History of Intelligence Focus: How Did Galton and Binet Differ in Their Approaches To Measuring Mental Abilities
History of Intelligence Focus: How Did Galton and Binet Differ in Their Approaches To Measuring Mental Abilities
Focus: How did Galton and Binet differ in their approaches to measuring mental abilities.
Historically, two scientists played seminal roles in the study and measurement of mental skills:
Francis Galton and Alfred Binet
Galton’s research convinced him that important people had “inherited mental
constitutions” that made them more fit for thinking than their less successful
counterparts.
Exhibiting his own belief bias, Galton dismissed the fact that the more successful people
he studied almost invariably came from privileged environments. For example: Students
studying in IIT, IIM’S are those who can pay for coaching classes.
Galton then attempted to demonstrate a biological basis for intelligence by showing that
people who were more socially and occupationally successful would also perform better
on a variety of laboratory tasks by measuring the “efficiency of the nervous system.”
He developed measures of reaction speed, hand strength, and sensory acuity. He even
measured the size of people’s skulls, believing that skull size reflected brain volume and
hence intelligence.
Galton’s approach to mental-skills measurement fell into disfavor because his measures
of nervous-system efficiency proved unrelated to socially relevant measures of mental
ability, such as academic and occupational success.
The concept of mental age was subsequently expanded by the German psychologist
William Stern to provide a relative score—a common yardstick of intellectual attainment
—for people of different chronological ages. Stern’s intelligence quotient (IQ) was the
ratio of mental age to chronological age, multiplied by 100:
Thus, a child who was performing at exactly his or her age level would have an IQ of
100. In our previous example, the child with a mental age of 10 and a chronological age
of 8 would have an IQ of (10/8) X 100 = 125.
A 16-year-old with a mental age of 20 would also have an IQ of 125, so the two would be
comparable in intelligence even though their ages differed.
Today’s intelligence tests provide an “IQ” score that is not a quotient at all. Instead, it is
based on a person’s performance relative to the scores of other people the same age, with
a score of 100 corresponding to the average performance of that age group.
Weschler
What was Wechsler’s concept of intelligence? How do the Wechsler scales reflect this concept?
Two decades after, Psychologist David Wechsler developed a major competitor to the
Stanford-Binet. Wechsler believed that the Stanford- Binet relied too much on verbal
skills. He thought that intelligence should be measured as a group of distinct but related
verbal and nonverbal abilities. He therefore developed intelligence tests for adults and for
children that measured both verbal and nonverbal intellectual skills.
In 1939 the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) appeared, followed by the
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) in 1955 and the Wechsler Preschool
and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI) in 1967. Today, the Wechsler tests (WAIS-
III and WISC-IV) are the most popular individually administered
Example of test in Weschler :
http://brainscale.net/memory-span/training
Definition of intelligence :
What is our working definition of intelligence?
Important to remember being smart is typically thought of as having good mental skills
that are instrumental to succeeding in school and in higher-level jobs and occupations. As
we shall see, people with good mental skills do indeed do better in school and on the job
in our culture. But if we view intelligence in broader perspective: as the ability to respond
adaptively to the demands of a particular environment. It’s important to remember, then,
that intelligence is not something that has concrete existence; it is, instead, a socially
constructed concept. For example: Street children are good at selling and in mathematics
despite not having a formal education. Street vendors are good at managing their business
despite having a formal business degree.
Definition of Intelligence is the ability to acquire knowledge, to think and reason effectively, and
to deal adaptively with the environment.
The nature of intelligence
Intelligence is hard to define
Psychologists have used two major approaches in the study of intelligence:
The psychometric approach attempts to map the structure of intellect and to discover the
kinds of mental competencies that underlie test performance.
The cognitive processes approach studies the specific thought processes that underlie
those mental competencies.
Psychometric
Factor Analysis:
How is factor analysis used in the study of intelligence?
G-Factor Theory :
What kinds of evidence supported the existence of Spearman’s g factor?
