Clinical Decision Making in Mental Health Practice
Clinical Decision Making in Mental Health Practice
Decision Making
in Mental Health
Practice
Published by To order
American Psychological Association APA Order Department
750 First Street, NE P.O. Box 92984
Washington, DC 20002 Washington, DC 20090-2984
www.apa.org Tel: (800) 374-2721; Direct: (202) 336-5510
Fax: (202) 336-5502; TDD/TTY: (202) 336-6123
Online: www.apa.org/pubs/books
E-mail: [email protected]
In the U.K., Europe, Africa, and the Middle East, copies may be ordered from
American Psychological Association
3 Henrietta Street
Covent Garden, London
WC2E 8LU England
The opinions and statements published are the responsibility of the authors, and such
opinions and statements do not necessarily represent the policies of the American
Psychological Association.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14711-000
This book is dedicated to the memory of Vincent Stephens, MD,
a loyal friend, consummate psychiatrist, and passionate psychotherapist.
CONTENTS
Contributors................................................................................................ ix
Foreword ..................................................................................................... xi
Gerald P. Koocher
Preface ....................................................................................................... xiii
Chapter 1. Overview and Challenges of Clinical Decision
Making in Mental Health Practice .................................. 3
Jeffrey J. Magnavita
Chapter 2. Clinical Expertise and Decision Making:
An Overview of Bias in Clinical Practice ...................... 23
Jeffrey J. Magnavita and Scott O. Lilienfeld
Chapter 3. A Dual Process Perspective on the Value of Theory
in Psychotherapeutic Decision Making ......................... 61
Jack C. Anchin and Jefferson A. Singer
vii
Chapter 4. Clinical Practice Guideline Development
and Decision Making ................................................... 105
Lynn F. Bufka and Erin F. Swedish
Chapter 5. Developing Clinical Practice Guidelines
to Enhance Clinical Decision Making ......................... 125
Steven D. Hollon
Chapter 6. Using Technology to Enhance Decision Making ........ 147
Franz Caspar, Thomas Berger, and Lukas Frei
Chapter 7. Clinical Decision Making When the
Stakes Are High ...................................................... 175
Jeffrey J. Magnavita
Chapter 8. Use of Empirically Grounded Relational Principles
to Enhance Clinical Decision Making ......................... 193
Ken L. Critchfield and Julia E. Mackaronis
Chapter 9. Integrating Ongoing Measurement Into
the Clinical Decision-Making Process
With Measurement Feedback Systems......................... 223
Thomas L. Sexton and Adam R. Fisher
Chapter 10. Clinical Decision Making and Risk Management ....... 245
Steven A. Sobelman and Jeffrey N. Younggren
Chapter 11. Teaching Clinical Decision Making ............................ 273
Gregg Henriques
Index ........................................................................................................ 309
About the Editor ...................................................................................... 321
viii CONTENTS
CONTRIBUTORS
xi
values. Less flattering descriptions from within the profession have included
a house of cards, the purchase of friendship, a means of social control, and
tradecraft.1 My personal favorite definition of psychotherapy came from the
secretary of the historic Boulder, Colorado, conference on the training of
psychologists, who satirically noted, “We have left therapy as an undefined
technique which is applied to unspecified problems with a nonpredictable
outcome. For this technique we recommend rigorous training.”2
In this context of complexity and radically different perspectives
Magnavita and his colleagues have compiled an impressive guide to making
sound clinical decisions in psychotherapy practice amid an array of potential
distractions. The readings guide us through decision-making biases, help us
base decisions on sound evidence, point us to existing guidelines, advise us on
the use of technology, help us to consider risk management, and advise us in
teaching. The result is a major resource to help us avoid the dangers of our own
biases and blind spots in a quest to avoid acting on Unwissenheit.
1Koocher, G. P., & Keith-Spiegel, P. (2008). Ethics in psychology and the mental health professions. New York,
NY: Oxford University Press.
2Lehner, G. F. J. (1952). Defining psychotherapy. American Psychologist, 7, 547.
xii FOREWORD
PREFACE
Bias is inherent in just about every part of our lives and is often an
unacknowledged but influential part of clinical practice. Understanding our
propensity for bias will go a long way toward enhancing our clinical decision-
making. Try this story, for example:
A father and his son are in a car accident. The father is killed and the son
is seriously injured. The son is taken to the hospital, where the surgeon
assesses the situation and declares, “I cannot operate because this boy is
my son.” Why is that so?1
The answer to this brainteaser, which as many as 75% of those who hear it
cannot solve, is that the boy’s mother is the surgeon. Gender bias is deeply
ingrained.
