ |
” indicates syllable boundaries), wítwyt ‘flower’ – as |wt|wt|, HítvyHyt ‘boat’ – as |Ht|v|Ht|. The
same is true for the Koryak language. E.A.Kreynovich [Kreynovich 1958] proposed for Koryak
words the non-vocalised representation (that is without y), but with marked syllable boundaries,
like |wt|wt|. Since the schwa is not part of such representation, the author does not consider it a
phoneme. In our opinion, such approach in neither adequate nor typologically justified, because
there arises a problem of dividing words into syllables – some words have unpredictable syllable
division. Nevertheless, E.A.Kreynovich was the first to pay attention to the crucial importance of
the notion of syllable for Chukchi-Kamchatkan languages. As we will see further, this notion is
absolutely necessary to create well-formed Alutor words.
5
From an historical point of view, the Alutor long E and O originated mostly from aj and
aw, or from yj and yw, and original e and o changed into i and u in most positions (they are
preserved only in closed syllables before or after q and H – see above), cf. the following related
words from Alutor and Koryak: Alutor jepílNyn [E] – Koryak jajpylNyn ‘hoof’, Alutor lJotítkyn
[O] – Koryak lJawtytkyn ‘the top of smth.’, Alutor @imNãlyk – Koryak @emNolyk ‘to miss (smb.)’,
but Alutor lóqtyk – Koryak leqtyk ‘to come back’, Alutor qùnpyN – Koryak qonpyN ‘always’
(these correspondences are discussed in detail in [Muravyova 1986a]).
5
allophones of one and the same phoneme – E or O. For practical reasons we do not
mark the length on e and o in practical transcriptions of Alutor words (except in
expressive words) − see 1.3.12.
6
Here the schwa does not break up an illicit cluster, because the corresponding cluster may
appear in another word (see 1.2.1). Such examples have reference to another phonological
constraint: a syllable with a long vowel cannot be a closed one. Thus we have nypØtyqin (while
*nypItqin is wrong), HÔlyHal (while *HAl Hal is wrong).
6
syllable, for instance, {pIty} ‘old’, {HAly} ‘axe’ 7 . As for the practical transcription, we
mark the vowel length on such vowels in all positions except in closed syllables.
The vowel A also occurs in the conjunctive, where it comes from y@ under
certain phonological conditions (see 1.3.3.2 and 1.3.6), cf. nAjôtyn (< ny-@-jaty-n <
{n-@-jat-n}) ‘if (s)he came’. It should be also noted that in Alutor the glottal stop
never occurs syllable-finally 8 .
1.1.1.5. Examples
In the examples given below the Alutor vowels are shown in the following positions:
1) word-initially (after a glottal stop), both in an open and closed syllable; 2) post-
consonantally, both in an open and closed syllable; 3) word-finally − either stressed
or not (a stressed vowel is marked with “ ′ ”, an unaccentuated word is marked with “
& ” − see 1.2);
[i]: @øvyk ‘say’, @ønmyk ‘swallow (smth.)’, mytønuk ‘eat berries’, møgga ‘who’,
@itítkyn ‘(s)he/it is’, @imNãlyk ‘miss (smb.)’, titítkyn ‘I am’, timNãlytkyn ‘I miss
him/her/it’, nínni ‘name’;
[I]: @Igítkyn ‘it is getting cold’, qItítkyn ‘(s)he/it is getting frozen’, @Øgyka ‘it is
cold’, nyqØtyqin ‘frozen’;
[u]: @ãvik ‘body’, @ãtty@ut ‘tree’, HasãHas ‘pink salmon’, tynãppy ‘hill’,
@umôkatyk ‘gather’, @ujøsvatyk ‘play’, rurãNa ‘bed’, tumôkavyk ‘gather (smth.,
smb.)’, @ínnu ‘(s)he/it’;
[U]: lUvítkyn ‘(s)he is sucking’, lÃvyk ‘suck’;
[e]: &gemat ‘maybe’, lóqtyk ‘come back’, leqtítkyn ‘(s)he is coming back’,
rarôtenyk ‘near a house’, @ísse ‘aunt’;
[E]: @ógyv ‘yesterday’, @ekãlatyk ‘make the bed’, jówal ‘orphan’, jewôsatyk
‘feel pity (for smb.)’;
[o]: Hùpta ‘entirely’, HoptílJ@u ‘all’, tytôtoratkyn ‘I'll sink’, &Horo ‘then’;
[O]: @ùjik ‘eat’, tojøtkyn ‘I am eating’, tùrak ‘sink’, torôtkyn ‘(s)he/it is
sinking’;
[y]: @ylla ‘mother’, @ypôNa ‘soup’, @yllôNi ‘younger brother’, nípqin ‘steep’,
@ypôtyk ‘cook soup’, typkôvyk ‘I was unable (to do smth.)’, Nôlly ‘herd’;
[a]: @ôkyk ‘son’, @ôngyt ‘festival’, @ujôtik ‘sledge’, môtka ‘whether’,
7
There are some examples in which I originated from ij, for instance, Alutor sIpítkyn
(historically from sijpytkyn) ‘it is withering’, cf. Koryak sejpyk. But in most cases Alutor long I,
U, A correspond not to combinations with j or w, but to single vowels, for instance, Alutor vIlyvil –
Koryak velyvel ‘thimble’.
8
It is possible that in some other cases the long vowel A also originated from a syllable-final
y@ or a@. A similar historical change is also possible for the long I (from i@ ) and U (from u@ ).
7
@anã@an ‘spring’, @alpínNyn ‘patch’, kaNãnaN ‘fishhook’, kaltøkal ‘beetle’, vøtga
‘at once’;
[A]: HAlítkuk ‘hew’, HÔlyHal ‘axe’.
1.1.2. Consonants
1.1.2.1. Inventory
The Alutor inventory of consonants is not large, it includes only 18 segments. The
consonants are distinguished in terms of p l a c e o f a r t i c u l a t i o n (labials vs.
dentalveolars vs. palatals vs. velars vs. uvulars vs. glottal laryngeals vs. epiglottal
laryngeals), m a n n e r o f a r t i c u l a t i o n (obstruents vs. sonorants; plosives vs.
fricatives vs. vibrants vs. glides; central vs. lateral), n a s a l i t y (oral vs. nasal) and
s e c o n d a r y a r t i c u l a t i o n (non-palatalized vs. palatalized). The classification of
consonants is given below in Table 2.
The status of each consonant will be discussed further. Alutor consonants are
divided into two subsystems − obstruents and sonorants. They differ greatly, so we
would like to consider them separately.
1.1.2.2. Obstruents
The obstruents are subdivided into two classes: plosives and fricatives. In Alutor all
the plosives are voiceless, and all the fricatives are voiced. The Alutor fricatives have
a rather broad passageway, so they are similar to sonorants. Fricatives are represented
by only two segments − v and g. It is remarkable that while Alutor fricative obstruents
8
are not numerous, there is great variety to the sonorants. 9
Alutor obstruents occur at all the places of articulation, except for the absence
of palatals.
Labials are represented by the two consonants − p and v.
Dentalveolars (both obstruents and sonorants) are represented by the dentals t,
n, l and the alveolar r. The dentals contrast in secondary articulation − palatalization;
they may be either non-palatalized or palatalized. The non-palatalized dental t does
not have a voiced fricative correlate 10 . The exact palatalized correlate of t would be tJ,
but this is not the sound we usually find in Alutor (actually, the sound tJ does occur,
but only in some Alutor words, cf. matka ~ matJka ‘interrogative particle’). The
sound we consider the palatalized correlate of t is represented mainly by two variants,
both close to the palatalized tJ, but not identical with it: the affricate tsJ and the
fricative sJ, the latter similar to a palatal fricative. The affricate occurs mainly in the
speech of the old generation, while the fricative is more widely used, it occurs in the
speech of young and middle-aged people. In papers on Alutor the fricative sound is
usually registered as s; in the present paper we also use s to write this sound in the
practical transcription. It is remarkable that the morphological alternation t/tsJ occurs
approximately in the same contexts as the alternations 1/1J, n/nJ (see 1.3.10). On
these grounds we refer to tsJ as the palatalized correlate of t. The phoneme tsJ has no
voiced counterpart 11 .
Velars are represented by the plosive k and the fricative g. The fricative is
rather close to the sonorants, it often sounds very much like the labial velar w 12 . It is
remarkable that g changes into w syllable-finally (see 1.3.5).
Uvulars are represented by the plosive q, which is slightly pharyngealized. In
9
It is possible that the Alutor system is changing from a system with two classes of
obstruents − plosives and fricatives, to a system with one class of plosives. That is just how the
Koryak consonants are often described (see [Trubetskoy 1958, Zhukova 1972]): all non-plosives
are treated as sonorants. But there are some typological facts that should be taken into
consideration: first, two labial non-nasal sounds are not common in the class of sonorants; second,
if g were a sonorant, we would have its vocalic correlate – a high back non-rounded vowel, but
there is no such vowel in the Alutor system. That is why we prefer to describe the Alutor system
as contrasting between plosives and fricatives. Cf. also the reconstruction proposed in [Mudrak
2000].
10
The corresponding phoneme is reconstructed for the Chukchi-Koryak protolanguage
[Muravyova 1986a]: in Alutor it coincided with t, in Koryak − with j/č, in Chukchi − with r, cf.
Alutor tiNak − Koryak jiNek − Chukchi riNek ‘fly’.
11
Even if there had been such a sound in the proto-Alutor language, it is likely to have
coincided with the palatal j.
12
Such a sound exists, for example, in the Polish language.
9
most cases it is a plosive, but sometimes it sounds like an affricate 13 .
Laryngeals deserve special consideration. The glottal stop @ is sometimes used
as an auxiliary segment, for instance, it is found before any word-initial vowel. In
such contexts the glottal stop is predictable: it is inserted in accordance with the
standard rules (see 1.3.3.1). But in other contexts this segment is unpredictable, so it
must be treated as part of the underlying representation, cf. ny@ôlyk (< {n@al-k})
‘become’ (the root {n@al}), @yllô@a (< {ylla@-a}) ‘by a mother’ (the root {ylla@}),
nymôl@a (< {n-mal-@a}) ‘well’ (the circumfix {n-...-@a}). In syllable-final position
the glottal stop behaves similar to the glides w and j (see 1.3.6). The sound H is an
epiglottal plosive laryngeal, that is a glottal stop accompanied by epiglottis
lowering 14 . Due to such configuration the larynx becomes closed and the pharynx is
narrowed in its lower part. Like the uvular q, the Alutor H is also slightly
pharyngealized. That is why both q and H make the adjacent vowels more wide. There
is an obvious contrast between the sounds @ and H, cf. jã@yk ‘reach’ − vøHyk ‘die’, but
the fact was not mentioned in previous papers on Alutor 15 .
1.1.2.3. Sonorants
Among the sonorants the most numerous group is the nasals; here we find the labial
m, two dentals – the non-palatalized n and the palatalized nJ, and the velar N.
Laterals are represented by the non-palatalized l and the palatalized lJ.
The palatalized dentals nJ and lJ belong to the same place of articulation as their
non-palatalized correlates l and n. The non-palatalized dentals t, n and l, like all other
non-palatalized sounds, are not velarized 16 , that is why they do not velarise the
following front vowels. It should be also noted that the opposition n/nJ, l/lJ is not
neutralized before the front vowels, so both variants − a palatalized and a non-
palatalized one − occur there (cf. kôlik ‘write’ vs. kalJøsityk ‘write (repeatedly)’).
The remainder of the sonorants do not require special comment. The consonants
13
That is why in Russian borrowings the Russian fricative x changes into the Alutor q, cf.
qylJøppy ‘bread’ (from the Russian xl’eb), @ãrvaq ‘shirt, dress’ (from the Russian rubáxa), &kolqos
‘collective farm’ (from the Russian kolxóz).
14
In the Koryak language [Zhukova 1972] the corresponding phoneme is realised as the
fricative pharyngeal and is written in the transcription as h.
15
The initial Alutor @ corresponds to the Koryak laryngeal @, while the non-initial Alutor @
and H (the latter – both initially and non-initially) correspond to the Koryak pharyngeal fricative
H, cf. the corresponding Koryak words jãHyk ‘reach’, vøHyk ‘die’.
16
Actually, sometimes there occurs a velarized variant of l (we often transcribed this sound
as w, not as l), but this may be due to the influence of the Russian language, where l is strongly
velarized.
10
w and j are glides, which may coalesce with the preceding vowel syllable-finally (see
1.3.6). As we have mentioned above, the consonant @ also acts like a glide in the
same context.
1.1.2.4. Examples
In the examples given below the Alutor consonants are shown in the following
positions: 1) word-initially, 2) intervocalicly, 3) post-consonantally, 4) pre-
consonantally, *5) word-finally.
[p]: pãnta ‘liver’, pãpu ‘float’, nínpyqin ‘old’, Hùpta ‘entirely’, @ipø@ip
‘smoke;
[v]: vøtku ‘just now’, @avølJuki ‘disobedient’, HítvyHyt ‘boat’, nívqin ‘black’,
givøgiv ‘year’;
[t]: tôtul ‘fox’, gøtak ‘look’, @ôktyka ‘impossible’, títka ‘walrus’, @ãkit
‘herring’;
[sJ]: sôwat ‘lasso’, Hurôsik ‘young man’, @ãmsan ‘firewood’, vôsqin ‘another’,
jôvas ‘later’;
[k]: kukôNa ‘pot’, jôkak ‘bend down’, gytkôlNyn ‘leg’, níktyqin ‘hard’ wômak
‘harpoon’;
[g]: gitøNa ‘ladder’, vagôlyk ‘sit down’, @ôsgi ‘now’, jØgyjig ‘mould’;
[q]: qamôNa ‘plate’, taqôtaq ‘side, facet’, @ãmqa ‘polar bear’, qãqluk ‘make a
hole’, jôqjaq ‘seagull’;
[@]: @ôNaN ‘god, idol’, @ajô@utkuk ‘fish with a rod’, @iw@øsik ‘drink’;
[H]: HitãHit ‘goose’, vø Hyk ‘die’, tôvHa1 ‘dried fish’;
[m]: mamøNa ‘fish drying shed’, nãmal ‘again’, mørmir ‘tear’, kãmNykum
‘voice’, @ôrym ‘chief’;
[n]: nykønyk ‘night’, junôtyk ‘live’, gírnik ‘animal’, tønga ‘what’, símNa
‘draught male reindeer’;
[nJ]: nJãmqin ‘warm’, @ønJat ‘trap’, gymnJônJnJus ‘I alone’, qunJgítka ‘a leg (of
two)’;
[N]: Nøtaq ‘two’, paNôtyk ‘get tired’, NãjNyn ‘tail’, jôNta ‘separately’, @ÔsyN
‘debt’;
[l]: límgylym ‘hood’, tôlak ‘pound’, líqlaN ‘winter’, nôlgyn ‘fur skin’, mømyl
‘water’;
[lj]: lJôNi ‘maiden’, HãlJa ‘man’, @aplJôkka ‘barefoot’, kawôlJniq ‘swallow’,
lJímNylJ ‘fairy tale’;
[w]: wítwyt ‘flower’, wirãwir ‘kind of pink salmon’, kítwir ‘hair’, pøwtak ‘be
strewn’, @ewô@iw ‘brain’;
[r]: rarôNa ‘house’, qurôNa ‘reindeer’, takrôtyk ‘go down’, qôrgan ‘marine
11
animal's young’, qôpar ‘glutton’;
[j]: jãNjuN ‘whale’, môja ‘where’, @ôNjak ‘praise’, pãjgyn ‘spear’, @ãjpy@uj
‘prize’.
17
The same situation is found in related languages. Some Koryak dictionaries (for instance,
[Korsakov 1939a]) register the standard pronunciation, while others (for instance, [Zhukova
1967]) record the fluent speech, cf. myjen ~ mjen ‘mosquito’. As P.Ja.Skorik writes [Skorik
1961:63], in Chukchi the process of omitting schwas “ is more widely spread in the western
dialect than in the eastern one”.
12
No connection is found between syllable boundaries and morpheme boundaries.
At the same time, there is an obvious correlation between morpheme boundaries and
the position of epenthetic schwas: the schwa often plays the role of an
“intermorpheme segment”, cf. ní-mky-qin = n-y-mk-y-qin (< {n-mk-qin})
‘numerous’ (see 1.3.3.2).
The morphology of Alutor combines morphemes in such a way that an
underlying representation may be ill-formed. It may have illicit sound strings, such as
initial syllables without onsets, illicit consonant clusters, hiatus etc. Illicit strings
usually appear word-initially and word-finally, and also at morpheme boundaries 18 .
In order to obtain well-formed words, we must modify the underlying representation
by means of certain phonological rules, among them initial consonant deletion, glottal
stop epenthesis, schwa epenthesis, and hiatus vowel deletion (see 1.3.2 and 1.3.3).
18
The underlying representation is represented in such a way that most of illicit strings do
not appear inside a morpheme (see, for instance, 1.1.1.2).
19
The length of these segments (the vowel of an open syllable and the final consonant of a
closed syllable) has been proven through experimental research (for details see [Kodzasov,
Muravyova 1980]).
13
In unaccentuated words the vowel length contrast is hardly detectable. It should
be noted that unaccentuated words can be clearly distinguished only if the syllable
supposed to bear stress is an open one, cf. jônut [A] ‘today’ vs. &janut [a] ‘earlier’
(the symbol “ & ” indicates the absence of stress in a word), @inøri [I] ‘blanket’ vs.
&siriri [i] ‘sandpiper’. If such syllable is a closed one, it is more difficult to reveal the
absence of stress.
The position of stress in Alutor can be described in terms of syllable weight,
thus it is a quantity sensitive system. Two types of syllables are distinguished here:
l i g h t (mono-moraic) syllables and h e a v y (bi-moraic) syllables. Syllables of the
|Сy| type are light syllables, while the other types are all heavy.
The position of stress in accentuated words is determined by the following three
phonological constraints:
1) only a heavy syllable can bear stress; light syllables are always unstressed;
2) only the first or the second syllable can bear stress (but the second syllable is
preferable);
3) a word-final syllable cannot bear stress.
These constraints help us to describe all the possible rhythmic patterns. First of
all, a monosyllabic cannot bear stress, for its only syllable is the final one (but there
exist some unaccentuated monosyllablic auxiliary words − see below). In case of a
disyllabic accentuated word, the first syllable is stressed (it always appears to be a
heavy one), for instance, @ôkyk ‘son’, kíttil ‘forehead’, wôla ‘knife’ 20 . Polysyllabics
may have stress either on the first or the second syllable, cf. tølpyqal ‘shoulder’,
tírgytyr ‘meat’, nØgyqin ‘cool’, vitôtyk ‘work’, NyrôlNyn ‘knee’, vagílNyn ‘nail, claw’.
For details see the stress assignment rules (1.3.4.).