The psychometric argument for intelligence as a general ability was first advanced by the
British psychologist Charles Spearman (1923). He observed that school grades in
different subjects such as English and mathematics, were almost always positively
correlated but not perfectly
That number of different cognitive and intellectual task measures tend to be correlated
with one another that is people who score high on one tend to score high on the others as
well.
Spearman concluded that intellectual performance is determined partly by a g factor, or
general intelligence, and partly by whatever special abilities might be required to perform
that particular task.
Spearman contended that because the general factor—the g factor—cuts across virtually
all tasks, it constitutes the core of intelligence. Thus, Spearman would argue that your
performance in a mathematics course would depend mainly on your general intelligence
but also on your specific ability to learn mathematics.
Today, many theorists continue to believe that the g factor is the core of what we call
intelligence.
Moreover, g matters a great deal as a predictor of both academic and job performance.
Spearmen had the notion that intelligence can be largely summed up with one score.
In contrast to Spearmen, several theorist have concluded that intelligence has multiple
components.
Spearman’s conclusion about the centrality of the g factor was soon challenged by L. L.
Thurstone therefore concluded that human mental performance depends not on a general
factor but rather on seven distinct abilities, which he called primary mental abilities
They included verbal comprehension, word fluency, perceptual speed memory, numerical
ability, spatial ability, and reasoning. Thurstone assembled a battery of test to measure
these abilities.
A multifactor theory more complex than Thurstone was proposed by Guildford. This
three dimensional theory suggest that there are 120 factors of intelligence that can be
divided into three dimension
Operations dimension
SI includes six operations or general intellectual processes:
Content dimension
SI includes five broad areas of information to which the human intellect applies the six
operations:
Product dimension
As the name suggests, this dimension contains results of applying particular operations to
specific contents. The SI model includes six products, in increasing complexity:
Guildford also suggested that intelligence involves divergent thinking that is being creative or
original in problem solving rather than convergent thinking that is trying to solve only one
correct answer for a problem. For example: in exam we have to give one correct answer.
Hierarchical theory:
Focus question: Differentiate between crystallized and fluid intelligence and indicate
their relation to aging and types of memory.
Raymond Cattell (1971) and John Horn (1985) proposed a new model of intelligence
Crystallized intelligence is the ability to apply previously acquired knowledge to current
problems. Vocabulary and information tests are good measures of crystallized
intelligence.
Crystallized intelligence, which is the basis for expertise, depends on the ability
to retrieve previously learned information and problem-solving schemas from long-term
memory. It is dependent on previous learning and practice.
Cattell and Horn’s second general factor is fluid intelligence, defined as the ability to
deal with novel problem-solving situations for which personal experience does not
provide a solution. It involves creative problem solving skills.
Fluid intelligence is dependent primarily on the efficient functioning of the central
nervous system rather than on prior experience and cultural context.
Fluid intelligence requires the abilities to reason abstractly, think logically, and manage
information in working (short-term) memory so that new problems can be solved.
Thus, long-term memory contributes strongly to crystallized intelligence, whereas fluid
intelligence is particularly dependent on efficient working memory.
Cattell and Horn concluded that over our life span, we progress from using fluid
intelligence to depending more on crystallized intelligence. Early in life, we encounter
many problems for the first time, so we need fluid intelligence to figure out solutions
makes us more knowledgeable, we have less need to approach each situation as a new
problem. Instead, we simply call up appropriate information and schemas from long-term
memory, thereby utilizing our crystallized intelligence. This is the essence of wisdom.
Because long-term memory remains strong even as we age, performance on tests of
crystallized intelligence improves during adulthood and remains stable well into late
adulthood. In contrast, performance on tests of fluid intelligence begins to decline as
people enter late adulthood.
The fact that aging affects the two forms of intelligence differently is additional evidence
that they represent different classes of mental abilities.
Sternberg believes that there is more than one kind of intelligence. He suggests that
environmental demands may call for three different classes of adaptive problem solving
and that people differ in their intellectual strengths in these areas:
1. Analytical intelligence involves the kinds of academically oriented problem-solving skills
measured by traditional intelligence tests.
2. Practical intelligence refers to the skills needed to cope with everyday demands and to manage
oneself and other people effectively.