This volume represents a new and exciting area for me after more than
3 decades of clinical practice and clinical research spent viewing videotapes
of psychotherapy sessions ultimately trying to decide on the optimal course
of action. My efforts, which I have compiled and published in a series of
1Grant, A., & Sandberg, S. (2014, December 6). When talking about bias. New York Times, p. 46.
xiii
volumes and articles, involve working to distill the essential elements of
psychotherapy. The latest of my books is Unifying Psychotherapy: Principles,
Methods, and Evidence From Clinical Science,2 written with my collaborator
and friend Jack C. Anchin, who shares a passion for clinical work and the
scholarly and scientific search for the unifying principles and components of
clinical science and psychotherapy. My current work represents a departure
from my early interests in the treatment of personality disorders, integrative
and unified models of psychotherapy, and personality theory.
It was Eileen Bonetti’s infectious belief in and knowledge of decision
analytics that brought my awareness to this topic and sparked my curiosity and
interest in decision making. Clinicians, as well as all health professionals, are
involved in a continual process of decision making, basing critical decisions
on various streams of information, from the microprocesses in interpersonal
relationships to the results of meta-analytic studies on the efficacy of various
treatment approaches. It became apparent after an extensive review of the
decision-making literature that clinical practitioners can benefit from this
important body of work; the result is this volume.
I was also inspired to explore this exciting field following my experience
serving on the Clinical Practice Guideline Advisory Steering Committee
that the American Psychological Association initiated to begin the impor-
tant process of developing clinical practice guidelines. What became appar-
ent to me is that many components of clinical practice are not supported by
empirical evidence with which to guide decision making. One example is
how we determine the length or frequency of sessions. We continue to use the
50-minute session as the standard unit for delivering psychotherapy, yet I am
aware of no empirical evidence that examines the influence of various session
lengths and formats on outcome. How, then, do you make decisions such as
that one when there is no empirical evidence on which to draw? Mostly we
rely on clinical lore, assuming that Freud had it right when he framed psycho-
therapy in sessions of 50-minute duration. I think we can improve how we
respond clinically by examining our biases.
This volume would not have happened without the support and col-
laboration of many people. I have been actively engaged with a number
of friends, scholars, and clinicians over the years who have influenced and
sharpened my thinking and improved my own decision-making processes. I
want to especially thank Jack C. Anchin and Steven A. Sobelman for their
friendship and collaboration over the years. We have taken on a number
of exciting projects and spent many hours trying to figure out how to make
2Magnavita, J. J., & Anchin, J. C. (2013). Unifying psychotherapy: Principles, methods, and evidence from
xiv PREFACE
clinical science and psychotherapy more effective. Steve has taught so
many of us about the value of technology bringing us digital immigrants to
the 21st century. Together we founded the Unified Psychotherapy Project.
I also thank William C. Alder, Frank Knoblauch, John Santopietro, Peter
Tolisano, and Anne Shapiro, all part of my core professional support and
friendship group over the decades. I remember Vincent Stephens, an impor-
tant member of our core group, whose loss we deeply mourn. I also express
my appreciation for the collaboration and friendship over the years to
Gregg Henriques, Kenneth L. Critchfield, Jay L. Lebow, Andre Marquis,
Jeffrey E. Barnett, Rosie Adam-Terem, Craig N. Shealy, David M. Allen,
Michael Alpert, Kristin A. R. Osborn, Lori Calabrese, and many others who
have influenced my work and collaborated with me in patient care. I acknowl-
edge the many people I have had the privilege of working with on the road
to achieving more abundance in their lives who have trusted me with their
concerns. Finally, I thank my partner, Anne G. Magnavita, and my daughters
Elizabeth, Emily, and Caroline for more than they will ever know.
PREFACE xv
Clinical
Decision Making
in Mental Health
Practice
You have made a decision to begin reading this volume, Clinical Decision
Making in Mental Health Practice, on the basis of certain information that
influenced your decision to pick up this copy, open it, and begin reading at
this moment. Other decision-making processes will determine whether you
become engaged in the topic and continue reading the book or lose interest
and decide to redirect your valuable resources elsewhere. You will not be dis-
appointed if you continue with the book; the contributors herein are well
informed and offer useful information and wisdom as well as new ways of
thinking to improve your decision making from various perspectives.
Perhaps, in the back of your mind, you have been preoccupied and per-
plexed by a complicated individual, couple, or family you are treating, and
this may have unconsciously attracted you to the title of this volume. Maybe
your challenging case didn’t fit your personal internal heuristics, or “rules of
thumb”—mental shortcuts on which we often base our decisions that are
sometimes reliable but, unchecked, are prone to errors of thinking. Clinicians
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14711-001
Clinical Decision Making in Mental Health Practice, J. J. Magnavita (Editor)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
3
probably utilize hundreds or more of these heuristics in daily practice. For
example, one that comes to mind is how I view poor eye contact as a marker
of anxiety about emotional closeness and intimacy. In spite of the fact that
you may be highly trained in a particular model of treatment or well versed
in a number of evidence-based approaches and deem yourself a well-trained
and competent or even “expert” clinician, you may be uncertain about how
to proceed with treatment in certain situations. Developing expertise is not a
straightforward proposition of attaining the appropriate education, training,
and practice—more is required of us. We have a duty to those we treat for
continuous improvement. Some of your cases may not be progressing in the
manner you anticipated or expected, possibly creating a crisis of confidence.