The class of unaccentuated words is represented by the following groups and
subgroups:
1) auxiliary words of different types, such as conjunctions, particles,
introductory words, and also some adverbs, cf. &qinaq ‘in order to’, &Horo ‘then’,
&matka ‘whether’, &kytvyl ‘not (prohibition)’, &gemat ‘maybe’, &agi ‘very’, &lygan
‘even’, &wasaq ‘sometimes’, &jaqi ‘later’;
2) some personal pronoun forms, such as:
a) the dative form with a light second syllable, cf. &gymykyN ‘to me’, &gynykyN
‘to you (sg)’, &@ynykyN ‘to him’, &murykyN ‘to us’, &turykyN ‘to you (pl)’, &@ytykyN
20
Actually, when morphemes are combined into a word, at intermediate stages there may
appear a monosyllabic word with the only stressed syllable or a disyllabic word with the second
stressed syllable, but such intermediate representations are then extended by means of some
special rules (see 1.3.4.3), cf. Nôjjy (< Nôj < {Naj-∅}) ‘mountain’, Nyvã-jjy (< Nyvã-j < {Nvu-j})
‘(s)he/it began’, gyvô-kki (< gyvô-k < {gva-k}) ‘to stick’.
14
‘to them’; the situation is quite different with the nominative singular which is an
accentuated form, cf. gímmy ‘I’, gítty ‘you (sg)’, @ínnu ‘(s)he/it’;
b) the nominative dual and the short form of the nominative plural − &muri ‘we
two’ and &muru ‘we’ (along with the full form murãwwi ) , &turi ‘you two’ and &turu
‘you (pl)’ (along with the full form turãwwi), &@ytti ‘they two’ and &@yttu (along with
the full form @ytãwwi) ‘they’;
3) Russian borrowings with the place of stress which does not conform to the
above-mentioned constraints, cf. &@ugurot ‘kitchen garden’ (from the Russian
ogorуd), &makasin ‘shop’ (from the Russian magasнn), &kapitan ‘captain’ (from the
Russian kapitбn); but other borrowings may be accentuated, cf. tônJa ‘Tanya’ (a
Russian proper name), marøna ‘Marina’ (a Russian proper name), qylJøppy ‘bread’
(from the Russian xl’eb);
4) words derived from some roots which represent the main parts of speech; this
group is represented by two subgroups:
а) words with a light syllable in potentially stressed position (= the second
syllable of a word), for instance &nykykaqin ‘hot’ (the root {ykyka}), {gytiN}
(&nygytiNqin ‘fast’), {jyl} (&tyjylyn ‘I gave it’), {jpyNat} (&jypyNatyk ‘stick’), {lyla}
(&@alJylJaki ‘the one having no eyes, blind’), &unJy@u ‘baby’; but some other words
derived from the same roots may become accentuated if a heavy syllable appears in
potentially stressed position, cf. tytônykykavNyn ‘I'll make it hot’ (the root {ykyka}),
lJylJôpyk ‘look’ (the root {lyla});
b) words with a heavy (usually open) syllable in potentially stressed position (=
the second syllable), for instance (each root is illustrated here by either the
nominative or the infinitive): &siriri ‘sandpiper’, &@ylJalJu ‘kid’, &@ymama ‘Mummy’,
&@ypapa ‘Daddy’, &kalJilJi ‘navel’, &lJaNi ‘girl’, &miti ‘Miti (a mythological proper
name)’, &HalJulJuk ‘ rock a baby to sleep’; a word derived from such root may
become accentuated if its second syllable is heavy and does not belong to the root, cf.
tytôHalJulJuNyn ‘I'll rock him/her to sleep’.
Unaccentuated auxiliary words and unaccentuated roots (that is group 4b) are
marked in the Dictionary.
Besides the above-enumerated classes, there is one more group of words with
peculiar prosodic properties. These are expressive words (mostly interjections) and
expressive variants of ordinary words, namely the “distant” vocative, the “distant”
locative and others. In such words the last vowel is lengthened (for details see
1.3.8.1) 21 , for instance, kolJomE (interjection of admiration), qyvyttE ‘I do not know!’
(interjection), @awEn ‘oh, crab!’ (cf. @awønaN ‘crab’), @asO ‘oh, salmon!’ (cf.
21
See also [Kodzasov 1975].
15
HasãHas ‘pink salmon’), tumgytOm ‘oh, friend!’ (cf. tãmgytum ‘friend’), @akOk ‘oh,
son!’ (cf. @ôkyk ‘son’), telON ‘there (farther)’ (cf. tólyN ‘there’), HøHOk ‘high in the
sky’ (cf. HøHyk ‘in the sky’), @amkykO ‘very many’ (cf. @ômkyka ‘many’). Native
speakers do not perceive such long vowels as stressed ones.
22
Here we follow the logical classification of phonological processes given in [Mel’иuk
1996].
16
special morpheme properties (also shown by the lowered script). Irrelevant segments
that separate X from A or B are marked in the context with “ ... ”. The symbol ∧
represents the null segment. The following symbols are used to indicate different
types of boundaries: “ # ” for a word boundary, “ = ” for a composite boundary, and “
- ” for an ordinary morpheme boundary. To describe the context, the logical
conjunctions “and” and “or” are sometimes used (they are underlined). All the rules
given below are illustrated by Alutor words. For each word we write its practical
transcription (see 1.3.12) and show its “phonological derivation”, that is a sequence
of several relevant intermediate stages coming from the underlying representation.
The derivation of a word is shown in round brackets and is marked with “ < ” (which
means “comes from”). The underlying representation is given in braces, the relevant
intermediate steps – without any brackets (but within the round brackets). Other
notations used in the rules are listed in Abbreviations and Symbols.
1.3.1. Reduplication
Reduplication in Alutor is used as a means of forming the nominative singular. The
information whether a noun employs reduplication to form the nominative singular is
shown in the Dictionary, the exact morphological structure of a word is defined by
morphological rules, so in this section we only describe the way a root copy is
formed. In Alutor a root copy always follows the corresponding root and consists of
only one syllable.
The root copy of a given reduplication is formed according to the following
rules (Rred marks a root to be reduplicated):
if a root to be reduplicated begins with a single consonant, the root copy
consists of the first three segments of the root, if it begins with a two-consonant
cluster or a vowel, the root copy consists of the first two segments of the root, i.e.
(1) 23 a. C1V1C2(…) ⇒ C1V1C2(…)=C1V1C2 / #_ and C1V1C2 ∈ Rred
b. C1C2... ⇒ C1C2...=C1C2 / #_ and C1C2 ∈ Rred
c. V1C1... ⇒ V1C1...=V1C1 / #_ and V1C1 ∈ Rred
Examples: kaltøkal (< {kaltired}) ‘beetle’, kãmNykum 24 (< kumN=kum
< {kumNred}) ‘voice’, HasãHas (< {Hasured}) ‘pink salmon’, wirãwir (< {wirured})
‘(kind of) salmon’, HÔlyHal (< HAly=HAl < {HAlyred}) ‘axe’, nykønyk (< nki=nk <
{nkired}) ‘night’, @anã@an (< anu=an < {anured}) ‘spring’, @ãjpy@uj (< ujp=uj <
{ujpred}) ‘prize’.
23
Let us be reminded once again that the rules are given here in the order they should apply
to the underlying representation.
24
After the described operation, some other modifications take place (for instance, the
segments y and @ are epenthesized).
17
1.3.2. Deletions
The following types of deletions occur in Alutor:
1) initial consonant deletion;
2) final vowel deletion;
3) hiatus vowel deletion.
The first two processes are found only in particular restricted groups of roots
and affixes, so they are partly morphologically defined (the corresponding roots and
affixes are marked in the Dictionary). But since these types of deletion play an
important role in creating the canonical syllable structure, we find it necessary to
describe them together with phonologically defined processes.
25
The fact that a root-initial consonant can be deleted in composites shows that a non-initial
root tends to be similar to the beginning of a word.
18
when there is no overt affix 26 following such morphemes. The vowel to be deleted is
always a post-stressed one. In most cases it is the vowel a that undergoes the
transformation, but some roots truncate their final i or u. The process is partly
morphologically defined, because morphemes truncating their final vowels form a
restricted group of the lexicon. In the Dictionary the final vowel of such roots (and
suffixes – see below) is shown in round brackets, cf. tôtul (the root {tatul(a)}) ‘fox’.
Some roots ending in a vowel do not truncate their final vowel, cf. pãnta (the root
{punta}) ‘liver’, @inøri (the root {iniri}) ‘blanket’.
The deletion is also attested for some suffixes, for example, the possessive
suffix {-in(a)} and other adjectival suffixes that historically originated from the
former, such as {-kin(a)}, {-lin(a)}, {-qin(a)}. The deletion of a root-final vowel
may also occur in a composite, so in this respect the vowel deletion is similar to the
consonant deletion.
The rules regulating final vowel deletion run as follows:
if a morpheme is marked for vowel deletion, it loses its final vowel in word-final
position; the deletion may also occur when the morpheme appears as a non-final part
of a composite, i.e.
(3)a. V1 ⇒ ∧ / _# and V1∈ Mvowel deletion
b. V1 ⇒ ∧ / _= and V1∈ Mvowel deletion (not regularly)
Examples: tôtul (< {tatul(a)}) ‘fox’, cf. tatãla-wwi ‘foxes’; @ømyt (< imt <
{imt(i)}) ‘burden’, cf. @imtø-ta ‘by a burden’; @yllôNi (< yllaNj < {yllaNj(u)})
‘younger brother’, cf. @yllôNju-ta ‘by a younger brother’; @aNqôkin (< {aNqa-
kin(a)}) ‘sea, marine (sg)’, cf. @aNqô-kina-wwi ‘sea, marine (pl)’; @asgønHylJu (<
asgin=HylJu < {asgi-in(a)=(t)Hylw}) ‘today’.
26
In the section “Morphology” (see Chapter 2) the final vowel deletion is treated as a
morphological means of forming the nominative singular, but in the section “Phonology” we
prefer to describe the vowel deletion as a phonological process.
19
boundary 27 , for instance, gøllin (< {ga-it-lin}) ‘(s)he/it was’.
NOTE. At first sight, the distribution of these two processes seems to be rather simple: one
is found at a composite boundary, the other – at an ordinary morpheme boundary. The problem is
to define what particular type of boundary appears in a word with two or more roots, that is in a
composite. Actually, we distinguish three types of composites: reduplications, incorporations and
compounds.
Some words appear with an epenthesized glottal stop between the vowels. Such situation is
typical of incorporations and reduplications, cf. gymík@unJunJuN (< {gymyk=unJunJu-N}) ‘to my
son’, @alô@al (< ala=al < {alared}) ‘summer’. Other words demonstrate vowel deletion. The latter
is typical of non-composites, cf. gøvlin *(< {ga-iv-lin(a)}) ‘(s)he said’. We regard these two
means of transforming hiatus as marking different degrees of coalescence between the parts of a
word: it is less so in case of epenthesis, and is more so in case of vowel deletion.
As for compounds, some of them epenthesize a glottal stop, while others delete a vowel, cf.
sasi=@ilNatgyrNyn ‘watchstrap’, but Haqømluk (< {Haqa-iml-u-k}) ‘drink vodka (=drink bad
water)’. We think that the difference between the two types of compounds lies in the degree of
coalescence, so compounds of the first group should be treated as productive compounds, while
compounds of the second group should be treated as non-productive ones (at least from the point
of phonology). We mark the difference in morphological representations of compounds by using
two different boundaries: in compounds like sasi=@ilNatgyrNyn (as well as in incorporations and
reduplications) we indicate a composite boundary, while in compounds like Haq-imluk we
indicate an ordinary morpheme boundary. When phonological rules apply, the type of boundary is
considered to has been already assigned to the underlying representation.
27
Actually, not any vowel may appear morpheme-initially or morpheme-finally, so the list
of possible combinations of vowels is considerably restricted.
28
When we use the sign “ = ” with blank spaces on both sides in the context of the rules, it
means equality, not a composite boundary (the latter is used without any spaces).
20
um-ka}) ‘it is warm’, @inómyk (< {ina-emy-k}) ‘scoop’, gôllyga (< {ga-yllyg-a})
‘with a father’, @armôvyk (< {arma-av-k}) ‘become a leader’, qãrin (< {qura-in})
‘reindeer's’, tykØmavyk (< {t-kImy-av-k}) ‘detain’, kônsuk (< {kansa-u-k}) ‘smoke’,
tysøguvyk (< {t-sigu-av-k}) ‘burn (smth.)’, &mitinak (< {miti-ynak}) ‘Miti (Esg)’,
@akmøtyk (< {akmi-at-k}) ‘take’, mytønuk (< {mtini-u-k}) ‘eat berries’, @imtønaN (<
{imti-inaN}) ‘burden’, kaNãnaN (< {kaNu-inaN}) ‘fishhook’, tytkíplJuvyk (< {t-
(t)kypl-u-av-k}) ‘kill (smb.) by striking on one's head’, @alJlJítkunaN (< {ally-tku-
inaN}) ‘scraper’.
NOTE. The rule given above has some exceptions, however. First, causatives derived from
the roots {iw@isi} ‘drink’ and {aw(y)ji} ‘eat’ appear as tiw@øsatyk ‘give water to drink’ and
tôwyjatyk ‘feed’ (instead of *tiw@isityk and *tawyjityk). It is probable that the vowel i in these
roots originated from y under the influence of the palatals s and j. Second, some roots preserve the
root-final a, for example, nutôgitanaN (< {nuta-gita-inaN}) ‘binoculars’. The exceptions can be
found in the Dictionary.
1.3.3. Insertions
The following types of insertions occur in Alutor:
1) glottal stop insertion;
2) schwa insertion.
In order to formulate the schwa insertion rules we need two additional rules
which describe marking irrelevant morpheme boundaries and metathesis of a schwa
and a consonant.
21
or not); a schwa breaks up a word-medial three-consonant cluster at a morpheme
boundary of any type, i.e.
(6)a. ∧ ⇒ y / #С1_(-)С2
b. ∧ ⇒ y / C1 _(-)С2#
c. ∧ ⇒ y / C1 _-C2C3 or C1 _=C2C3
d. ∧ ⇒ y / C1C2 _-C3 or C1C2 _=C3
Examples: typôNa 29 (< {tpa-Na}) ‘stone hammer’, nymôlqin (< {n-mal-
qin(a)}) ‘good’, môqym (< {maqm(i)}) ‘arrow’, tôtyk (< {tat-k}) ‘bring’, jatítkyn (<
{jat-tkyn}) ‘(s)he is coming’, jagísvinin (< {jag=svi-ni-n}) ‘(s)he cut his/her foot’,
nørvyqin (< {n-irv-qin(a)}) ‘sharp’, @ønmy@in (< @inm=@in < inm=in < {inm=in})
‘gulp’; nôlgyHytvu (< {nalg=Hytv-w}) ‘canoe’.
Now let us comment on these rules. In the case where there is a one-consonant
morpheme in a three-consonant cluster, namely -С2-, the schwa is inserted at only one
of the boundaries, to the left or the right of this morpheme (therefore, the string
C1C2C3 does not change into C1yC2yC3). The boundary that does not count here is
referred to as “irrelevant” one. In the rules given above irrelevant boundaries are
considered null boundaries, cf. mytíntakravna (< {myt-n[-]takr-av-na}) ‘we took
them down’, where [-] is an irrelevant boundary.
The following one-consonant morphemes are found in Alutor which may
appear word-medially: 1) @- − the conjunctive prefix; 2) n- − the medial variant of the
causative marker; 3) -N − the suffixal part of some circumfixes − the potential marker,
the desiderative marker and also the derivative marker meaning ‘create (smth.)’.
When any of these morphemes appears as С2 in the string C1-С2-C3, the following
rules apply to reveal which of the boundaries should be treated as irrelevant:
if there is a non-final string of segments C1-С2-C3, the left boundary of the
medial consonant is considered irrelevant in case it is the prefix @-, and the right
boundary in case it is the prefix n-; in case the medial consonant is the suffix -N, the
left boundary is considered irrelevant, except when the suffix -N is followed by the
morph -ki (the marker of the infinitive, the locative or of a converb), in which case
the right boundary is considered irrelevant 30 , i.e.
29
In the surface representation we attach an inserted y to the preceding morph, cf. jôty-k
*(< {jat-k}) ‘come’.
30
This additional rule can be interpreted in the following way. As the glottal stop is avoided
in syllable-final position (in this position y@ => A, see 1.3.6), and the fact that the morphs N and ki
appear close to each other can be explained historically (here ki comes from kyj, as the Kerek data
show [Skorik 1968]), the principle of irrelevancy can be formulated as follows: the irrelevant
boundary is a boundary between a root and a one-consonant affix, i.e. if possible they are not
broken up by a schwa.
22
A. 31 a. C1-C2-C3 ⇒ C1[-]C2-C3 / V1_V2 and C2 = Pref @
b. C1-C2-C3 ⇒ C1-C2[-]C3 / V1_V2 and C2 = Pref n
c. C1-C2-C3 ⇒ C1[-]C2-C3 / V1_V2 and C2 = Suf N and C3V2 ≠ Suf ki
d. C1-C2-C3 ⇒ C1-C2[-]C3 / V1_V2 and C2 = Suf N and C3V2 = Suf ki
Examples: mín@ypi1alatkyn (< {myn[-]@-pila-la-tk-n}) ‘we would leave
him/her/it’; mytíntakravna (< {myt-n[-]takr-av-na}) ‘we would get him/her/it
down’; @atôn@alNyka (< {a-ta-n@al[-]N-ka}) ‘(s)he does not want to become’, cf.
tan@ôlyNki (< {ta-n@al-N[-]ki}) ‘want to become’.
Evidently, the rules marking irrelevant boundaries precede the schwa insertion
rules, namely rules (6c), (6d).
31
As this rule describes only the boundary marking, not a phonological process, it is
numbered with a capital letter.
32
Metathesis of this type is attested not only for dentalveolars, but also for some other types
of consonants. For example, the same change occurs when one of the consonants is the causative
marker − the prefix -n-, while the other consonant is not a dentalveolar, cf. myt-y-n-gyjulavyn (<
{myt-n-g(y)jul-av-n}) ‘we taught him/her’ (the root {g(y)jul}), cf. nojulqin (< ny-gjul-qin < {n-
g(y)jul-qin(a)}) ‘educated’. We fail to describe such examples by means of standard rules.
23
3) rules “adjusting” impermissible rhythmic patterns – the syllable addition
rules (1.3.4.3).
33
Since these rules describe only marking strings of segments, but do not changing them,
they are indicated by means of capital letters.
34
These rules form a separate group − suprasegmental rules − when compared to segment
changing rules. That is why they are numbered with Roman numerals.
24
in a disyllabic word the stress is placed on the first syllable, provided it is
heavy; if the first syllable is light, the stress shifts to the second (heavy) syllable 35 ,
i.e.
II. a. − − ⇒ – − / #_ #
b. ∪ − ⇒ ∪ – / #_ #
Examples: tôtul ‘fox’, kíttil ‘forehead’, pôNkan ‘cap’, pyHãnny (< pyHãn <
{pHun(a)}) ‘mushroom’, gyvôkki (< gyvôk < {gva-k}) ‘stick’.
in a polysyllabic word the stress is placed on the second syllable, provided it is
heavy; if the second syllable is light, the stress shifts to the first syllable, provided it is
heavy 36 , i.e.
III. a. − − ⇒ − – / #_...
b. ∪ − ⇒ ∪ – / #_...
c. − ∪ ⇒ – ∪ / #_...
Examples: qurôNa ‘reindeer’, nymôlqin ‘good’, nysíqqin ‘cold’, @ynpíqlavul
‘old man’, @atHílka ‘it is painful’; @ínpyNav ‘old woman’, nølgyqin ‘white’, nJùtyqin
‘thin’.