3. Creative intelligence comprises the mental skills needed to deal adaptively with novel
problems.
For example: Sternberg found that Brazilian street children were very proficient at the math
required to carry on their street businesses, despite the fact that many of them had failed
mathematics in school.
Sternberg believes that educational programs should teach all three classes of skills, not just
analytical-academic skills. In studies with elementary school children, he and his colleagues have
shown that a curriculum that also teaches practical and creative skills results in greater mastery
of course material than does a traditional analytic, memory-based approach to learning course
content
Mental competencies
Some psychologists think this is too limited a definition to capture the range of human
adaptations. They believe that intelligence may be more broadly conceived as relatively
independent intelligences that relate to different adaptive demands.
In recent writings, Gardner (2000) has also speculated about a ninth possible intelligence, which
he calls existential intelligence, a philosophically oriented ability to ponder questions about the
meaning of one’s existence, life, and death. Gardner’s first three intelligences are measured
by existing intelligence tests, but the others are not. Indeed, some of Gardner’s critics insist
that these other abilities are not really part of the traditional concept of intelligence at all and that
some of them are better regarded as talents. However, Gardner replies that the form of
intelligence that is most highly valued within a given culture depends on the adaptive
requirements of that culture
Gardner further suggests that these different classes of abilities require the functioning of
separate but interacting modules in the brain
Emotional Intelligence
Focus questions: Describe the four branches of emotional intelligence.
Another form of adaptive ability lies within the emotional realm, and some theorists believe that
emotional competence is a form of intelligence.
According to John Mayer and Peter Salovey, emotional intelligence involves the abilities to read
others’ emotions accurately, to respond to them appropriately, to motivate oneself, to be aware of
one’s own emotions, and to regulate and control one’s own emotional responses.
Cultural differences
How is intelligence assessed in non-Western cultures?
Traditional intelligence tests such as the WAIS and the Stanford-Binet draw heavily on
the cognitive skills and learning that are needed to succeed in Western educational and
occupational settings.
They tend to have strong verbal content and to rely on the products of Western schooling.
Taken into a cultural context where smart is defined in different ways and requires other
kinds of adaptive behavior, such tests cannot hope to measure intelligence in a valid
fashion.
For example, the WAIS does not measure the ability to create herbal medicines, construct
shelters, or navigate in the open sea.
Robert Sternberg (2004) has advanced a theory of successful intelligence in which
intelligence is whatever is required to meet the adaptive demands of a given culture.
Sternberg believes that fundamental mental skills are required for successful behavior in
any culture. These include the ability to mentally represent problems in a way that
facilitates their solution, to develop potential solutions and to choose successfully among
them, to utilize mental resources wisely, and to evaluate the effects of one’s action plans.
What differs is the kinds of problems to which these basic intellectual skills are applied.
People from different cultures may think about the same problem in very different ways .
Two main approaches have been taken to meet the challenges of cross-cultural
intelligence assessment. One is to choose reasoning problems that are not tied to the
knowledge base of any culture but that reflect the ability to process and evaluate stimulus
patterns.
Raven Progressive Matrices, a test that is frequently used to measure fluid intelligence
On this nonverbal task, you must detect relationships and then decipher the rules
underlying the pattern of drawings in the rows and columns of the upper figure. Finally,
you must use this information to select the figure that is the missing entry from the eight
alternatives below.
The Raven test has been used in many cultures and measures a general mental capacity
that is also measured by traditional intelligence tests in our culture (Jensen, 1998).
Scores on the Raven correlate positively with measures of IQ derived from traditional
tests, yet they seem to be more “culture fair.”
A second and more challenging approach is to create measures that are tailored to the
kinds of knowledge and skills that are valued in the particular culture. Such tests may
measure how smart an individual is in terms of the practical skills and adaptive behaviors
within that culture. Scores may be unrelated or even negatively correlated with other
measures of intelligence, yet they may predict successful functioning within that culture.
If intelligence is defined as the ability to engage in culture-specific adaptive behavior,
then who is to say that the culture-specific measure is not a valid measure of intelligence
in that context?