For example, a patient may have dropped out of treatment and harmed himself
or someone else, showing deterioration in functioning instead of improving.
None of us likes to face these disappointing outcomes. Often treatment doesn’t
unfold as depicted in the textbooks or manualized versions of treatment so
ubiquitous these days. Treatment is often in the moment, and our responses
require trust in our intuition. With certain cases, you may be perplexed, con-
fused about how to proceed, and worried about how to make appropriate deci-
sions when there are few empirical data to guide you. You may find yourself
trying harder with a favored approach because you have already invested a
lot of time and energy in pursuing a particular course of action. I found this
to be a common response while in training in brief dynamic therapy and
afterward as a supervisor. I noticed that it was not uncommon for trainees to
increase their effort with a particular method, such as defensive restructuring,
when they were not getting the desired results. Instead of trying a different
method, they might resort to what is best described as “hammering”—
looking for the results viewed in the master therapist’s videos by doing more
of the same approach. An increase in the frequency and intensity of a method
all too often would lead to iatrogenic reactions and premature termination.
In one of my cases, a patient walked out of the session, and there were others
who never returned.
Although one psychotherapeutic strategy may work wonderfully with
many patients, with others, increasing our efforts and investment in a tried-
and-true strategy may prove ineffective or worse. In such a situation, trainees
may fall prey to the sunk-cost effect, which occurs when one continues to
invest in something in which he or she has already made an investment of
time and energy (Kahneman, 2011). In economics and business, we can see
that people often invest more money in a failing venture in an attempt to
recoup their losses. This may also be true for some addictive behaviors. Those
readers who treat gamblers know this narrative all too well. You have heard
people say that after they lost all of their money they used their credit card to
try to get back what they lost—this is the sunk-cost effect in operation. We
4 JEFFREY J. MAGNAVITA
may also witness the sunk-cost fallacy (Kahneman, 2011) in the treatment
context. This is a type of bias that can occur after we invest our resources in
pursuing a particular approach to treatment and then find ourselves trying to
apply this approach to all our patients instead of using tools of differential
treatment selection and finding the approach that best fits the patient and
his or her situation. This may be when it is warranted to seek a consultation
or to get a trusted colleague’s perspective.
We are also prone to want to present our best selves to the public. This
is evident when you attend treatment conferences where cases are presented.
Almost everyone presenting audiovisual tapes of their therapy sessions at
these conferences shows their best work. We select videotapes of patients
demonstrating great process and outcome. This selection bias may have
serious consequences for trainees, as it surely did when I was in training. This
often leads to what decision researchers term confirmation bias—we honor
data that support our approach and dismiss that which calls what we do into
question (Kahneman, 2011). Viewing positive treatment outcomes proves
the veracity of the approach being demonstrated, and ignoring treatment fail-
ures intensifies this effect. How would the field evolve if instead we presented
our worst outcomes or compared our best with our worst outcomes at these
conferences? Would the learning process progress more rapidly by a study of
errors and mistakes instead of only ideal treatments?
You may have made assumptions about patients in your clinical prac-
tice on the basis of diagnostic labels accrued from previous therapists,
resulting in an anchoring bias—placing too much weight on one aspect or
trait, which reduces the utility of predicting (Tversky & Kahneman, 1974).
What biases do you notice when a referring clinician describes a patient
as “psychopathic,” “borderline,” “narcissistic,” or “a pedophile”? Which
one of these labels has greater emotional activation? I think I can predict
which one does. Emotional responses can bias our judgment, compromising
our decision making. Diagnostic labels serve as anchors that may color our
thinking and influence our decision making on the basis of very limited infor-
mation, thereby leading to reliance on a representative heuristic (Kahneman,
2011), whereby we prematurely arrive at conclusions without sufficient data,
using too few data points. We may hastily assume that a very slender indi-
vidual suffers from an eating disorder. The sheer complexity or uniqueness
of some of our cases may leave us uncertain of both diagnosis and treatment
approach.
Without an understanding of decision theory we are more likely to fall
prey to various traps in our thinking or to biases of which we are unaware
but that nevertheless influence us. Decision theory allows us to be aware of
the underlying processes from which we derive our decisions. The quality of
evidence from which we make decisions is essential, and efforts to remain
6 JEFFREY J. MAGNAVITA
2010)? These questions and many others can be understood through the lens
of decision analytics that are based on advances in decision theory and through
empirical investigation of various ways that we make decisions and the traps
we can fall into.