NOTE. The root {aw(y)ji} ‘eat’ has two morphs in the underlying representation: {awyji}
and {awji} (the latter appearing as {oji} – see 1.3.6). The morph {awyji} is used in the causative
tôwyjatyk ‘feed’, and the morph {oji} is used in the non-causative @ojik ‘eat’. Words derived from
the morph {oji} are stressed as if were derived from the morph {awyji}, cf. tùjitkyn (< {t-oji-
tkyn}) ‘I am eating’, and not *tojøtkyn.
Words that do not acquire stress according to these rules are considered
unaccentuated, cf. &najylyn ‘they gave him/her/it’, &tyjylyn ‘I gave him/her/it’ (the
accentuated root {jyl}), &tyHalJulJun ‘I rocked him/her to sleep’ (the unaccentuated
root {HalJulJu}) (some exceptions were mentioned in 1.2.2, group 4b).
35
In Alutor there are neither stressed disyllabics with the second light syllable nor stressed
disyllabics with the first light and the second open syllable.
36
Otherwise, i.e. if the first syllable is light, the word does not acquire any stress.
25
adding a vowel (the vowel i − in case the last consonant is the suffix -k or -t 37 , the
schwa − in all other cases), i.e.
(8)a. ∧ ⇒ C2y / Vstressed(-)C1_# and C2 = C1 and C1 ≠ Suf k, t
b. ∧ ⇒ C2i / Vstressed-C1_# and C2 = C1 and C1 = Suf k, t
Examples: Nôjjy (< Nôj < {Naj}) ‘mountain’, Nôlly (< Nôl < {Nal}) ‘herd’,
pyHãnny (< pyHãn < {pHun}) ‘mushroom’, pylôtty (< pylôt < {plat}) ‘headscarf’,
tynãnny (< ty-nã-n < {t-nu-n}) ‘I ate it’, qytíjjy (< qytí-j < {(l)qyt-j}) ‘(s)he went
away’; gyvôkki (< gyvô-k < {gva-k}) ‘stick’, lylôtti (< lylô-t < {l(y)la-t}) ‘two eyes’.
NOTE. The supplementary open syllable may optionally become a closed one by means of
adding a sonorant N, cf. gímmy ~ gímmyN (< gím < {gym}) ‘I’.
37
The plural marker {w/wwi} seems to have originally had the same distribution of morphs
as the markers k and t: the morph wwi is likely to appear after a stressed syllable, while w
appears in other positions, cf. pantô-wwi ‘pieces of fur from reindeer's legs’, but pylôk-u (<
pylôk-y-w < {plak-w}) ‘fur boots’; but at present the morph -wwi is used not only after a stressed
syllable, but also after any stem ending in a vowel, cf. tatãla-wwi ‘foxes’; a stem ending in a
consonant may have both -u and-uwwi, cf. myng-ãwwi ~ míng-u ‘hands’.
26
in syllable-final position vowel-and-glide combinations aj (not immediately
before j), aw (not immediately before w, v), a@ produce long vowels E, O, A
respectively 38 , i.e.
(10) a. aj ⇒ E / ( _ |# or _ |C1) and C1≠j
b. aw ⇒ O / ( _ |# or _ |C1) and C1≠w,v
c. a@ ⇒ A / _ |# or _ |C1
Examples: nymróqin (< {n-mraj-qin}) ‘right’, cf. nymrôjigym ‘right (1sg)’;
@inóvykli (< @inajvykli < {ina-jvykl-j}) ‘(s)he hit me’, cf. jyvíklynin ‘(s)he hit
him/her’; rarópyN (< {rara-jpyN}) ‘along a house’, cf. rurãjpyN ‘along a bed place’;
rarôkinegym (< rarakinEgym < {rara-kina-jgym}) ‘the one from the house (1sg)’,
cf. nymílrujgym ‘quick (1sg)’; @inôgale (< @inagalE < {ina-gala-j}) ‘(s)he passed me
by’, cf. pøNku-j ‘(s)he jumped’; lùtyk (< {lawt-k}) ‘in a head’, cf. lôwyt ‘head’;
@ojølatka (< {a-wjil-at-ka}) ‘do not cast a shadow (=do not stand in my light)!’, cf.
wyjølatyk ‘cast a shadow’, @inùjivatyk (< @inawjivatyk < {ina-gjiv-at-k}) ‘show’, cf.
gyjøvatyk ‘show (smth.)’; @ílla (< yllA < {ylla@-∅}) ‘mother’, cf. @yllô@-a ‘with a
mother’, kôla (< kalA < {kala@-∅}) ‘idol’, cf. kalô@-a ‘by an idol’.
No coalescence takes place in case of geminates jj, ww (the glottal stop does not
occur syllable-finally before @), cf. tylôjjy ‘(s)he came’, @awwôka ‘do not be
caught!’.
The coalescence of the schwa and a glide follows the rules:
in syllable-final position schwa-and-glide combinations yj (not immediately
before j), yw (not immediately before w), y@ produce vowels E/i, O/u, A respectively:
vowels E, O appear in root-initial position (and also in the suffixes -jpyN, -jt which are
treated as roots in this respect 39 ), vowels i, u − in root-final position and in (other)
suffixes (including the suffix -jpyN − optionally) 40 , i.e.
(11) a. yj ⇒ E / _ |C1 and j = root-initial and C1 ≠ j
b. yj ⇒ i / (_ |# or _ |C1) and (j = root-final or j = suffix-initial) and C1 ≠ j
c. yw ⇒ O / _ |C1 and w = root-initial and C1≠w
d. yw ⇒ u / (_ |# or _ |C1) and (w = root-final or w = suffix-initial) and C1 ≠
w
e. y@ ⇒ A / _ |C1 and C1 ≠ @
Examples: tóvyklyn (< {t-jvykl-n}) ‘I hit him/her’, cf. jyvíklynin ‘(s)he hit
him/her’; winvópyN ~ winvøpyN (< {winv-jpyN}) ‘along a road’, cf. rurã-jpyN ‘along a
38
But in post-stressed position these vowels appear as short ones (see 1.3.7).
39
These suffixes obviously originated from the roots {jp} and {jt}.
40
This process seems to be related to the rhythmic structure: the vowels i , u, a appear as
short correlates of the long vowels E, O, A in non-stressed position, cf. the unique reduplication
ewô=@iw (< {ewared}) ‘brain’.
27
bed’; @y11ôNi (< @yllaNyj < {yllaNj(u)}) ‘younger brother’, cf. @y11ôNju-ta ‘by a
younger brother’; mømi (< myjmyj < {mj=mj}) ‘ashberry’, cf. ní-mjy-qin ‘bitter’;
nymøtigym (< {n-mit-jgym}) ‘skilled (1sg)’, cf. ny-mílru-jgym ‘quick (1sg)’; jylqôti
(< {jylq-at-j}) ‘(s)he slept’, cf. pøNku-j ‘(s)he jumped’; tùnan (< {t-wna-n}) ‘I took it
away’, cf. wynôkki ‘take away’, tojãlatyn (< tywjulatyn < {t-gjul-at-n}) ‘I learnt it’,
cf. gyjãlatyk ‘learn’; &HylJu (< HylJyw < {(t)Hylw}) ‘in the daytime’, cf. &Hylwytis@atyk
‘spend all day long’; pãpu (< pywpyw < pygpyg < {pg=pg}) ‘float’, cf. pygôtyk
‘float’; mytônu (<mytany-w < {mtan-w}) ‘mosquitoes’, cf. mytøni-wwi ‘berries’;
tAjylyn (< ty@jylyn < {t-@-jyl-n}) ‘I would give him/her/it’, cf. ty@øtyk (< {t-@-it-k}) ‘I
would be’, ty@ílqytyk (< {t-@-lqyt-k}) ‘I would go’.
No coalescence takes place in case of geminates jj, ww (the glottal stop does not
occur syllable-finally before @), cf. qytíjjy ‘(s)he went away’, tíwwak ‘I stuck’.
In some rare cases the combination “the vowel i + the glide j ” 41 also coalesces.
The change follows the rule:
in syllable-final position a combination ij (not immediately before j) produces
the corresponding long vowel I, i.e.
(12) ij ⇒ I / _ |C1 and C1 ≠ j
Examples: @imtØpytkyn (< {imti-jp-tky-n}) ‘(s)he is carrying it on (his/her)
back’.
41
It would be natural to expect the coalescence uw => U, but it is unattested in Alutor (the
combination uw occurs only before w).
42
This rule may have some reference to the irregular distribution of alternates ô and y found
in the root {la@u / ly@u / s@u} ‘to see’, cf. ty-lô@u-tkyn ‘I see him/it’, but ly@ã-kki ‘to see’, @inJô-
s@u-k ‘to seek’.
28
position. It often takes place in reduplications derived from roots with long vowels.
Quantitative reduction follows the rule:
a long vowel is shortened in post-stressed position, i.e.
(14) Vlong ⇒ Vshort / Vstressed ..._
Examples: HÔlyHal (< HAly=HAl < {HAlyred}) ‘axe’, rarôtenyk (< {rara-tEny-k})
‘near a house’, rarôkinegym (< rara-kin-Egym < {rara-kina-jgym}) ‘the one from a
house (1sg)’, @í11a (< yllA < {ylla@-∅}) ‘mother’.
1.3.8. Fortitions
Fortitions are represented in Alutor by the following two phonological processes:
1) expressive vowel lengthening;
2) consonant gemination.
29
immediately before a word-final vowel, provided the preceding vowel is a stressed
one, i.e.
(16) t ⇒ tt / #CVstressed _-V2# and t ∈ Rgemination
Examples: jôtti (< jôt-i < {jat-j}) ‘(s)he came’; cf. tyjôtytkyn (< {t-jat-tkyn}) ‘I
am coming’.
Consonant gemination is obviously related to syllable addition when the
consonant is geminated too − see 1.3.8.2.
30
Examples: vitôllat (< {vit-at-la-t}) ‘they worked’, tírran (< {t-tra-n) ‘I tore
it’.
3) contact dentalveolar palatalization, namely:
at a syllable boundary a non-palatalized dentalveolar t, n, or l changes into its
palatalized correlate immediately before the corresponding palatalized
dentalalveolar s, nJ, or lJ respectively, i.e.
(19) a. t ⇒ s / _s
b. n ⇒ nJ / _nJ
c. l ⇒ lJ / _lJ
Examples: @arôssityk (< {arat-sit-k}) ‘pour’, mytônJnJaqu (< {mtan-nJaqu})
‘big mosquito’, @ystãlJlJyqyk (< ystul-lJyqy-k < {ystul-lJq-k}) ‘on a table’.
4) palatalization of the sonorant dentals n, l before the palatal j (here both
segments change), namely:
at a syllable boundary a non-palatalized dental n or l together with the
following palatal j change into the corresponding geminated palatalized dental nJnJ
or lJlJ respectively, i.e.
(20) a. nj ⇒ nJnJ
b. lj ⇒ lJlJ
Examples: tínJnJun (< ty-nju-n < {t-nju-n}) ‘I guarded him/her/it’, tilJlJøta (<
{til-jita}) ‘by a wing’.
5) complete assimilation of the nasal n before sonorant dentoalveolars, namely:
at a syllable boundary a sonorant nasal n changes into the corresponding
sonorant dentalveolar immediately before a sonorant dentalveolar l or r, i.e.
(21) a. n ⇒ l / _l
b. n ⇒ r / _r
Examples (this assimilation is attested only for the medial variant of the
causative marker -n-): tyllóvyn (< {t-n-levy-n}) ‘I took him/her/it (with me)’,
tyrrItylavyn (< {t-n-rItyla-av-n}) ‘I saw him/her/it in (my) dream’.
6) complete assimilation of the uvular q, namely:
at a syllable boundary an obstruent uvular q changes into k when it is
immediately preceded by a velar k, i.e.
(22) q ⇒ k / _k
Examples: tôkkin (< {taq-kin(a)}) ‘why’.
7) complete progressive assimilation of the labial v , namely:
at a syllable boundary an obstruent labial v changes into w immediately after a
labial w, i.e.
(23) v ⇒ w / w_
Examples: tíwwak (< ty-wva-k < {t-gva-k}) ‘I stuck’.
31
1.3.10. Irregular Distant Dental Palatalisations
Alutor alternations which we call distant palatalizations are rather peculiar from a
typological point of view. The alternation itself looks like this: in some morphemes
(mostly roots) a dental non-palatalized consonant changes into its palatalized
correlate in the context defined by another morpheme of the same word (mostly an
affix), cf. vilJãpilJ (< {vilu-pilJ}) ‘little ear’. Since some such morphemes (but not all
of them) contain a palatalized consonant and the changing segment becomes a
palatalized one, we treat this process as distant palatalization, and refer to the
corresponding morphemes as “palatalizing” ones 43 .
The process takes place in roughly the following environment:
if there is a palatalizing morpheme in a word, a non-palatalized dental t, n, or l
changes into its palatalized counterpart s (seldom tJ), nJ, or lJ (in some cases s)
respectively, i.e.
(24) a. t ⇒ s (seldom tJ ) / ∃Mpalatalizing
b. n ⇒ nJ / ∃Mpalatalizing
c. l ⇒ lJ (in some cases s) / ∃Mpalatalizing.
Examples: gasvôs@atylqivlin (< {ga-(t)va-s@at-lqiv-lin(a)}) ‘(s)he was
sitting’, anJãs@atyk (< {anu-s@at-k}) ‘spend spring’, alJôs@atyk (< {ala-s@at-k})
‘spend summer’, nJyvøssykuk 44 (< {nvil-tku-k}) ‘stop (repeatedly)’, lJóvytkuk (<
{levy-tku-k}) ‘walk’, mysôtkuk (< {mla-tku-k}) ‘break’.
But in reality the distribution of the alternants and the context they appear in
turn out to be more complicated than it is shown in the above-mentioned rule. First,
palatalizing morphemes may not contain a palatalized consonant 45 , cf. HønJnJykvyn (<
{Hinn-kv-n}) ‘scarf’ ({kv} is a palatalizing suffix). Second, palatalizing affixes may
cause palatalization in one root and not affect another root, cf. lJóvytkuk (< {levy-tku-
k}) ‘walk’, but @ôngytytkuk (< {angyt-tku-k}) ‘celebrate’ ({tku} is a palatalizing
suffix). Third, it is difficult to find a strict distribution between the palatalized
consonants lJ and s (actually, we think that the alternation l/s is more ancient, while
l/lJ is an innovation). Fourth, there exist not only palatalizing affixes, but also
palatalizing roots, for palatalization appears in some affixes, cf. nJãmqin (< {n-um-
qin(a)}) ‘warm’ ({um} should be treated as palatalizing root), but nurôqin ‘distant’.
And finally, there are duplicate affixes with the alternation l/s, which slightly differ
43
A.N.Zhukova [Zhukova 1972] refers to similar alternations in the Koryak language as
“consonant harmony”.
44
When the dental of a root is palatalized, the dental of an affix is usually palatalized too.
45
At the same time, if an affix has a palatalized consonant, it is not necessarily a palatalizing
one.
32
in meaning and therefore are treated on the synchronic level as different affixes, cf. {-
l@at} ‘iterative action’ − {-s@at} ‘constant action’ (see the list of affixes in the
Dictionary). All these facts show that the distant palatalization in Alutor cannot be
described in terms of formal rules, so the exact distribution of the alternants is
lexically defined (see the Dictionary).
1. Reduplications
(1)a. C1V1C2(…) ⇒ C1V1C2(…)=C1V1C2 / #_ and C1V1C2 ∈ Rred
b. C1C2... ⇒ C1C2...=C1C2 / #_ and C1C2 ∈ Rred
c. V1C1... ⇒ V1C1...=V1C1 / #_ and V1C1 ∈ Rred
2. Deletions:
1) initial consonant deletion
(2)a. C1 ⇒ ∧ / #_C2 and C1 ∈ Rconsonant deletion
b. C1 ⇒ ∧ / =_C2 and C1 ∈ Rconsonant deletion (not regularly)
2) final vowel deletion
(3)a. V1 ⇒ ∧ / _# and V1∈ Mvowel deletion
b. V1 ⇒ ∧ / _= and V1∈ Mvowel deletion (not regularly)
3) hiatus vowel deletion
(4)a. V1 ⇒ ∧ / _-V2 and V1 < V2
b. V2 ⇒ ∧ / V1-_ and (V2 < V1 or V2 = V1)
3. Insertions:
1) glottal stop insertion
(5)a. ∧ ⇒ @ / #_V
b. ∧ ⇒ @ / =_V
2) irrelevant boundary marking
A. a. C1-C2-C3 ⇒ C1[-]C2-C3 / V1_V2 and C2 = Pref @
b. C1-C2-C3 ⇒ C1-C2[-]C3 / V1_V2 and C2 = Pref n
c. C1-C2-C3 ⇒ C1[-]C2-C3 / V1_V2 and C2 = Suf N and C3V2 ≠ Suf ki
d. C1-C2-C3 ⇒ C1-C2[-]C3 / V1_V2 and C2 = Suf N and C3V2 = Suf ki
3) schwa insertion
(6)a. ∧ ⇒ y / #С1_(-)С2
b. ∧ ⇒ y / C1 _(-)С2#
c. ∧ ⇒ y / C1 _-C2C3 or C1 _=C2C3
d. ∧ ⇒ y / C1C2 _-C3 or C1C2 _=C3
33
4) schwa and consonant metathesis
(7)a. y-C2 ⇒ -C2y / C1_C3 and C1,C2 = dentalveolar (optionally)
b. y=C2 ⇒ =C2y / C1_C3 and C1,C2 = dentalveolar (optionally)
34
7. Vowel reduction
(13) a ⇒ y / _HVstressed and a ≠ Pref a
(14) Vlong ⇒ Vshort / Vstressed ..._
8. Fortitions
1) expressive vowel lengthening
(15) a. y ⇒ O / in expressive words
b. a ⇒ O / in expressive words
c. i ⇒ E / in expressive words
d. u ⇒ O / in expressive words
2) consonant gemination
(16) t ⇒ tt / #CVstressed _-V2# and t ∈ Rgemination
35
c. l ⇒ lJ (in some cases s) / ∃Mpalatalizing
36
raw=raw (rules Ba, Bb) ⇒ |raw=|raw| (rule Cc) ⇒ − − (rule IIa) ⇒ rôw=raw (rule
10b) ⇒ rÙ=rO (rule 14) ⇒ [rÙ=ro]; the practical transcription is rùro ‘ptarmigan’.
37
Example 5. {ga-it-lin(a)} ‘be (3sg res)’ (rule 3a) ⇒ ga-it-lin (rule 4a) ⇒ g-it-
lin (rules Ba, Bb) ⇒ |g-it-|lin| (rule Cc) ⇒ − − (rule IIa) ⇒ g-øt-lin (rule 18a) ⇒ [g-
øl-lin]; the practical transcription is gøllin ‘(s)he was’.
38
39
Comment: V etom fajle snjaty
CHAPTER 2. Morphology vse ispravlenija, on prakticheski
gotov r formatirovaniju.
U menja takie pros’by:
In this chapter we consider the main word classes, rich in inflectional morphology, 1. Sledit’, chtoby tablicy ne
razryvalis’, vkljuchaja ix
namely, the verb, adjective, noun, and pronoun. nazvanija, i byli na odnoj stranice.
Imejte v vidu, chto ja nemnogo
pereformatiroval tablicy, chtoby
2.1. Verbs oni luchshe chitalis’. V chastnosti
razdvinul stroki.
The verbs can be divided into two classes, f i n i t e (inflected) and n o n - f i n i t e . 2. V Table 3 sleduet
raspolozhit’affiksy odnogo tipa
Finite forms are defined as compatible with the head predicate position of an drug pod drugom, naskol’ko eto
pozvol’aet shirina tablicy. Dlja
independent clause. Non-finite forms can occupy the head predicate position of a etogo luchshe pol’zovat’sja
tabuljaciej, a ne probelami.
subordinate clause as well as be part of the analytical verb form. 3. Proverit’v primerax, chtoby
Finite forms have several conjugations depending on the number of agreement glossa byla raspolozhena pod
slovom. Eto ne vsegda bylo tak.
slots. First of all there are polypersonal and monopersonal conjugations. In
Comment:
p o l y p e r s o n a l conjugation the verb has two agreement slots: a prefix and a suffix
slot. Transitive verbs agree with the agentive and patientive arguments (referred to
below as A - a n d P - a r g u m e n t s ). Intransitive verbs agree with the sole core
argument 46 (referred to below as S - a r g u m e n t ). It is important that the S/A-
argument, i.e. the argument bearing the Principal hyperrole (see argumentation
below) is the main controller of agreement. In m o n o p e r s o n a l conjugation the verb
has only one agreement slot – the suffix slot controlled by the S/P-argument, i.e. the
argument bearing the Absolutive hyperrole. Furthermore there is a number of
i m p e r s o n a l forms that do not distinguish person values of core arguments.
First, we will consider three conjugation types of finite and non-finite verb
forms.
46
In this case both agreement slots are used, see below.
47
The distribution of the imperfective’s allomorphs (-tky /-tkyn /-tkyni-) is not quite clear.
40
The category m o o d is also obligatory. It has four values:
- i n d i c a t i v e (IND), does not have its own specific markers;
- o p t a t i v e (OPT), does not have its own specific markers 48 ;
- p o t e n t i a l (POT), circumfix marker ta-...(-N) 49 , framing the verb stem;
- c o n j u n c t i v e (CONJ), prefix marker @-/A- 50 .
Besides these, in Alutor there is also an impersonal form of the i m p e r a t i v e
(see 2.1.3).
Finally, the finite verb agrees 51 in person and number with its core arguments,
distinguishing three persons (1st/2nd/3rd person) and three numbers — s i n g u l a r
(sg), d u a l (du) and p l u r a l (pl) 52 . Intransitive verbs agree with their sole core
argument (S - a r g u m e n t ) marked with the nominative, and have, aspect and mood
being fixed, 9 person-number forms. The transitive verb agrees simultaneously with
the agentive argument (A - a r g u m e n t ) marked with the ergative as well as with the
patientive argument (P - a r g u m e n t 53 ) marked with the nominative, and has, aspect
48
The mood value is coded cumulatively, together with person-number values, see below.
49
The suffix component of the circumfix can be covert, for example, in imperfective
potential. The potential marker coincides with desiderative marker (see 2.1.3 below).
50
The marker A- is a phonetic variant of the marker @- in specific positions (see 1.3.6).
51
In Alutor the person-number markers are combined with the unmarked usage of personal
pronouns in the core argument positions, so it is possible to interpret them as agreement markers.
52
As the paradigms show, semantically this tripartite personal opposition has its own
internal structure. The opposition ‘singular — non-singular’ (sg — nsg) consists of six deictic
items. Elementary single entities are defined in terms of speech act participants (1sg = ‘speaker’,
2sg = ‘hearer’) and non-participants of the speech act (3sg = ‘somebody’). Non-singular entities
are totalities of elementary entities: 1nsg = ‘speaker and hearer/somebody’, 2nsg = ‘hearer and
somebody’, 3nsg = ‘somebody and somebody’ (i.e. the countable number opposition of
homogenous entities is present only in the 3rd person). Exactly these entities are ordered in the
Deictic Hierarchy (see Figure 1 below). The arrangement of non-singular entities with respect to
singular entities is semantically motivated. Thus, ‘1nsg’ includes, besides the speaker, the hearer
and/or the 3rd person. Such an entity is lower on the Hierarchy than ‘1sg’. Simultaneously this
entity is higher than ‘2sg’, because it includes the speaker, and this factor is most important for
the Hierarchy. The motivation for ‘2nsg’ is similar.
Within the scope of the non-singular there is a proper number opposition of dual and plural
(du and pl), because the number of homogenous elementary entities increases, and this opposition
uses iconically specific coding devices (see 2.1.1.2.4). Dual does not have a surface expression (it
is possible to postulate the zero marker), and plural is expressed by a specific morpheme —
pluralizer -la.
In that way the prototypical number opposition is represented in the subsystem of 1/2nd
person nsg, and partially in 3rd person sg/nsg.
53
See 3.1.6 on agreement with the addressee argument.
41
and mood being fixed, 63 person-number forms.
The conjugation of the intransitive verb ‘jump’ and the transitive verb ‘beat’ are
presented in Appendix. The linear structure of the finite verb form is presented in
Table 1. The nucleus of the form is its verb stem. It has the zero rank. Morpheme
slots that precede the stem (prefixes) have negative rank numbers increasing from
right to left. The slots following the stem (suffixes) have positive rank numbers
increasing from left to right.
-4 -3 -2 -1 0 +1 +2 +3 +4
S/A mood inversion incorpo- stem plura- aspect/ A+P S/P
agree- ration lizer agree- agree-
mood
ment ment ment
COMMENTS on Table 1
1. The leftmost slot (rank -4) is occupied by the a g r e e m e n t m a r k e r s of S/A-
arguments, see 2.1.1.2 as well as Table 5. The rightmost slot (rank +4) is occupied by agreement
markers of the S/P-arguments, see Table 6. Additionally, these agreement slots can be occupied
by n o n - t r a n s p a r e n t agreement markers 54 .
2. Prefixal mood markers (the conjunctive marker @- and the potential marker ta-)
occupy rank -3. The suffix component of the potential mood marker (-N), as well as the
imperfective aspect marker (-tkyn) have the rank +2.
3. The pluralizer -la occupies rank +1, immediately following the stem.
4. The slot following the prefixalal mood marker (rank -2) is occupied by the specific
i n v e r s i o n marker ina- (see below, Comment 1 on Table 8). Rank +3 is intended for
portmanteau agreement markers (-ni, -gy, see 2.1.1.2.4 below).
5. Finally, the stem of an incorporated word can be placed in immediate preposition to the
stem, rank -1 (on incorporation see 3.1.4).
6. Any affix slot can be empty. However there is no verb form coinciding with the bare
stem, i.e. at least one of the eight slots should be filled.
54
The non-transparent marker na-/nan-/yn- ‘LOW.A’ occupies the prefix slot with rank
-4, see 2.1.1.2.4, and the marker -tki ‘2nsg.A+3.P’ occupies the suffix slot with rank +4, see
2.1.1.2.3.
42
agreement forms), but rather the less than trivial rules of a g r e e m e n t m a r k e r
selection. Namely, only a portion of the inventory of agreement markers is
t r a n s p a r e n t in the sense that they directly correspond to the person-number value
of the corresponding argument. In many slots of the verb paradigm there are
deviations from this ideal semiotic pattern (the so called n o n - t r a n s p a r e n t
markers). This phenomenon should be given particular consideration.
55
A hyphen follows the prefix markers and precedes the suffix markers. The absence of the
material or zero marker in an expected agreement slot means that this slot is absent in this box of
the paradigm.
43
TABLE 3. Schematic person-number paradigm of transitive verb (POT, PF)
P A
1sg 1du 1pl 2sg 2du 2pl
1sg ina- ina- -tyk ina- -la -tyk
1du na- -myk na- -myk na- -myk
1pl na- -la -myk na- -la -myk na- -la -myk
2sg t- -gyt myt- - myt- -la-gyt
gyt
2du t- myt- -∅ - myt-
tyk
-tyk -∅ -tyk
2pl t- -la-tyk myt- -la- myt- -la-tyk
tyk
3sg t- -n myt- -n myt- -la-n ∅- -n -tki -la -tki
3du t- -na-t myt- -na- myt- -na-t ∅- -na-t -tki -tki
t
3pl t- -na-w myt- -na- myt- -na-w ∅- -na-w -la -tki -la -tki
w
P A
3sg 3du 3pl
1sg ina- -∅ na- -gym na- -gym
1du na- -myk na- -myk na- -myk
1pl na- -la -myk na- -la -myk na- -la -myk
2sg na- -gyt na- -gyt na- -gyt
2du na- -tyk na- -tyk na- -tyk
2pl na- -la -tyk na- -la -tyk na- -la -tyk
3sg -ni-n na- -n na- -n
3du -ni-na-t na- -na-t na- -na-t
3pl -ni-na-w na- -na-w na- -na-w
44
In the prefix and suffix agreement slots with ranks -4 and +4 two number values
for 1st/2nd person arguments — singular (sg) and non-singular (nsg), are
distinguished, and the opposition of dual and plural is expressed by a specific
morpheme, the pluralizer -la, occupying the nearest suffix slot after the stem.
In Table 3, the regular font indicates transparent person-number agreement
markers that have single person-number values for particular A/P-arguments, while
the boldface indicates non-transparent agreement markers that do not have a direct
correlation with the person-number values of their respective arguments.
In Table 4 the transparent person-number agreement markers in perfective
potential are presented (naturally, without the pluralizer -la).
COMMENTS on Table 4
1. As the Table shows, prefixalal S- and A-agreement markers are the same (they are
presented in the first line of the Table). This slot can be occupied by material markers of the 1st
person, as well as the zero marker 56 of the 2nd person sg. Other persons do not have prefixal
agreement markers 57 .
2. The suffix slot is occupied by person-number S- and P-markers (the second and third lines
of the Table respectively). Suffixal person-number S-markers correspond to 2nd person non-sg
and 3rd person, i.e. person-number S-markers are in complementary distribution among the prefix
and suffix slots: S-arguments ‘I’, ‘we’, ‘you (sg)’ have prefixal agreement markers, while ‘you
(non-sg)’, ‘(s)he/it’, ‘they’ have suffix markers. In contrast to S-arguments, all persons of P-
arguments have corresponding person-number P-markers 58 , material forms of S- and P-markers
being different (the single exception is the marker -tyk ‘2nsg.S/P’).
3. 3rd person P-markers consist of the -n(a) person marker and a number marker. In the
56
This description differs forms with zero marker and forms that lack any agreement marker.
Practically for particular boxes of the paradigm the descriptive choice depends on systematic
considerations.
57
Here and below lack of an agreement marker is indicated by the dash.
58
The ‘1sg.P’ marker -gym has a restricted usage; see below.
45
singular, the final vowel of the person marker undergoes syncope59 , in the dual the marker -t is
added, in the plural the markers -w or -wwi are added.
Thus, A- and P-markers of transitive verbs are in complementary distribution
between prefix/suffix slots. The S-markers of intransitive verbs are represented in
both slots, the choice depending on the person value. The S-arguments ‘I’, ‘we’, ‘you
(sg)’ are similar (from the point of view of the slot type and the material form) to the
respective A-arguments of transitive verbs, while ‘you (pl)’, ‘(s)he/it’, ‘they’ are
likened (from the point of view of the slot type) to the respective P-arguments of
transitive verbs 60 . It seems that this person differentiation is not random. It
demonstrates the opposition between speech-act participants (l o c u t o r s ) and
participants that are external to the speech act (n o n - l o c u t o r s ). The boundary
between locutors and non-locutors is inside the 2nd person rather than between 2nd
and 3rd person, as is usually assumed. In Alutor the hearer (‘you (sg)’) is a speech-act
participant, while 2nd person nsg (‘you (pl)’) includes, beside the hearer, other
persons, and this is the reason to interpret this totality as a non-locutor. As shown
below, the agreement paradigm testifies, based on cross-linguistically relevant (see
[Kibrik 1997b) opposition between locutors and non-locutors, to the existence of the Comment: Ssylka na russkuju
stat’ju, sm. Bibliography, gde est’
multi-member D e i c t i c H i e r a r c h y in Alutor, see Figure 1. Kibrik 1997a: “Beyond...”. V
Bibl. nado postavit’ indeksy “a” i
“b”]
FIGURE 1. The Deictic Hierarchy
1sg > 1nsg > 2sg > 2nsg > 3sg > 3nsg
locutors > non-locutors
59
This means of coding the singular is used also for the nominative singular of some nouns,
see 2.3.1.1, as well as for number inflection of adjectives (-qin(a), -kin(a) and others), see 2.2.
60
These similarities have independent manifestation in the effects of markedness reversal,
see below in 2.1.1.2.3.
46
suffixal S-markers are combined with values of mood as well as aspect. The only
pure agreement marker is the suffix marker -tyk ‘2nsg’ 61 .
Tables 5-6 show a formal correspondence between combined markers and their
grammatical values. However, from the point of view of an explanatory description
such a formal correspondence is absolutely insufficient. It is natural to suppose that
the means of merging the values of person, mood, and aspect into single markers are
not random, they are motivated in many cases by the nature of these values
themselves. Some assumptions of this kind are presented in the Comments on the
tables, though a full explanatory description is the goal of further investigations.
COMMENTS on Table 5
1. S/A-markers consistently merge the indicative and the potential as bjective moods.
The use of these moods presupposes the existence of corresponding events. The indicative mood
characterizes the event as existing in a real world, and the potential mood, as existing in a virtual
world. The conjunctive and the optative are unified by the idea of esirable modality: they
indicate the events of the world of a speaker's desires (see the semantics of mood in 2.1.7.1 —
2.1.7.4).
2. In unmarked bjective moods (IND and POT) only locutors have the prefixal
agreement markers, and these markers can be considered pure. In esirable moods the mood
and deictic values are combined 62 , therefore non-locutors also have prefix markers.
3. 2nd and 3rd person conjunctives (CONJ) have the single marker n-, indicating a S/A-
participant, whom the speaker does not control. The choice of the same marker for 3rd person
optative (OPT) is not random.
4. 2nd person optative has a specific marker q-, which is differentiated from the 3rd person.
61
It is remarkable that only this agreement marker unifies S- and P-markers, cf. Tables 6-7.
62
The single deviation is conjunctive for 1sg.S/A (t-), coinciding with the markers of
“objective” moods (see Comment 5 to Table 5).
47
The 2nd person optative semantically coincides with the imperative 63 , because exactly in this
speech situation the speaker perlocutively affects the addressee with the speech act.
5. The 1nsg value does not differentiate between the conjunctive and the optative: these
moods have the single myn- agreement marker 64 , in contrast to 1sg. In the context of the
conjunctive 1sg uses the t- marker, identical to the one used in the unmarked moods, and such a
merger takes place only for this person. Probably the maximal degree of control on the part of the
speaker over the desirable event is important: inasmuch as the speaker has an S/A-role in the
event, the probability of its accomplishment is estimated by the speaker to be very close to real
event.
6. A unique 1sg marker is present in the optative (m-). In this mood the speaker is
simultaneously the sender as well as the recipient of the prescription. It should be taken into
account that this mood does not have its own marker and differs from the indicative only by
means of agreement markers, i.e. the speech acts with different S/A-arguments are cognitively
different. The following types of desires are distinguished: a desire addressed to the speaker
himself (m-), addressed to the speaker and other persons (myn-), addressed to the hearer and,
optionally, to other persons (n-) (in the latter case the addressee of desire being virtual rather than
actual).
63
Usually in Chukotko-Kamchatkan languages the mood under consideration is called
imperative, see [Zhukova 1968] (Alutor), [Zhukova 1972] (Koryak), [Volodin 1992] (Kerek),
[Skorik 1977] (Chukchi). However the semantics of 1st and 3rd persons’ S/A-arguments does not
have a perlocutive component of inducement to the action (see the separate analysis of the
meanings of these forms in [Muravyova 1990a]). It should be mentioned that Alutor rarely uses
the imperative proper, see 2.1.3.
64
At that the forms of conjunctive and optative in general are distinct.
48
Aspect, mood 1sg 1nsg: 2sg 2nsg 3sg 3du 3pl
du/pl
PF+POT -_ ~ -i -_ -_ ~ -i -tyk -∅ ~ -i -t -t
PF+IND -k -myk -j -tyk -j -gy@yt -t
PF+CONJ -k -myk -n -tyk -n -na-t -na-w
PF+OPT -k -myk -gi -tyk -n -na-t -na-w
IPF+POT -_ -_ /-t -_ -tyk -∅ -t -t
IPF+IND -_ -_ /-t -_ -tyk -∅ -t -t
IPF+CONJ -_ -_ /-t _ -tyk -n -na-t -na-w
IPF+OPT -_ -_ /-t _ -tyk -n -na-t -na-w
COMMENTS on Table 6
1. As mentioned above, the ‘2nsg.S’ marker, -tyk, is largely independent from aspect/mood
values, and also identical to the ‘2nsg.P’ marker (see Table 7).
2. In contrast to the imperfective 65 , the perfective tends to be combined with specific locutor
suffixal markers (absent only in the perfective potential 66 ). 1st person agreement markers are
identical for the indicative, conjunctive, and optative. In these moods the singular is -k ‘1sg.S+PF’
marker, non-singular is -myk ‘1nsg.S+PF’ (identical to the marker ‘1nsg.P’). As with the 2nd
person singular, its markers are also combined with mood values: -j ‘2sg+PF+IND’, -n
‘2sg+PF+CONJ’, -gi ‘2sg+PF+OPT’.
3. Usually in the context of perfective potential (with the final component -N) and the
singular S-arguments the suffixal agreement markers are absent (see Appendix), however
sporadically the marker -i (the same for all persons 67 ) appears, thus it can be given a value of
‘sg.S+PF+POT’.
4. It should be mentioned that an interpretation of the empty suffix slot for ‘2sg.S+PF.POT’
is complicated. The proposed interpretation (the lack of markers) is motivated by the locutor
status of ‘2sg’, merging with the 1st person. However another interpretation is also possible, that
is, the existence of a zero marker. The following generalizations favor this interpretation: if it is
true, then (a) the tendency for the 2nd and 3rd persons to merge into singular markers becomes
stronger, (b) 2nd person singular in the perfective always has a suffix marker.
5. The 3rd person has its own subsystem, for clarity presented in Table 6a. First, 3rd person
65
In this aspect locutors do not have overt suffix markers, see Comment 7 below about
marker -t ‘1pl.S’ in imperfective aspect.
66
See, however, Comment 3 below.
67
See, for example, [20:13]: gymmy ty-ta-r.ety-N-i ‘I will return home’, [9:25]: t-ina-n.u-
N-i, t-ina-toli-N-i ‘(you) are going to roast me and eat me’, [6:23]: ta-vusqy.svi-N-i ‘when it
gets dark’.
49
differentiates all three number's values 68 , second, it contrasts bjective and esirable moods.
In esirable moods the values of person (-n/-na marker) and number ([syncope of the final -a]
‘sg’, -t ‘du’, -w ‘pl’) are split, see Table 7 and Comment 3 on Table 4. In the bjective moods,
excluding the perfective indicative, the singular (-∅) and non-singular (-t marker) are
differentiated.
6. The perfective indicative, i.e. the most unmarked aspect-mood form, occurs, on the
contrary, in the most peculiar way, in the context of 3rd person S-argument. 3rd person singular
has the marker -j ‘3sg+PF+IND’, identical to the ‘2sg+PF+IND’ marker. However, it is most
remarkable that 3rd person dual (‘they two’) has a unique marker -gy@yt ‘3du+PF+IND’. 3rd
person plural is not combined by aspect values; the marker -t is combined with mood only and is
identical in the indicative and potential irrespective of aspect.
7. The existence of the single locutor suffix -t ‘1pl.S’ marker in imperfective, identical to the
‘3nsg.S’ marker for POT and IND, suggests an idea that ‘we many’ includes third persons in the
set of participants (‘we many’ = ‘I+they’), i.e. plurality iconically marks 3rd person rather than 1st
person. This interpretation being accepted the suffixal locutor S-markers are present only in the
perfective aspect with the exception of the potential mood.
8. After stems with a single final consonant the 3nsg S-marker -na- sometimes has the
allomorph -yna.
COMMENTS on Table 7
1. In general P-markers are context-free. The only exception is the usage of -tyk as ‘2.P’ in
the context of OPT/CONJ and 1nsg A-argument. It should be mentioned that in this case (‘we
68
Motivation of this property of the 3rd person is discussed in the footnote 95.
50
<A> — you <P>’) ‘we’ has an exclusive value. Nevertheless the motivation of this P-argument
number neutralization in this context needs additional explanation.
2. Non-locutor P-markers are identical to S-markers in OPT/CONJ (see Table 6a).
3. The agreement marker -gym ‘1sg.P’, presented in square brackets has restricted usage. It
is used only in the context of the 3nsg A-argument (see below).
4. It should be mentioned that -gym ‘1sg.P’ and -gyt ‘2sg.P’ are closely connected with the
agreement markers of the adjective conjugation (see 2.2). They are enclitic forms of respective
personal pronouns ‘I’ and ‘you (sg)’.
A P Agreement markers
2nsg 3 -tki
3sg 3 -ni
3nsg 1/2/3 na-
3sg 2 na-
3sg/2 1nsg na-
3sg/2 1sg ina-
51
generally harmonizes with the A-role, and the rightmost member with the P-role.
Therefore the A-role is mostly unmarked for the ‘I’-argument, and the P-role for the
‘they’-argument, and vice versa, the A-role is generally marked for the ‘they’-
argument, and P-role for the ‘I’-argument. With respect to the intermediate positions
of the Deictic Hierarchy, markedness of the A-role increases in the Hierarchy from
left to right, whereas markedness of the P-role increases from right to left. The critical
point of this Hierarchy is the boundary between locutors and non-locutors. It is
normal for locutors to play the A-role 69 , whereas it is normal for non-locutors to play
the P-role. The following person-number combinations of A- and P-roles are
congruent with this tendency 70 :
A P
I — you sg/you nsg/him/them
we — you sg/you nsg/him/them
you sg — him/them
It is easy to see that these combinations of deictic categories trigger the regular
(pure and combined) agreement markers. Deviations from the regular coding are
found in the context of the following person-number A- & P-combinations:
A P
you sg — me/us
you nsg — me/us /him/them
he — me/us /you sg/you nsg/him/them
they — me/us /you sg/you nsg/him/them
69
Moreover, locutor can play the P-role in case when the A-role is also occupied by locutor
outranking the former in the Deictic Hierarchy.
70
English pronominal constructions are used for the sake of clarity.
52
(b) if the A-argument is outranked by the P-argument on the Deictic Hierarchy, the non-
transparent marker is needed (it is important that it is the same na- marker).
(c) If the non-locutor A-argument is deictically not lower than the P-argument 71 , the special
portmanteau non-transparent markers are used, whose choice depends on the deictic properties of
the A-argument (-tki ‘you nsg — him/them’; -ni ‘he — him/them’).
The P-marker also can be the starting point. It takes place in the following case:
(d) the most deictically unnatural, i.e. the most marked P-argument (= ‘I’)
needs the non-transparent marker (ina-).
COMMENTS on Table 8
1. It is necessary to emphasize the essential distinction between na- and ina- markers. na-
71
In this case the P-argument is also non-locutor.
53
occupies the regular prefix slot for A-markers (rank -4). ina- has a specific prestem slot (rank
-1), it is controlled by the P-argument and is identical to the antipassive marker, i.e. it is an
i n v e r s i o n marker. It is also essential that in the context of this agreement marker the A-
argument is marked by suffixal agreement markers identical to S-markers 72 . For examples see the
perfective forms of the verb ‘beat’ below:
72
B.Comrie calls these forms pseudo-intransitive [Comrie 1980].
73
This is also supported by the co-occurence of this marker with the non-transparent marker
–gy; see below.
54
TABLE 9. Schematic paradigm with the non-transparent marker -gy in the optative
A- P-argument
argument 3sg 3du 3pl
IPF+2sg q- -tky -n q- -tky -na-t q- -tky -na-w
IPF+2du q- -tkyni -gy-tki q- -tkyni -gy-tki q- -la-tkyni -gy-tki
IPF+2pl q- -la-tkyni -gy-tki q- -tkyni -gy-tki q- -la-tkyni -gy-tki
PF+2sg q- -gy-n q- -gy-na-t q- -gy-na-w
PF+2du q- -gy-tki q- -gy-tki q- -la -gy-tki
PF+2pl q- -la -gy-tki q- -gy-tki q- -la -gy-tki
74
It should be taken into consideration that these forms are very rare in the speech. In texts
(Part I) only one occurence of this marker in the optative imperfective was found. There are also
some divergences between the paradigm presented in this volume and the paradigm in [Zhukova
1968: 304-05]: in the latter paradigm there are forms q-.....-tkyni-gy-na-t ‘2sg.A+OPT-....-IPF-
2.A+3-3.P-du’, q-.....-tkyni-gy-na-w ‘2sg.A+OPT-....-IPF-2.A+3-3.P-pl’.
75
See Comment 7 on Table 6 about “exceptional” marker -t ‘1pl.S+IPF’.
76
See also [Mel’čuk 1986].
55
— 1st/2nd person S/A/P-arguments;
— 3rd person S-arguments in “objective” moods — indicative and potential 77 ;
— 3rd person P-arguments in the context of the non-transparent marker -tki 78 .
As one can see, there are accessible arguments that are in the scope of
pluralization, and non-accessible arguments that are out of the scope of pluralization.
First, the dual vs. plural distinction of the 3rd person A-arguments does not have any
overt expression, including the pluralizer. Second, in the context of suffixal markers -
t ‘3du’ and -w ‘3pl’, the 3rd person S- and P-arguments are non-accessible.
Intransitive verbs have the following rule of pluralizer insertion: accessible S-
argument controls pluralizer insertion.
Pluralization of transitive verbs is more complicated: the verb has only one
pluralizer slot and two potential plural agreement controllers, — A- and P-arguments,
if accessible. Plural values are possible in the following combinations:
A-argument P-argument Pluralizer
sg pl -la-
du pl -la-
pl sg -la-
pl du NO
pl pl -la-
As one can see, an accessible plural P-argument always needs the pluralizer,
whereas an accessible plural A-argument needs the pluralizer only in the context of a
singular P-argument. It is clear that the lack of the pluralizer in case of a plural A-
argument codes the dual value of the P-argument, i.e. the lack of the pluralizer in this
case is marked. This is an argument for the postulation of the marker -∅- ‘du.P’.
Assuming this to be the case, the pluralizer slot is free, and it becomes potentially
accessible for the pluralizer controlled by respective A-argument only in case of the
singular P-argument. Thus, accessible arguments can be the real controllers of
pluralization in the following pluralization conditions:
77
When du/pl number is not coded by markers -t/-w, see Table 6.
78
When du/pl number is not coded by 3rd person markers -t/-w, see Comment 2 to Table 8.
56
2.1.2. Monopersonal conjugation of finite verbs
Two finite forms belong to this type, one of them has the 3sg marker ga 79 -...-lin, the
other has the marker n 80 -...-qin. These forms have only one (suffixal) agreement slot,
so they can be called forms with m o n o p e r s o n a l agreement.
n-...-qin forms are identical to the adjective conjugation (therefore they are
called d e v e r b a l a d j e c t i v e s a n d glossed as ADJ`) and ga-...-lin forms are
analogous to the conjugation of derived adjectives 81 (these forms are called
r e s u l t a t i v e and glossed as RES).
The argument controlling agreement of these verb forms varies depending on a
verb's transitivity/intransitivity: intransitive verbs agree with the S-argument (marked
with the nominative), transitive verbs agree with the P-argument (also marked with
the nominative).
Person-number agreement markers of these forms coincide with those of the
adjectives, namely: agreement is controlled by p e r s o n (1, 2, 3) and n u m b e r (sg,
du, pl) of the appropriate argument. Instead of the expected resultative marker *-
lina(w(wi)), the 3pl marker has the marker -laN with variants -laNin || laNina || -
laNina(w(wi)). The deverbal adjective, along with the -qina(w(wi)) marker, often
uses the resultative markers.
Number sg du pl
Person
1 ga-...-jgym ga-...-muri ga-...-muru
2 ga-...-jgyt ga-...-turi ga-...-turu
3 ga-...-lin ga-...-lina-t ga-...-laN ~ -laNin ~ -laNina(w(wi))
79
Before the vowel-initial stem it has the variant g-.
80
Before the consonant-initial stem it has the variant ny-.
81
These markers are used for the formation of the denominal habitive adjectives; see 2.2.2.
57
Number sg du pl
Person
1 n-...-jgym n-...-muri n-...-muru
2 n-...-jgyt n-...-turi n-...-turu
3 n-...-qin n-...-qina-t n-...-qina(w(wi)) ~ -laN ~ -laNin ~ -laNina(w(wi))
ga-...-lin finite forms are very frequent in narrative texts. They belong to the
domain of real modality and describe a situation a s a f a c t rather than as an event
(on opposition between fact/event see [Arutyunova 1988]). With that, these forms
signify, as a rule, the result of the action; therefore they are interpreted as resultative.
They often have the additional meaning of indirect evidentinality.
The n-...-qin finite forms signify a habitual action that usually takes place or
potentially can take place.
82
For the lack of an example of deverbal adjective cf. a similar predicative form a-npy-k-
egym ‘A-old-PRED-1sg’.
58
c. ynnu ny-waNi-qin.
She (usually) does needlework.
d. sully ny-jyl-qin?
Is there salt on sale?
e. pyHunny ny-nu-qin.
The mushroom is edible.
b. a-pHa-ka, [email protected].
A-feel.thirsty-PRED` 2.A+OPT-1sg.P-give.water.to.drink-PLUR-2nsg.S
(I) feel thirsty, give me water to drink. [3:2]
c. taraN-i titkymsys@y-n,
put.up.a.yurt-3sg.S+PF Titkemsesen -NOM+SG
83
The imperative function is carried out by 2nd person optative, see 2.1.7.3.
84
In front of vowel initial stems the prefix component of the marker is missing.
59
aktyka unJunJu ny-pila-ni-n,
impossible child+NOM+SG 3.A+OPT-leave-3sg.A+3P-3sg.P
nuta-N aktyka ny-lqyty-n, a-tirNy-ka.
tundra-DAT impossible 3.S+OPT-go-3sg.S A-cry-PRED`
Titkemsesen put up the yurt. (He) could not leave the child and go to the tundra,
(because he) {i.e. the child} was crying. [7:48]
This form is very often optionally accompanied by the particle ally which has a
negative meaning (see details in 2.1.5):
85
The desiderative marker occupies a position inside the verb stem, therefore it can co-occur
with the potential marker (cf. (iv) with (ii-iii)):
60
b. a-tHym=la@u-ka mur.gina sAkIt-u?
A-bone=see-PRED` our+NOM+PL sister-NOM+PL
Haven't you found the bones of our sisters? [8:77]
It is also used for the formation of the analytical negative (see 2.1.4).
The imperative is formed by means of the circumfix ga-...-a/-ta 86 . Imperative
forms do not have the aspectual values, and lack regular personal markers 87 .
Nevertheless the imperative has restricted inflectional devices. First, it has number
agreement with a 1-2nd person argument identical to that of the polypersonal
conjugation (the marker -la, see 2.1.1.2.5). Second, it has 1sg P-argument agreement
by means of the inversion marker -ina, see 2.1.1.2.3. For example:
86
Variant -ta occurs after final full vowel stems, and variant -a — after final consonant or
final y stem.
87
It is natural to suppose that the prefix and suffix parts of the imperative marker occupy the
personal S/A/P-slots of verb form (ranks -4 and +4). It is also possible to regard the imperative as
derived from umnarked perfective stem.
61
auxiliary depends on the (in)transitivity of the lexical verb: intransitive verbs are
combined with the auxiliary ‘be’ and transitive verbs with the auxiliary ‘do, have’.
The analytical forms have positive as well as negative polarity. The lexical verb
inside p o s i t i v e a n a l y t i c a l f o r m s has the suffix marker -a/-ta (identical to the
ergative marker). There is also the suffix marker -N, see (9b), identical to the dative
marker. These forms are almost impossible outside of analytical verb forms; therefore
their converbal (see below) interpretation is not desirable 88 . We will refer to them as
the analytical form of the lexical verb 89 (glossed as ANLT).
The following are examples with intransitive verb:
88
There are two textual examples contradicting this interpretation that deserve a special
mention:
-a/-ta form, according translation, is used here as a converb, however in both cases the
subordinate as well as main verbs are the same: ‘eat’ and ‘wash down’. There is an assumption
that in this case the -a/-ta form is not a free one, but it is a part of idiomatic expression signifying
‘to eat and drink simultaneously’.
89
The analytical representation permits negative polarity, nuN- prefix marker being added;
see 2.1.5.
62
{LC: The Ilir and Alut people left at first for Wiwen from here, and at once from there,}
from Wiwen, at the same time three military troops left... [19:26]
Auxiliary verbs are compatible with different mood values: (10a) exemplifies
the indicative, (10b) the optative, (10c) the conjunctive, and (10d) the potential.
See 2.1.5 below for a discussion of the n e g a t i v e a n a l y t i c a l f o r m s .
Besides the regular auxiliaries, the verb n@al- ‘become’ 90 may also function as
90
A single example has also been documented with the auxiliary verb awwav- ‘leave’ in the
context of the lexical motion verb:
63
an auxiliary, indicating the starting point of the event expressed by the negative form
of the lexical verb:
64
(12) a. qytymmy my-vitaty-k!
not 1sg.S+OPT-work-1sg.S+PF
I will not work!
b. qytymmy qy-vitat-gi!
not 2sg.S+OPT-work-2sg.S+PF+OPT
Do not work!
Type (2) has two realizations. In combination with the negative particle ally, it
contrasts with the positive forms of the real moods. In combination with the particle
kytvy(l), it represents the prohibitive as a negative correlate of optative/imperative.
In type (2) reduction of the negative marker is possible (the negative particle is
absent):
65
Type (3) is the analytical extension of type (2) for real moods. Both full and
reduced realizations are possible.
Type (4) uses a negative analytical representation of the lexical verb as a basic
structural element in combination with the optative auxiliary verb. This type is a
negative correlate of the optative. Optionally the presence of a negative particle is
possible.
b. [email protected] myny-nty-nawwi.
NEG-look.for-go-ANLT 1nsg.A+OPT-do-3pl.P
Let's not go to look for them. [6:14]
66
b. numal unJunJu ally a-jajary=ttil-kyl@in.
again child+NOM+SG not A-drum=put-NEG+3sg
(My) child doesn't put the drum down again. [8:35]
2.1.6.1. The infinitive is derived from the verb stem by means of the -k/-ki marker
(identical to the locative marker), the allomorph -ki usually being used after stems
with final -N, cf. alu-k ‘pick berries’, tannuN-ki ‘fish’. In accordance with phonetic
rules (see 1.3.4.3), the allomorph -k can be realized as -kki after the stressed syllable,
cf. gyvô-kki ‘get stuck’. Most frequently the infinitive is used in the context of phasal
verbs, however other contexts are also possible (see details below in 2.1.7.5).
2.1.6.2. The supine is derived from the verb stem by means of the marker -nvyN. It is
used in the context of motion verbs as well as of other verbs where the expression of
goal is necessary (see (18a-c)). If the goal of the main action is prevention rather than
accomplishment of an action, the negative correlate of the supine is used (tymNi-...-a/-
ta) (see 18d).
2.1.6.3. Converbs are also derived from the verb stem. They are often marked by
affixes similar to the spatial cases. Converbs do not have any inflectional categories.
67
They are usually used in subordinate clause and express temporal relations between
the main and subordinate clauses.
The -k/-ki converb shares the same marker as the locative. This converb refers
to a situation that immediately precedes the main action:
This marker can also be added to the resultative (ga-...-laN) deriving a ga-...-
laN-ki converb with approximately the same meaning, cf.:
The -kyN converb derived from the potential shares the same marker as the
lative. This converb refers to a situation as following the main action:
68
(22) ynN.in naqam tury=pkir-inaNu awyn
this+NOM+SG well just-arrive-CONV already
aNaNta.l@aty-tky, tHyl-ly@y-N ra.lqiv-i.
sing-IPF be.sick-ATR-DAT enter-3sg.S+PF
And this (shaman), having just arrived, already was singing. (He) dropped in on the
sick person. [29:19]
The ga-...-a/-ta and geqy-...-a/-ta converbs share the same markers with the
comitative and associative, respectively, and refer to an action that is parallel to the
main one:
b. qutkinJnJaqu qy-meNy=rwil-la-gy-tki
Qutkinnaqu+NOM+SG 2.A+OPT-big=beat.up-PLUR-2.A+3.P-2nsg.A+3.P
awyn-miti-ma geqy-n.qujma.viri.v-a.
COMIT-Miti-COMIT CONV-lower.trousers-CONV
...beat up Qutkinnaqu together with Miti badly, lowering (their) trousers. [10:68]
The -jpyN converb shares the same marker with the prolative. This converb
refers to an action that is parallel to the main one and is intimately bound with it:
69
2.1.6.4. The participle is derived by means of the attributivizer -l@ 92 added to the
verb stem. The head noun phrase of a relative clause, as well as a (relativized)
coreferential argument of a subordinate clause headed by the participle, should have
nominative marking (see 3.4.1). In this position the participle also has nominative
marking and agrees in person and number with the dependent S/P-arguments by
means of suffix markers:
sg du pl
1st person -jgym -muri -muru
2nd person -jgyt -turi -turu
3rd person -n -t -u(wwi)
92
This marker derives attributive forms from nouns, adjectives, and adverbs, for example:
rynny-l@y-n ‘horned’ (ср. rynny-lNyn ‘horn’), tinmyla-l@y-n ‘that who deceives’ (cf. tinmyla-
‘false’), a-jIly-ky-l@y-n ‘that who without mouth’ (ср. a-jIly-ka ‘without mouth’).
Besides there is also the attributivizer -s@- that means ‘holding the property in great
degree’, for example: aNaNy-l@y-n ‘that who is godlike, shaman’ (cf.aNaN- ‘godlike’) — aNaNy-
s@y-n ‘the strongest between shamans’.
70
2.1.7. The values of some verbal categories
2.1.7.1. Indicative
The indicative refers to the events belonging to the real world, i.e. events that took or
are taking place. Temporal deixis of respective forms is secondary. The perfective
usually describes events that took place before the speech act, see (27a). The
imperfective is neutral to the temporal axis. It can be used for events proceeding
during the speech act as well as prolonged incompleted events before the speech act,
see (27b):
The imperfective also describes prolonged events that continuously take place
or are repeated. Thus, texts 25-28, describing traditional customs, abound in
indicative imperfective forms, for example:
71
(29) Nynvys.saN gymmy ty-walqivy-lqivy-k to numal
several.times I+NOM1sg. S-run.away-LQIV-1sg.S+PF and again
na-lla-tkyni-gym sykola-N.
LOW.A-carry-IPF-1sg.P school-DAT
I had run away many times (before), but every time [=again] I was returned to school.
[37:5]
In (29) both actions are repeated, however the first verb (‘run away’) has
perfective marking, and the second one (‘carry’) — imperfective marking. This is
because the first action is punctual and the second is prolonged.
In analytical negative forms relating to the temporal domain of the definite
future the auxiliary verb can have imperfective marking:
2.1.7.2. Potential
Potential means that the event P does not belong to the world of real events,
nevertheless P can potentially be in this world. This event does not have any
epistemic restrictions. It is quite natural that this type of event is closely related to
events that, in languages with the grammatical category of tense, pertain to future.
Therefore in most cases potential verb forms are translated as future forms:
2.1.7.3. Optative
The optative, like the conjunctive, pertains to moods that refer to the mental state of
the speaker. The prototypical properties of the optative are the following:
— the speaker is thinking about a virtual event P;
72
— the speaker wants event P to be performed;
— the speaker highly estimates the reality of the approach of the event P;
— the speaker is talking about this.
The speaker's capability to control the approach of event P depends on the
person of the active participant (actor) of this event.
In the context of the 1sg actor (identical to the speaker) the optative, along with
the speaker/actor's wish for the performance of event P, refers to his intention or wish
to accomplish it:
The usage of the 1pl optative is analogous: the optative refers to the speaker's
intention to perform an action and his address to the listener to do this together with
him:
73
Qutkinnaqu, maybe we will go (and) visit (my) home. [7:24]
c. nuN-alu-sqiv-a myn-ity-tkyn.
NEG-pick.berries-go-ANLT 1nsg.S+OPT-be-IPF
We won't have to go picking berries. [7:18]
It the context of a 3rd person S/A-argument, the optative refers to the speaker's
wish that the event in question happens to that 3rd person:
In the dialogue the speaker (the man) presupposes that the hearer (the bear) wishes
the skis had stayed home at night.
It is not by chance, from the point of view of this mood, that the optative is
obligatory in the context of a predicate of impossibility to perform the action, because
the actor's wish to perform it is presupposed:
74
b. aktyka unJunJu ny-pila-ni-n,
impossible child+NOM+SG 3.A+OPT-leave-3sg.A+3P-3sg.P
nuta-N aktyka ny-lqyty-n, a-tirNy-ka.
tundra-DAT impossible 3.S+OPT-go-3sg.S A-cry-PRED`
{LC: Titkemsesen put up the yurt. He} could not leave the child and go to the tundra,
(because he) {i.e. the child} was crying. [7:48]
In (38b) the intention to leave the child and go into the tundra is ascribed to the actor
of these actions (to Titkemsesen) rather to the speaker. The semantics of the
following optative is similar:
From the point of view of Kirumsen's arrival, the village being deserted is interpreted
by him to be as a result of some cause preventing the natural intention of the
inhabitants to be outdoors.
In the context of the 2nd person S/A-argument, i.e. in the context of appeal to
the addressee in connection with his actions, the semantics of the optative naturally
acquires a perlocutive connotation of speech influence over the addressee's actions:
75
gy-nanny gy.ninny q-ina-jyl-gi .
you-ERG your+3sg A+OPT-1sg.P-give-2sg.S+PF+OPT
Rettetiniangawet, let's exchange clothes. I will give you my clothes, (and) you give me
yours. [7:32]
In sum, from the point of view of the proposed analysis of the optative, 1st and
3rd person forms are most closely related to its prototypical meaning, while 2nd
person forms are the natural extension of this meaning towards semantics of the
imperative 93 .
2.1.7.4. Conjunctive
The conjunctive's semantics is related to an event P that is desirable for the speaker,
but is not realized in the actual world. In contrast to the optative, also related to the
domain of desirability, the conjunctive represents the desirability of event P as a
given, without the speaker's aim to participate actively in the event. The degree of
reality of an event can vary, from highly probable, especially if the speaker is the
actor of the event, to irreal— in conditional clauses. Conjunctive verb forms are
translated in different ways.
Forms in the 1sg actor conjunctive are translated as “should not I do P”, where
P is denoted by the conjunctive verb form:
93
Traditional analysis presupposing the 2nd person imperative to be the prototypical
meaning for these forms fails on the grounds of the usage of this mood with the 1st and especially
with the 3rd person.
76
Probably it is enough. I will begin to prepare, {because there is our former sister.} I
wish I could attack her and subject her to an excruciating death. [20:54]
Forms in the 1pl actor conjunctive are translated as “If we could do P”:
(43) a. anJmy¥@ak myn-@y-tur=@oji-la-tky-t...
at.least 1nsg.S+CONJ-CONJ-fresh=feed-PLUR-IPF-3nsg.S+IPF
If only we could eat fresh meat,... [8:6]
In this case the wish component of the meaning is forgrounded. Sometimes the
conjunctive means the availability of potential resources to accomplish event P:
77
(45) yjatuk, miti, ny-@-etu-n
if.only Miti+NOM+SG 2sg.A+CONJ-CONJ-give.birth-3sg.P
Nav.akka.pilJ.
daughter+NOM+SG
If only you, Miti, were to give birth to a daughter. [5: 3]
Examples of the 3rd person conjunctive are rare in our collection of texts. In the
following sentence an irreal event looking like a real one is talked about:
78
both of them? [30:5]
The conjunctive as well as the optative is used in purpose clauses with the
conjunction qinaq ‘in order that/to’:
79
ty-@y-ttily-n semy.k milgy-k, to tita
1sg.A-CONJ-put-3sg.P close.to fire-LOC and when
n-A-pytkaty-n wutt.in piNpiN-in
3sg.S+CONJ-CONJ-shoot-3sg.S this+3sg gunpowder-POSS+3sg
kuka-jyr@y-n, to qinaq rara-Na
pot-contents-NOM+SG and in.order.to house-NOM+SG
nan-@y-nqu-n.
LOW.A+CONJ-CONJ-carry.away-3sg.P
I shall put this pot with gunpowder near the fire so that it will explode and blow up the
house. [22:87]
2.1.7.5. Infinitive
The infinitive forms sentential complements of phasal verbs (with typical meanings
‘begin’ 94 , ‘continue’, ‘finish’). Phasal verbs inherit the transitivity / intransitivity
properties of subordinate verbs marked as the infinitive. In the context of an
intransitive embedded verb the main verb has an intransitive agreement pattern and
agrees with a noun marked with the nominative:
b. ...qutkinJnJaqu-nti ga-r.ety-lina-t to
Qutkinnaqu-NOM+DU RES-return.home-RES-3du.S and
ga-Nvu-lina-t jun.aty-k.
RES-begin-RES-3du.S live-INF
...the Qutkinnaqus returned home and began to live {there again}. [6:45]
c. ...inJas q-il-la-tyk valatkyla-k...
enough 2.S+OPT-be-PLUR-2nsg.S run.after-INF
...Well, that's enough. Stop running (after me)... [8:59]
However, in the context of a transitive embedded verb the main verb gets a
transitive agreement pattern:
94
Statistically the overwhelming number of infinitive textual occurences are governed by the
verb ‘begin’.
80
(51) a. q-ina-Nvu-la-tyk turgy-nan gymmy
2.S+OPT-1sg.P-begin-PLUR-2nsg.S you-ERG I+NOM
kyla.l@aty-k...
run.after-INF
Begin to pursue me... [21:89]
In (51a) the arguments turgynan ‘you’ and gymmy ‘me’ are governed by the
verb ‘pursue’ and at the same time they control agreement of the verb ‘begin’. In
(51b) the main verb is inflected for monopersonal conjugation and agrees with the
patientive argument for the plural.
In case of decreased transitivity of the embedded infinitival verb, as a result of
P-argument incorporation, the phasal verb also receives an intransitive interpretation:
b. ...ga-pkav-lina-t ta-squn-Ny-k...
RES-be.unable.to-RES-3du.S DESID-stand.up-DESID-INF
95
Dependent verb is usually marked as the desiderative because the denoted event belongs
to the “desirable” world.
81
...they couldn't stand up... [30:52]
d. a-ryks.av-ka oji-k.
A-hurry-PRED` eat-INF
Don't hurry to eat. [11:19]
Adjectives can also occasionally govern the infinitive construction that specify
an adjective's truth domain:
82
t.u-kki.
eat-INF
...In winter various dry fish is delicious [=good for eating]. [33:33]
In the context of the phasal verb ‘begin’, the infinitive is often combined with
the desiderative marker specifying the modal description of the event: the infinitive
designates that the agent of the event attempts to accomplish the action:
83
ga-Nvu-lina-t ta-katgu.tvi-N-ki.
RES-begin-RES-3du.S DESID-become.strong-DESID-INF
The two brothers grew up (and) began to practise in order to become strong. {RC: Later
they became strong enough to stop shooting with arrows with wooden tips...} [20:2]
2.2. Adjectives
Alutor shows a clear morphological opposition between two classes of adjectives:
q u a l i t a t i v e adjectives and r e l a t i v e adjectives. These classes of adjectives
have different markers and belong to different derivation types. Qualitative adjectives
appear as an original lexical group, while relative adjectives are derived from other
parts of speech (mostly nouns).
From a syntactic point of view adjectives are used either a u t o n o m o u s l y
(i.e. as separate words, usually attributes or predicates), or in i n c o r p o r a t i o n . An
adjective can be used attributively only when its head noun is marked for the
nominative (see 2.2.4). In this case an attribute agrees in p e r s o n and n u m b e r
with its head noun. If an adjective is used predicatively, it also agrees in person and
number with its S-actant, so the forms used in both situations are identical. In the case
its head noun is not a nominative case form, an adjective is usually incorporated (for
details see 2.2.4).
We will first consider qualitative and relative adjectives used autonomously,
and then discuss adjective incorporation.
84
TABLE 12. Person-number forms
of the qualitative adjective mraj- ‘lucky’
Number sg du pl
Person
1 ny-mraj-igym ny-mre-muri 96 ny-mre-muru
2 ny-mraj-igyt ny-mre-turi ny-mre-turu
3 ny-mre-qin ny-mre-qina-t ny-mre-qina(-w) ~
ny-mre-laN ~
ny-mre-laNin ~
ny-mre-laNina(w(wi))
Substantivized adjectives used in oblique cases (that is those different from the
nominative) are not found in Alutor.
Another adjective form is derived from a qualitative stem by means of a-...-ka.
It is called the p r e d i c a t i v e form. Its only function is to indicate a predicate 97 :
96
Here the diphthong-like combination aj changes into e according to the coalescence rules,
see 1.3.6.
97
Thus, there are two adjective forms used predicatively: the form marked with n-...-qin
and the form marked with a-...-ka. Their distribution is not clear, but it is possible that they
express the difference between the permanent and temporary state respectively (so they are
similar to Russian full and short adjectives).
85
piNinaN.
Pinginang+NOM+SG
Emqa's son, Pinginang (was) quick [=skilled]. [21:84]
b. am-pensija-wwi tin a-mqy-ka-wwi ...
only-pension-NOM+PL well A-small-PRED-3pl
Only the pension is not quite enough [=small] ... [34:4]
c. ... gymmy ajevaq a-npy-k-egym.
I+NOM already A-old-PRED-1sg
... Really I'm already old. [19:76]
The examples given above show that the form marked with only a-...-ka
indicates the 3rd person singular. The corresponding plural form has a pluralizer -
w(wi), the 1st and the 2nd person markers are the same as for other adjectives (see
Table 11), among them -jgym, -jgyt (these two undergo certain phonological changes,
see 1.3.6).
The adverbial modifier is also derived from a qualitative stem. It is marked with
the adverbializer n-...-@a, for instance: ny-katgu-@a ga-rwil-lin ‘(he) strongly
tugged it’ [20:66] (cf. ny-katgu-qin ‘strong’), Navaq nJy-mqy-@a myt-sulJaty-tky-
nawwi ‘if we salt them only a little’ [33:29] (cf. nJy-mqy-qin ‘little, small’). The
suffixal part of the adverbializer occupies the position of agreement marker.
Qualitative adjectives have several forms that can be referred to as d e g r e e s
of comparison.
The prefix mal-, added to an adjective, expresses a greater degree of the
quality:
Another derivative marker, namely the suffix -l@, is identical to the verbal
attributivizer 98 . It is used to build forms expressing a high degree of quality:
98
The attributivizer is usually added to a verbal stem and changes its form, while qualitative
adjectives are originally of an attributive nature.
86
(Once) a rich man Qisgayat became sick. [29:4]
In example (61) the form pyttuNy-l@y-n ‘the rich’ is derived from the adjective
ny-pyttuN-qin ‘rich’. Such forms can be used both attributively (as appositions) and
non-attributively (as nouns). Being used attributively, they agree in case and number
with the head noun.
The highest degree of quality can also be expressed by means of the suffix -s@,
identical with the verbal attributivizer expressing intense actions. The suffix is often
accompanied by the prefix am-, cf.:
b. am-mis@a-s@y-n it@y-n
SUPERL-beautiful-SUPERL-NOM+SG fur.coat-NOM+SG
g-epy-lin ...
RES-put.on-RES+3sg.P
They were dressed in the most beautiful fur coats 99 {RC: and were going around the
house.} [6:9]
Another way to express a high degree of quality is to add the prefix ynan- (yn-
ynnuxwin ynan-@yssaNi ‘Here is the youngest (daughter), this one’ [32:12]), often
accompanied by the suffix -s@ (cf. ynan-meNy-s@y-n qylik=tytka ‘the biggest male
walrus’ [22:14]).
In a comparative construction the object of comparison appears in the dative
case, while the adjective is in its usual form, but with the incorporated quantifier
Hopty- ‘most’:
99
It is remarkable that the Alutor word is in the singular, which means that there is only one
object for each possible subject of the action.
87
2.2.2. Relative adjectives
R e l a t i v e adjectives (= the adjectival form of a noun), express a general modifying
meaning. They are usually derived from nouns, and sometimes from adverbs and
verbs. They are marked with the adjectivizer -kin(a) (further shortened to kin). Being
used attributively, a relative adjective agrees in person and number with its S-actant.
Thus, in the 3rd person it is marked with kin/-kina-t/-kina(-w(wwi)) ‘3sg/du/pl’,
and in the 1st or 2nd person the standard person-number markers are used, following
the adjectivizer -kin(a), for instance, -kin-egym 100 ‘1sg’, see Table 13.
Number sg du pl
Person
1 -kin-egym -kina-muri -kina-muru
2 -kin-egyt -kina-turi -kina-turu
3 -kin -kina-t -kina(-w(wi))
Examples:
In (64a) the relative adjective is derived from a noun, in (64b) from an adverb,
and in (64c) from a verb.
Being used co-predicatively, a relative adjective may express an ablative
meaning 101 :
100
The endings -egym/-egyt are phonological variants of the agreement markers -jgym/-
jgyt, which are added to the suffix ending in a-, namely -kina.
101
Thus, the allative and ablative functions have no special markers in Alutor.
88
(65) a. numal ty-wa.lqivy-k sykola-kin-egym.
again 1sg.S-run.away-sg.S+PF school-REL-1sg
Again I ran away from school. [37:4]
b. ... t-araty-k ujatik-kin-egym ...
1sg.S-fall-1sg.S+PF sledge-REL-1sg
... I fell from the sledge ... [37:9]
Number sg du pl
Person
1 ga-...-jgym ga-...-muri ga-...-muru
2 ga-...-jgyt ga-...-turi ga-...-turu
3 ga-...-lin ga-...-lina-t ga-...-laN ~
ga-...-laNin ~
ga-...-laNina(-w(wi))
Examples:
b. [email protected] g-unJunJu-laN.
all HABIT-child-3pl
They (were) all with children. [8:64]
89
way as a possessive adjective (see 2.2.3). Adverbialization is shown in (67a), and the
caritive adjective form in (67b):
Number sg du pl
Person
1 a-...-kyl@-igym 102 a-...-kyl@y-muri a-...-kyl@y-muru
2 a-...-kyl@-igyt a-...-kyl@y-turi a-...-kyl@y-turu
3 a-...-kyl@in a-...-kyl@ina-t a-...-kyl@ina(-w(wi)
102
The vowel i of the morph -igym appears as a result of the coalescence of an epenthetic y
and j (see 1.3.6).
90
of possessive adjectives derived from 1st declension nouns
Number sg du Pl
Person
1 -in-egym -ina-muri -ina-muru
2 -in-egyt -ina-turi -ina-turu
3 -in -ina-t -ina(-w(wi)
Since 2nd declension nouns distinguish both singular and non-singular (see
Table 18), they also distinguish number of the base noun (the possessor) in the
possessive forms. When the possessor is singular, the possessive suffix -in(a) is
placed after the oblique stem marker -yn(a), but -in(a) is often omitted, so the
following optional markers appear: -yn-in ~ -yn ‘3sg’, -yn-ina-t ~ -yna-t ‘3du’, -
yn-ina-w ~ -yna-w ‘3pl’, for instance: qutkynJnJaqu-nin (~ -n) ‘3sg’, qutkynJnJaqu-
ninat (~ nat) ‘3du’, qutkynJnJaqu-ninaw (~ -naw) ‘3pl’ − the possessive forms of the
singular noun qutkynJnJaqu ‘Qutkinnaqu, NOM+SG’, with the ergative
qutkynJnJaqu-nak ‘Qutkinnaqu, ERG+SG’. When the possessor is non-singular (=
dual or plural), the possessives are marked with -tg (obviously related to the oblique
stem marker -tyk): -tg-in ‘3sg’, -tg-ina-t ‘3du’, -tg-ina-w ‘3pl’, for instance,
utkinJnJaqu-tgin unJunJu ‘Qutkinnaqus' son’ − the possessive form derived from the
plural noun qutkynJnJaqu-tyk ‘Qutkinnaqu, NOM+PL’, with the ergative
qutkynJnJaqu-tyk ‘Qutkinnaqu, ERG+PL’. The possessive person-number markers
are shown in Table 17.
Singular possessor
Number sg du pl
Person
1 -ynin-egym ~ -ynina-muri ~ -ynina-muru ~
-yn-egym -yna-muri -yna-muru
2 -ynin-egyt ~ -ynina-turi ~ -ynina-turu ~
-yn-egyt -yna-turi -yna-turu
3 -ynin ~ -yn -ynina-t ~ -yna-t -ynina(-w(wi)) ~
-yna(-w(wi))
91
Non-singular (dual or plural) possessor
Number sg du pl
Person
1 -ytgin-egym -ytgina-muri -ytgina-muru
2 -ytgin-egyt -ytgina-turi -ytgina-turu
3 -ytgin -ytgina-t -ytgina(-w(wi))
92
it “syntactic” incorporation, cf. ilgy=qura-ta ‘by a white reindeer, ERG’ vs. n-ilgy-
qin qura-Na ‘white reindeer, NOM+SG’, meNy=rara-k ‘in a big house, LOC’ vs.
ny-meNy-qin rara-Na ‘big house, NOM+SG’, yllyg-ina=rara-k ‘in the father's
house, LOC’ vs. yllyg-in rara-Na ‘the father's house, NOM+SG’.
2.3. Nouns
The semantic and syntactic functions of a noun are expressed mostly by the items of
nominal grammatical categories (case-number marking and person-number marking –
see 2.3.1.1, 2.3.1.2 and 2.3.2), in some cases by derivative markers of location
(2.3.1.3) and adjectivization (2.3.3); some local functions are expressed outside a
locative noun by means of adpositions (2.3.1.3). Nouns are used either
a u t o n o m o u s l y or in i n c o r p o r a t i o n . Being used autonomously, a noun is
marked for a set of grammatical categories. Incorporated nouns are just bare stems,
without any category markers.
The set of grammatical categories a noun is inflected for depends on its
syntactic position in a sentence. Three types of noun forms are distinguished on the
morphological level: nominal, predicative and adjectival. The n o m i n a l form is
used in the position of an actant, the p r e d i c a t i v e form − in the position of a
predicate, the a d j e c t i v a l form − in the position of an attribute.
Case and number, in the forms where number is distinguished, are expressed by
93
fused items. Most of the items are suffixes, but the comitative and associative
markers are circumfixes. Besides these, some nouns form the nominative singular by
means of an operation − the final vowel truncation or the root reduplication.
Alutor nouns are divided into three groups with two sets of person-number
markers called d e c l e n s i o n s : the 1st declension and the 2nd declension.
The first group includes common nouns denoting non-humans. These nouns are
inflected using 1st declension items. 1st declension forms distinguish number
(singular, dual and plural) only in the nominative. In other cases (= oblique cases)
number is not distinguished, but is sometimes marked using verbal agreement or
defined by the context.
The second group includes proper names (human and those of domestic
animals) and kinship terms denoting elder relations. 2nd declension forms distinguish
number not only in the nominative, but also in some oblique cases; in the nominative
they distinguish singular, dual and plural, while in oblique cases they distinguish only
singular (SG) and non-singular (NSG)
The third group includes all other common nouns denoting humans. These
nouns are inflected using either 1st or 2nd declension forms (though the distribution
of these forms is not fully clear). For instance, the noun yllygy- ‘father’ has the
ergative yllyg-a (1st declension), which expresses either singular or plural, depending
on the context, and also the ergative yllygy-nak (2nd declension, singular) and yllygy-
tyk (2nd declension, plural). The case-number forms are given in Table 18.
94
TABLE 18. Case-number inflections (nominal forms)
103
The short variants result from the phonetic allomorphs -epyN/-ipyN (see below).
95
The final vowel truncation is accompanied by standard phonological processes,
cf. tatul (root {tatula}) ‘fox’, but akyk (root {akka}) ‘son’, imyt (root {imti}) ‘burden’
(since consonant clusters do not occur word-finally, a schwa is inserted here − see
1.3.3).
The inflectional ending -n is added to stems ending in either a vowel or a
consonant 104 , cf. gytga-n (< {gytga-n}) ‘late autumn’, nalgy-n (< {nalg-n}) ‘fur
skin’.
The inflectional ending -lNyn is usually used with nouns denoting body parts
(both human and animal's), names of animals, and also with nouns denoting one
object of a pair, cf. vilu-lNyn ‘ear’, masvi-lNyn ‘animal's chest’, Higy-lNyn (< {Hig-
lNyn}) ‘wolf’, lili-lNyn ‘mitten’. After stems ending in a homorganic consonant the
inflectional ending -lNyn appears as -lyNyn (see 1.3.3.3), cf. aNar-lyNyn ‘star’.
The inflectional ending -qal is used only with nouns denoting one object of a
pair, cf. pylak-qal (along with pylaky-lNyn) ‘fur boot’, lili-qal (along with lili-lNyn)
‘mitten’.
The inflectional ending -Na is used with a few nouns that probably belong to
the original Alutor lexicon: ypa-Na ‘soup’, kuka-Na ‘pot’, mami-Na ‘fish drying
shed’, qura-Na ‘reindeer’, rara-Na ‘yurt, house’, ruru-Na ‘bed, sleeping place, titi-
Na ‘needle’, typa-Na ‘stone hammer’.
Many original and some loan-words form the nominative singular by copying
(= reduplicating) the root morph. Such words belong to different semantic groups, cf.
anu-@an (< {anured}) ‘spring’, wiru-wir (< {wirured}) ‘(kind of) salmon’ (for
reduplication rules see 1.3.1).
The ergative markers -a/-ta are distributed according to the final segment of a
stem: the variant -a is used with stems ending in a consonant or a schwa, while the
variant -ta is used with stems ending in a non-schwa vowel, cf. tyrg-a ‘meat, ERG’,
jIl-a (< {jIly-a}) ‘tongue, ERG’, , but gytka-ta ‘leg, ERG’.
In the locative the inflectional ending -ki is characteristic of stems ending in N
(and sometimes in w), in other cases the ending -k or its phonological variant -kki are
used (the latter − after a stressed vowel), cf. lyqlaN-ki ‘winter, LOC’, rara-k ‘house,
ERG’, mimly-k ‘water, LOC’, but lylô-kki ‘eye, LOC’.
The dative, as well as the lative (the latter can be treated historically as a double
case composed of the locative and dative items), has no variants.
In the prolative the morph -gypyN is used with the stems ending in a single
consonant, while in other cases the morph -jpyN is used. If a stem ends in the vowel -
104
If a stem ends in a consonant, an epenthetic y is inserted to break up an illicit consonant
cluster (see 1.3.3.2).
96
a, the morph -jpyN appears as -epyN; the same variant is used if a stem ends in a two
consonant cluster, because in such cases the epenthetic y is inserted, and the
combination yj in most cases changes into e, though in some rare cases – into i (see
1.3.6), so the marker -ipyN 105 may appear, cf. qaNJav-gypyN ‘ravine, PROLAT’, nut-
epyN (< {nuta-jpyN}) ‘land, PROLAT’, winv-epyN ~ winv-ipyN (< winvy 106 -jpyN <
{winv-jpyN}) ‘road, PROLAT’. The morphs -e and -i are short variants of the morphs
-epyN and -ipyN respectively.
The equative markers -u/-nu have the same distribution as the ergative markers
-a/-ta: the variant -u is used with stems ending in a consonant or a schwa, while the
variant -nu is used with stems ending in a non-schwa vowel, cf. tavHal-u ‘dried fish,
EQUAT’, keN-u (< {keNy-u}) ‘bear, EQUAT’, but ypa-nu ‘soup, EQUAT’.
The variants -a/-ta in the comitative and associative are identical with those of
the ergative and have the same distribution, cf. ga-qlavul-a ‘husband, COMIT’, ga-
masla-ta ‘butter, COMIT’, geqy-masla-ta ‘butter, ASSOC’.
105
These variants appear because the inflection -jpyN historically comes from the root {jp},
and undergoes changes given in rule (11) (see 1.3.6) either as a root or a suffix.
106
Here and in some other cases the vowel y is epenthetic.
107
We call this form the “expressive” vocative.
97
Qutkinnaqu took a stick ... [6:10]
The instrumental and ergative functions can be expressed by the ergative within
the same clause:
The l o c a t i v e is used to express: a) essive (see (71a)) and lative (see (71b))
local functions, b) peripherally governed actants (see (71c)):
98
Qutkinnaqu said to the fish ... [1:11]
The e q u a t i v e is used to express functions ‘such as X’, ‘as X’, with verbs like
‘become (smth., smb.)’, ‘turn (into smb.)’, ‘work as (smb.)’ etc.:
99
b. ... inu-nu ga-jyl-lin keN-a...
food.for.a.trip-EQUAT RES-give-RES+3sg.P bear-ERG
The bear gave (his own rib) for food . [12:16]
100
[10:68]
101
APUD ‘place near an object’, the marker -teny, for instance: ra-teny-k ‘(to
dance) by the neighbouring house’ [39:4], inmy-teny-k ‘(to build a village) near the
cliff’ [23:3], ynykka-teny-k ‘(to put smth.) near himself’ [29:21].
If there is no obvious contact between the two objects, adpositions 108
expressing various local functions are used (they govern the locative case form), cf.
ymiNqal tynupy-k ‘behind a hill’, ujatiki-k gytulNyNqal ‘at the side of a sledge’,
rymetyNqal tynupy-k ‘far behind a hill’, inmy-k telyNqal ‘on the other side of a rock’,
tilirra-k semyk ‘near Tilirran {the village Tilichiki}’.
Number sg du pl
Person
1 -jgym -muri -muru
2 -jgyt -turi -turu
3 = NOM+SG = NOM+DU =NOM+PL
108
These auxiliary words can be used both prepositionally and postpositionally (though the
latter is more common). We refer to them as adpositions and use this term in the Dictionary
(adpostions are marked with the abbreviation adp). Some Alutor adpositions consist of two parts:
the main part (the root) and the suffix with a local function, cf. tely.Nqal ‘on that side of’, with the
root {tely} ‘there, that’ and the suffix {-Nqal} meaning ‘the side of’.
109
For the 3rd person see 3.3.
102
b. ... mur-uwwi [email protected]@y-muru.
we-NOM+PL male.cousin-1pl
{LC: This is sinful, we won't kill you, because} we are cousins. [22:38]
c. Nav=HytHy-turu, Hymul@y-turu ...
female=dog-2pl weakling-2pl
You bitches, you weaklings, {RC: you, oh, you couldn't kill me!} [17:23]
Example (80c) shows that the S-actant itself may be absent, but it still controls
the agreement of the predicate.
The predicative is also used as an apposition for another noun or pronoun (the
pronoun itself may be absent).
103
information (for details see 3.1.4).
Nouns are incorporated by other nouns when they are used attributively in the
underlying structure and denote non-specific objects, for instance: tanNy=@irra-n
‘the enemy party’ [21:10], Hasu=nym.jyr@y-n ‘a village of pink salmon’ [3:21],
viHe=rara-Na ‘a grass house’ [6:16], wir=imyt ‘the load of alder bark’ [7:8]. Not all
linguists consider this type of compounding incorporation, but it is obviously a highly
productive type of compounding.
2.4. Pronouns
2.4.1. Personal pronouns
Free personal pronouns for the 1st, 2nd and 3rd person, singular, dual and plural,
occur in Alutor. The 3rd person singular does not distinguish between either human/
non-human or animate/ non-animate. Substantivized demonstratives are also used as
3rd person pronouns (see below).
Dual and plural pronouns are derived from one and the same stem and differ
only in the nominative and sometimes in the ergative, in other oblique cases one and
the same form is used for each case (in the ergative the difference between the dual
and plural can be marked through verbal agreement). Thus, there are 9 pronouns, but
only 6 pronominal stems: {gym} ‘I’ (NOM gymmy), {gyt} ‘you (sg)’ (NOM gytty),
{yn-/y-} 110 ‘(s)he/it’ (NOM ynnu), {mur} ‘we’ (NOM muri ‘we two’, NOM
muru(wwi) ‘we’), {tur} ‘you (nsg)’ (NOM turi ‘you two’, NOM turu(wwi) ‘you
(pl)’)’, {yt} ‘they’ (NOM ytti ‘they two’, NOM ytuwwi ‘they’).
Personal pronouns have much in common with nominal forms of the noun: they
are used in the same syntactic positions and are marked for case. But, unlike nouns,
personal pronouns are not incorporated, they are used only as autonomous words.
Adjectives derived from pronominal stems and similar to adjectival forms of the noun
belong to another group of pronouns − possessive pronouns.
Alutor personal pronouns have approximately the same set of cases as nouns.
10 cases are distinguished: n o m i t a t i v e , e r g a t i v e , l o c a t i v e , d a t i v e ,
prolative, contactive, causal, equative, comitative, associative.
As for the Lative, its pronominal form is likely to coincide with the dative form, so it
is not represented in the list of cases.
There is some divergence from the case system of nouns as regards the
realization of different cases.
The nominative singular is marked with a zero morph; in the 1st and 2nd person
110
The morph y- is usually used before markers beginning with n-.
104
an additional syllable appears (see 1.3.4.3), in the 3rd person a suppletive stem is
used. The nominative dual is marked with -i ; the same form is optionally used as the
ergative dual (this marker does not occur with nouns). The nominative plural is
marked with -u(wwi). All ergative forms (including dual − optionally) are marked
with the special pronominal marker -nan.
In some case forms special suffixes occur; that have no meaning, their only
function is to “extend” a pronominal stem. They are: -yk, -kka, -ula, -g 111 . In some
plural case forms (equative, comitative and associative) possessive pronoun stems are
used (see 2.4.2). The complete paradigms of personal pronouns are given in Table 20.
Singular pronouns
Person 1 2 3
Case
1 nominative gymmy gytty ynnu
2 ergative gym-nan gy-nanny y-nanny
3 locative gymy-kki gyny-kki yny-kki
4 dative gym-yky-N gyn-yky-N yn-yky-N
5 prolative gymy-kk-epyN gyny-kk-epyN yny-kk-epyN
6 contactive gymy-kka-jit(a) gyny-kka-jit(a) yn-ykka-jit(a)
7 causal gymy-kka-kjit(a) gyny-kka-kjit(a) yn-ykka-kjit(a)
8 equative gymm-ula-nu gytt-ula-nu ynn-ula-nu
9 comitative awyn-gymm-ula-ma awyn-gytt-ula-ma awyn-@ynn-ula-ma
10 associative geqy-gymm-ula-ta geqy-gytt-ula-ta geqy-@ynn-ula-ta
111
In the glossed texts these suffixes are treated as parts of pronominal stems (see Part I).
105
Non-singular (dual and plural) pronouns
Person 1 2 3
Case
1 nominative mur-i (du) tur-i (du) ytt-i (du)
mur-uwwi (pl) tur-uwwi (pl) yt-uwwi (pl)
2 ergative mur-i (du) ~ tur-i (du) ~ ytt-i (du) ~
mur-gy-nan (du) / tur-gy-nan (du) / yt-gy-nan (du) /
mur-gy-nan (pl) tur-gy-nan (pl) yt-gy-nan (pl)
3 locative mury-kki tury-kki yty-kki
4 dative mur-yky-N tur-yky-N yt-yky-N
5 prolative mury-kk-epyN tury-kk-epyN yty-kk-epyN
6 contactive mury-kka-jit(a) tury-kka-jit(a) yty-kka-jit(a)
7 causal mury-kka-kjit(a) tury-kka-kjit(a) yty-kka-kjit(a)
8 equative mur-gina-nu tur-gina-nu yt-gina-nu
9 comitative awyn-mur-gina-ma awyn-tur-gina-ma awyn-@yt-gina-ma
10 associative geqy-mur-gina-ta geqy-tur-gina-ta geqy-@yt-gina-ta
106
TABLE 21. Possessive pronouns
Person
autonomous incorporated autonomous incorporated
1 gym-nin(a)- gym-yk- mur-gin(a)- mur-yk-
2 gy-nin(a)- gyn-yk- tur-gin(a)- tur-yk-
3 y-nin(a)- yn-yk- yt-gin(a)- yt-yk-
Person
autonomous incorporated autonomous incorporated
1 gymy-kka- gymy-kka- mury-kka- mury-kka-
kin(a)- kin(a)- kin(a)- kin(a)-
2 gyny-kka-kin(a)- gyny-kka-kin(a)- tury-kka-kin(a)- tury-kka-kin(a)-
3 yty-kka-kin(a)- yty-kka-kin(a)- yty-kka-kin(a)- yty-kka-kin(a)-
Number sg du pl
Person
1 -jgym -muri -muru
2 -jgyt -turi -turu
3 [the vowel -a -t -w(wi)
truncation]
COMMENTS on Table 22
The 3rd person possessive pronouns may have an additional syllable after a stressed vowel
(see 1.3.4.3), cf. gynønny ‘your (sg)’, ynønny ‘his/her/its’, but gymnin ‘my’, murgin ‘our’, turgin
107
‘your (nsg)’, ytgin ‘their’.
112
Some demonstratives may be used as bare roots, without any suffixes. In such cases they
are referred to as demonstrative particles, cf. Nan ‘there’ (particle) – Nanin ‘that’ (pronominal
adjective).
113
In the Dictionary case-number forms of the 1st and 2nd declension are marked with
“human” or “non-human” respectively.
108
Besides demonstrative adjectives, there are also demonstrative adverbs derived
from the same roots: wutku ‘here’, wutkepyN ‘from here’, Nanyk ‘there’, igynNinak
‘that is why’ and others. In order to express great distance from the speaker and the
hearer the iconic vowel lengthening is used (see 1.3.8.1), for instance, instead of
Nanyk ‘there’ the form NOnyk ‘far over there’ is often used.
(84) mik-igyt?
who-2sg
Who are you? [5:12]
When the pronoun {taq} ‘what’ is used attributively in the underlying structure,
it is obligatorily incorporated; in this case it means ‘what; what kind of’ 114 . Compare:
(85) taq=Nav.@an-igyt?
what=wife-2sg
(It's not important) whose wife you are. [13:10]
A pronoun similar to the adjectival form is found only for the pronoun ‘who’ −
it is the possessive pronoun mik.in(a)- ‘whose’ 115 . It is used like any other
possessive adjective marked with -in(a) and it may be substantivized like any other
pronominal adjective.
114
Cf. the English pronoun what used attributively in phrases like what book?.
115
Actually, there exists an interrogative pronoun derived from the root {taq} by means of
the suffix -in(a), that is the pronoun {taq.in(a)}. However, it is used only to denote the object of
certain verbs of speech such as ‘say, speak (about smth.)’ in an interrogative sentence, cf. taq.in
ynN.ina iv-la-t? ‘What did they say?’ [10:48]
109
Comment: V etom fajle
okonchatel’nyj vid glavy. Vse
CHAPTER 3. Syntax ispravlenija ubrany. Dlja kontrolja
est’fail KI_SYNT_w91, v
kotorom ispravlenija pokazany.
Esli vozniknet
In this chapter we shall briefly, without any ambitions to completeness, consider neobxodimost’chto-libo izmenit’,
pokazhite mne.
some of the most important topics of clause structure.
116
In Alutor there are many ambient verbs with a zero S-argument indicating ‘environment
of the event’:
(i) mury-kki wutku lyq.laN-ki a-qajav-ka, agigatka
we-LOC here winter-LOC A-freeze-PRED` very
wyjal.aty-tkyn, kytig.aty-tkyn.
be(of.snowstorm)-IPF wind.blow-IPF
Here it’s cold in winter. There are strong snowstorms and wind. [38:1]
(ii) imjaq Hitu-ru@-i.
already goose-fly.in-3sg.S
Geese have already flown in.
110
marked with the ergative and controls the prefix agreement slot in polypersonal
conjugation; the P-argument is marked with the nominative and controls the suffix
agreement slot in polypersonal conjugation. In monopersonal conjugation agreement
is controlled by the P-argument.
I n t r a n s i t i v e verbs have one core S-argument marked with the nominative
that controls the suffix agreement slot in monopersonal conjugation and both
agreement slots in polypersonal conjugation.
From the point of view of case marking Alutor uses the e r g a t i v e
alignment 117 , unifying the S/P-arguments (with the Absolutive hyperrole) and
opposing them to the A-argument. Monopersonal conjugation is analogous (the
Absolutive is the agreement controller). An interpretation of polypersonal conjugation
is more controversial. Namely, the S-, A- and P-arguments use different sets of
controlled agreement slots: The S-arguments control the prefix as well as suffix slots,
the A-arguments control the prefix slots and the P-arguments control the suffix slots.
Therefore, from the point of view of polypersonal conjugation the t r i p a r t i t e
alignment is realized. From the point of view of the marker's material form, the prefix
markers bring the A- and S-arguments in line with t h e a c c u s a t i v e alignment
(with the Principal hyperrole), opposing them to the P-argument; see 2.1.1.2.
In the suffix slots the sets of S- and P-markers are different, therefore the suffix
slot realizes the tripartite alignment opposing the S/A/P-arguments.
In Alutor there are different means of including a verb's arguments into the set
of core arguments as well as excluding them from the set of core arguments. The first
process is called i n c r e a s e , and the second one d e c r e a s e of their syntactic status.
These processes often take place simultaneously as the status of some argument
increases while the status of another one decreases.
From the point of view of the increase/decrease of the syntactic status, the
nominative position is the most important. This position is always present in a clause
(it can lack material form in the context of ambient verbs or in case of ellipsis). It is
the target of all increasing processes. Therefore, the Absolutive hyperrole can
correspond to different primary semantic roles. In contrast to the nominative position,
the ergative position is stable from the point of view of its role identification: it
merges the agent and experiencer in the Agentive hyperrole. Almost always, it is the
initial position of the government pattern of the respective verb 118 . The only exception
117
For a more detailed discussion of the typology of syntactic constructions and the
corresponding hyperroles see [Kibrik 1997a].
118
This position can be added in the case of causativization, however in this case it is a
derivational process producing a new lexeme rather that a diathesis change.
111
is when an intransitive verb with an agent in the S-position becomes transitive (see
below). In this case the syntactic status of the agent argument decreases. This position
cannot be the target of an increasing process.
Devices that combine increase and decrease are the following: varying
government pattern, antipassivization, incorporation, and causativization (with
preservation of the number of arguments). Autonomous increase of syntactic status
takes place in case of agreement controlled by a dative NP and in case of
causativization of intransitive verbs: in the latter case the verb receives the second
core Agentive position. Let us consider these devices in more detail.
In (1a) the only agentive argument, ‘I’, is the core one, while in (1b) both NPs
are the core arguments. In the latter case the goal ‘he’ is included in the number of
core arguments. The government pattern of the verb gala- ‘pass by’ is analogous. In
intransitive usage it has a peripheral place argument:
The commonly used verb iv- ‘say’ has two government patterns: <smb.: N, to
smb.: D, smth.: Scompl >; <smb.: E, to smb.: N, smth.: Scompl >:
112
(3) a. qutkinJnJaqu ynny-N iv-i...
Qutkinnaqu+NOM+SG fish-DAT say-3sg.S+PF
Qutkinnaqu said to the fish... [1:11]
The case marking of arguments in (3b) can be simply worked out with the help of
verbal agreement.
3.1.2. Lability
Labile verbs license one-place as well as two-place government patterns. The first
realizes an intransitive and the latter a transitive usage without the addition of any
markers to the verb. In Alutor both labile schemes are present, A-lability preserving
the agentive argument in both patterns as well as P-lability preserving the patientive
argument.
T h e À - l a b i l i t y results in increase / decrease 119 of the status of the
patientive argument. In the transitive government pattern the patient occupies the
nominative position while in the intransitive pattern the patient is unspecified or is
absent from the government pattern of the verb:
b. ...gymmy ty-tivla-tkyn.
I-NOM 1sg.S-beat.with.stick-IPF
{LC: Before (I) come into the house,} I beat (my clothing) with a stick.
The following verbs are similar: [email protected] ‘start fishing with a hook, with a
rod’ <smb.: N>, <smb.: E, smth.: N>; alJlJy.tku- ‘remove a superficial layer (peel a
potato, skin a fish, shave a tree, process a skin, scrape)’ <smb.: N>, <smb.: E, smth.:
N>. Interesting in this respect is a certain Alutor verb derived from the interrogative
pronoun taq ‘what’, taq- ‘what to do’ <smb.: N>, < smb.: E, to smb.: N>:
119
Depending on which government pattern is accepted as the primary one.
113
Sometimes do something!
b. ...mik-ynak yn-taqy-na?
who-ERG+SG LOW.A+OPT-what.to.do-3pl.P
{LC: Skis are in their own place} . Who will do anything to them? [13:31]
In (5a) the verb has intransitive conjugation, while in (5b) it has transitive
conjugation with an overt ergative A-argument.
P - l a b i l i t y licenses the presence/absence of an Agentive argument in a
government pattern in which the nominative patient of the verb is preserved. For
example the verb tku- is P-labile: ‘be finished’ <smth.: N>; ‘finish, wipe out’ <smb.:
E, smb/smth.: N>’. Derivational relations of the verb ujp- are more complicated: ‘be
cut, be pricked, thrust a splinter (into)’ <smb.: N; with smth.: E>; ‘stick, string’
<smb.: E, smth.: N; in(to) smth: L>.
3.1.3. Antipassive
3.1.3.1. Antipassive syntax
The increase of syntactic status is possible in the context of multi-place verbs because
the single argument of a one-place verb initially occupies the highest position. The
increase is marked with the antipassive marker ina-, identical to the ‘1sg.P’
114
agreement marker (see 2.1.1.2.3) 120 . It most frequently increases the status of the A-
argument of a transitive verb:
120
As claimed above, the general function of this morpheme is to mark an unusual filling of
a specific syntactic position; with this function in mind this marker is called the inversion marker.
121
In Alutor the ergative case syncretically marks a core Agentive argument or a peripheral
argument initially bearing the instrument semantic role or indicating a demoted patient.
122
This verb tends to have the suffix -at added in case of antipassivization.
115
He drove the boat to the coast.
b. y-nanny argiN inJa-nJsem.avy-tky-ni-n Hytyv-a.
he-ERG coast+NOM ANTI- draw.nearer-IPF-3sg.A+3P-3sg.P boat-ERG
He drives the boat to the coast.
116
(12) a. ...ina-tg.aty-tkyn tatulJ-pilJ sasusaNawyt.
ANTI-dig-IPF fox-DIMIN+NOM+SG Sasusangawet
{LC: Later she saw, not far away} a vixen (named) Sasusangawet {=“fox-
woman”}(who) was digging. [11:2]
At the same time, it is important to note that in Alutor there are s y n t a c t i c contexts
favoring the antipassive. Thus in a relative clause the deleted coreferential NP should
be marked with the nominative. If in a primary diathesis this NP is marked with an
oblique case, antipassivization is necessary:
In (13) the verb t/n.pynkyt.at- is the causative from the verb pynkyt- ‘get
stuck’, it has the government pattern <smb.: E; smth.: N; in smth.: L>. In the primary
structure of the relative clause the coreferential NP ‘enemy’ takes the locative form,
therefore antipassivization takes place. In (14) the relativized NP ‘friends’ is marked
in its primary diathesis with the ergative, so the antipassive is used.
The usage of the antipassive has not been exhaustively investigated. It is
relatively rare in the texts, however the tendency for the syntactic usage of the
nominative is attested, i.e. the tendency towards syntactic ergativity in Alutor; see
[Kibrik 1979].
117
3.1.4. Incorporation
3.1.4.1. Syntax of incorporation
Syntactically incorporation can be described as a process resulting in some
constituent (t a r g e t o f i n c o r p o r a t i o n ) being deprived of its independent
syntactic position and being included in the stem of a word that immediately governs
it (g o a l o f i n c o r p o r a t i o n ). Incorporation does not obligatorily entail the
change of the argument structure, its functions are more general, therefore in this
section it is reasonable to consider this process from the point of view of its syntactic
potential in general.
The goal of incorporation can be the head of a VP (the verb) or of a NP (the
noun). As a result v e r b a l and n o m i n a l compounds 123 appear. Any modifiers of
VPs and NPs can be the targets of incorporation. They can belong to different word
classes.
The types of targets will be considered separately for VPs and NPs.
3.1.4.1.1. VP incorporation
Incorporation of the P-argument is most widespread. In this case the verb usually
becomes intransitive, while its initial A-argument becomes the S-argument:
In (15a) the transitive verb ‘go around’ becomes intransitive and agrees in non-
singular with the contextually retrievable S-argument ‘dogs’. In (15b) there is an
overt S-argument keNyn ‘bear’ of the intransitivized verb ‘open’. The benefactive
preserves its marking as a peripheral argument. However, it is not obligatory. In the
context of patient incorporation a benefactive can be promoted to the nominative
position, transitivity of the verb being preserved:
123
Such insight on incorporation is widely presented in the works on Chukotko-Kamchatkan
languages, cf. [Skorik 1948; 1977; Zhukova 1953; 1972; Muravyova 1989b; 1990b].
118
(16) mitE, qy-milgy=nna-gy-na Navy.sq.e.
Miti+NOM+SG 2.A+OPT-fire=carry.out-2.A+3.P-3pl.P girl+NOM+PL
Miti, carry out the fire for the girls. [8:62]
The verb ir- ‘hit accidentally; come across’ is a two-place intransitive predicate:
<smb./smth.: N, smth.: L>. Under incorporation of the initial S-argument the vacated
position is filled by a contextually retrieved locative argument (‘great-grandfather’).
Incorporation is a widespread process in the case when an S/P-argument is a
possessive NP. In this case the head of such a NP is incorporated, the possessor being
raised:
b. to Horo ga-qmi=tku-laN...
and after.that RES-arrow=finish-RES+3pl.S
But then they ran out of (their) arrows [=they arrow-finished], {RC: and then they
began to fight with spears.} [21:13]
119
b. ... ga-la@u-lin syge=pitqy-lJ@y-n.
RES-see-RES+3sg.P sand=hide-ATR-NOM+SG
{LC: In the morning people woke up, and} found (the man) hidden in the sands.
[18:19]
3.1.4.1.2. NP incorporation
The head noun of a NP as the goal of incorporation can be marked with the
nominative or an oblique case. In case of a head noun in the nominative incorporation
is optional, while in case of a head noun in an oblique case incorporation is obligatory
(in other words, noun phrases marked with an oblique case cannot have free
modifiers).
Different head noun modifiers can be the targets of NP incorporation. First,
adjectives, determiners, and numerals can be the targets of incorporation.
c. Nyru.n=nym.jyr@y-N myn-@y-ju@y-na
three=village-ADV 1nsg.A+CONJ-CONJ-reach-3pl.P
HytHy.mty.l@-u...
120
Karaga.person-NOM+PL
If only (our) three villages could overtake the Karaga people... [19:6]
121
decreases the syntactic rank of constituents that are out of the scope of assertion and,
more generally, beyond the focus of attention 124 . If incorporation affects the
constituent that occupies a prestigious position in a clause, it opens the possibility of
raising the status of another constituent occupying a peripheral position in the initial
structure.
Along with this, it is possible to confirm the usage of incorporation for the
purpose of overcoming syntactic restrictions over syntactic structure. For example, -
l@- participles form reduced relative clauses whose nominative argument is
coreferential with the head NP. With respect to this restriction it is sometimes
necessary to free the nominative position, incorporating the P-argument of a transitive
verb:
3.1.5. Causativization
Causativization is a standard means of adding an agentive argument to a verb's
semantic valence. In this case an intransitive verb becomes transitive. In Alutor this
124
For details see [Muravyova 1990b].
122
type of causative derivation is a very productive means of increasing transitivity.
Besides this, a causative derivate can heighten the status of a peripheral argument
without adding a new argument. For example, the intransitive verb ‘walk around’
<smb.: N, smth.: L> becomes transitive under causativization, cf.:
In (27b) the causative verb re-interprets the initial peripheral argument ‘house’
as the Absolutive. Initial S-argument receives an iconic agentive marking in the
ergative case.
3.1.6. Dative agreement
Transitive verbs usually agree with A- and P-arguments. However a personal
recipient/addressee of a three-place verb can “usurp” control over the P-marker:
123
ga-la@u-lin ynny-@yn.
RES-see-RES+3sg.P fish-NOM+SG
One day Qutkinnaqu found [=saw] a fish somewhere. [1:2]
In nominal clauses with adjectival predicates 125 both word order structures —
SV and VS — are possible:
125
As mentioned in 3.3.3, the adjective in these clauses is in fact substantivized.
124
(32) a. ga-jun.aty-lqiv kuuty-k ny-katgu-qin Hujamtawil@y-n.
RES-live-LQIV Кuut-LOC ADJ-strong-ADJ+3sg person-NOM+SG
(There was) a strong person {i.e. Emqa} (who) lived on (the island) Kuut. [21:1]
b. Navy.sNy-n ny-tur-qin...
woman-NOM+SG ADJ-young-ADJ+3sg
A young woman {RC: prepared to give birth to a child and cried in the tundra.} [15:3]
125
3.2.3. The Sentence
The order of the main and subordinate clauses is not fixed. Besides, the embedding of
parts of the subordinate clause into the main clause is possible:
Since in nominal clauses this type of verb is not used as an auxiliary (see
below), there are grounds to consider existential clauses verbal.
126
qajylJHinny-kin.
[Qayelinne-REL+3sg]
{LC: Once there lived an old man from Alut {the village Alutorka} called
Qutawweyngen and his wife Kemlingawet.} (And) Qutawweyngen's brother
Aqaningvit, (was) from Qayelinnen {the village Khayilino}. [30:2]
In (37a) the order of NPs is SV, in (37b) it is inversed: VS. The nominal
argument can be expressed as a demonstrative/deictic pronoun wuttin ‘this’:
The head of a predicate NP is marked with the predicative form of a noun (see
2.3.2), namely the overt personal markers controlled by 1st/2nd person S-arguments:
127
b. y.ninny unJunJu qur=etaty-l@-u ity-tkyn.
his/her+3sg child+NOM+SG reindeer=drive-ATR-EQUAT be-IPF
...his son was a reindeer drover. [16:11]
c. ...aNaNy.l@-u t-ity-lqi.
shaman-EQUAT POT-be-LQIV+PF
{LC: If you step over me along the waist,} you will become a shaman. [24:12]
b. tyrup=tyrgy-tyr n-alJlJo-qin,
Teruppe=meat-NOM+SG ADJ-sweet-ADJ+3sg
num kiwyl n-alJlJo-qin.
again blood+NOM+SG ADJ-sweet-ADJ+3sg
Teruppe's meat (was) sweet. (Its) blood (was) also sweet. [10:3]
Such clauses confirm that if a generic noun is missing (see examples (41a-b)),
the adjective is substantivized.
In the predicate position an adjective can be complicated by the degree marker
or marked as the caritive or predicative:
126
This noun historically goes back to a participle as well.
128
Qutawweyngen is a deceiver... [30:35]
129
3.3.4. Identification clauses
Identification clauses have the same structure as other nominal clauses: they consist
of two adjacent NPs being in a relation of referential identity. The auxiliary verb it-
‘be, be found’ marked with the resultative can also be used:
127
This word is also used in comparative constructions.
128
Square brackets in the interlinear translation line mark the boundaries of a relative clause.
130
qynut maN.ina Nytu.gyrNy-k ina-nJnJu-l@-u ...
[that which+3pl entrance-LOC ANTI-watch-ATR-NOM+PL]
Only my friends, those who were watching over the exit, {RC: they ended up spearing
each other.} [21:108]
When the relative clause is in preposition, the following main clause often has
the conjunction to ‘and’; see (49).
The full coreferential NP can be found in the subordinate clause, the main
clause containing the correlative pronoun Nanin :
This type of participial construction has the target of relativization in the main
clause; compare the similar structure of the finite relative clause (3.4.2.2).
131
clauses. The main as well as the dependent NP can be the target of relativization.
129
The main NP can be accompanied by the correlative pronoun ‘that/there’.
132
t-il-la-tkyni-tki turgy-nan.
POT-be-PLUR-IPF-2nsg.A+3.P you-ERG
{LC: and you have another big concern;} you should sort things out with those who
killed your cousins. [22:73]
133
material markers of a relative clause 130 :
In (54) the head NP ynN.ina ‘these’ is marked with the nominative and is
governed by the transitive verb ‘walk in wooden masks around smb.’, wereas a
deleted coreferential dependent NP would be expected to bear the ergative.
130
This case is similar to clause union (see 4.5).
134
turgy-nan ligi lyN-la-tkyni-tki nuta.lJqy-n
you-ERG know know-PLUR-IPF-2nsg.A+3.P [land-NOM+SG
ny-mal-qin kyNav.sity.nv-u java-k,
ADJ-good-ADJ+3sg place.for.wrestling-EQUAT use-INF]]
to Nany.k q-u@al-la-tyk gymyky-N.
and there 2.S+OPT-wait-PLUR-2nsg.S I-DAT
...(and) for the moment go to the place, which you know (as) a good place for fighting,
and there wait for me. [22:33]
Initially inside the relative clause there is a sentential complement (‘[you know
[which place is a good one for fighting]]’) with the NP ‘which place’ coreferential
with the main NP.
The finite technique of relative clause formation favors variation of the target of
relativization: it is easy to see the similarity between the two opposed targets of
relativization (main vs. dependent NPs) in case of unspecified main NPs, cf. (56) and
(53).
135
[email protected] jypa-ni-n.
RES-fill-RES+3sg.P take.off-3sg.A+3P-3sg.P
In any case, (she) took off (these trousers), filled with pieces of turf. [14:41]
In (57a) two clauses are united: ‘he had seen their sister’ and ‘they had loaded
their sister on the sledge’ sharing the common argument ‘sister’, also in (57b) —
‘they left Sisisen there’ and ‘they tied up Sisisen’ share the common argument
‘Sisisen’, and in (57c) the common argument ‘trousers’ is presupposed: ‘she took off
(these trousers)’ and ‘somebody filled the trousers with pieces of turf’. It is clear that
in all cases at least one predicate is marked with the marker ga-...-lin.
In (58) clause union is most likely motivated by the inability of the situation to be
semantically divisible (take off and give).
136
falsely died by the fire]. [2:24]
In (61a) the second NP is deleted (anaphoric reduction), while in (61b) the first one is
deleted (cataphoric reduction).
In case of pronominalization or deletion a specific situation can arise if the
clause holding the controlling NP is transitive: what argument of the clause controls
pronominalization/deletion of the reduced NP in the other clause? Statistically the A-
argument prevails, however it is far from being an obligatory rule:
137
(63) to Nan.in-yn ga-pry-lin wamylka-lNyn, to
and that-POSS.SG+3sg RES-take.off-RES+3sg.P lip-NOM+SG and
ally tinga a-n.ilu-ka ga-nty-lin.
not what A-move-PRED` RES-do-RES+3sg.P
And (he) cut [=took away] his lip and (he) did not move at all. [20:40]
3.7. Reflexivization
In Alutor argument reflexivization (affecting the argument position) is expressed with
the help of the reflexive pronoun uvik ‘oneself’ (lit. ‘body’), and possessive
reflexivization (affecting the possessive position) uses the pronoun sininkin/sinin
‘one's own’ (the second variant is used in case of incorporation).
The question of control of reflexivization is of special interest. According to the
field report by F.A.Antsiferov (1978), the A/S-argument is the controller of
reflexivization:
138
is the A-argument marked with the ergative, because in the context of incorporation
of the P-argument ‘nose’ its possessor is raised into the opened P-position. In (65c) S-
argument controls reflexivization of the dative argument.
Possessive reflexivization allows the A- and P-controllers:
131
Such a technique is used in case of argument reflexivization as well:
139
140