A certain amount of energy will be required to master the material in
this volume. Some readers may consider the deliberative system of think-
ing “lazy” and would rather defer to their intuitive “fast” response system
(Kahneman, 2011), which will have already biased readers with informa-
tion regarding the stickiness of the title, status of the publisher, the names
of the editor and contributors, endorsements, or even the attractiveness of
the cover. Your personal biases will influence how effortfully you engage with
and learn the material presented in this volume. Your hidden biases may
also determine whether as a result of reading this book your clinical decision
making is enhanced and clinical efficacy improved. You have made a decision
to continue reading this volume, and as the volume editor, this pleases me
because there is so much information in the remaining chapters that you will
find useful and practical. Your decision to invest resources in this volume was
made using two very different but interrelated styles of thinking—one fast
(you may have impulsively picked the book off a shelf in a bookstore respond-
ing to some intuitive sense that it might be worthwhile) and the other slow
(you may have carefully read an advertisement and deliberately called on an
internal book-buying algorithm). You probably are unaware, or only vaguely
aware, of what influenced your decision to allocate your precious resources of
money and time that could be spent in other activities. Take a few minutes
to consider what factors influenced you, but try to remember that I already
made some suggestions that may have created a recency effect. Just as you
made this decision for reasons of your own, I decided that the topic of deci-
sion making and its application to mental and behavioral health was worth
the allocation of my resources. I am confident that it has been, although I
admit I may have succumbed to attributional bias—justifying to myself that
this is an important topic that is worth my effort (Heider, 1958). Maybe you
received a brochure online or in the mail, read a book review that piqued
your interest, were exploring a related topic and found this book, or were
attracted to the cover at the annual convention or on the shelf in a book-
store. At some level of awareness you weighed the cost–benefit outcome of
investing your resources, made a decision, and followed a course of action
about which I am pleased. All of these responses, and many more, are
heavily influenced by hidden biases associated with two main systems for
decision making, one tending to be more cognitive and the other more
emotionally derived. These systems exert a powerful influence on just about
every aspect of clinical decision-making practice and research and more
broadly on every decision you make.
8 JEFFREY J. MAGNAVITA
clinical decision making, especially in situations of uncertainty, which are so
common in clinical practice where our ability to predict is often so limited.
This volume will introduce you to the rapidly advancing science of decision
analytics and review the decision biases that affect us all. More important,
this volume will explore the application of decision theory through a number
of interrelated topics relevant to clinical practice, research, training, and
behavioral health care administration. Decision theory is influencing just
about every scientific discipline, including mathematics, sociology, behav-
ioral economics, computer science, and many others, and yet even though
psychologists have conducted most of the essential research, many psycho-
logists are not familiar with this topic. Because mental health professionals
have been slow to absorb these groundbreaking new interdisciplinary devel-
opments, it is my hope that reading this book will be a step in remedying
this lack of knowledge by exposing you to the advantages of using deci-
sion theory as a framework for clinical practice. I hope that you will con-
tinue to read and absorb the concepts in this volume and that that this
knowledge will provide you with a foundation in decision analytics. This
deeper appreciation for the value of decision theory and related topics will
help you achieve better therapeutic results by increasing your awareness
of the biases that detract from optimal decision making. This volume also
offers strategies and approaches that will mitigate these biases and thus
enhance your decision-making skills and optimize the outcomes of all your
decisions.
10 JEFFREY J. MAGNAVITA
Wrong” (2013) challenged the assumption that science is self-correcting.
Daniel Kahneman was quoted in an open letter challenging the widely accepted
concept of priming (the notion that decisions we make are influenced by
irrelevant events presented right before a choice is made) in psychological
research as poorly founded, and many studies trying to replicate findings
that seemingly support priming have failed to show support. Jerome Kagan
(2012), in his book Psychology’s Ghosts: The Crisis in the Profession and the
Way Back, challenged many of the assumptions that serve as the basis for
contemporary psychology. The assumptions that we make and may hold dear
influence a cascade of treatment issues from diagnostic labels to the treat-
ments that follow. Kagan illustrated the assumptions, often unexamined and
based on limited evidence, that guide our conceptualizations and approach
to treatment. He held that a biological bias may prematurely lead clinicians
toward suggesting a pharmacological approach, on the basis of a belief that
genetic predispositions have led to certain symptom constellations. Kagan’s
view is that although psychopharmacological treatment for mental disorders
is fraught with biases, it continues to dominate the clinical landscape even
though most psychotropic medication “can be likened to a blow on the
head and resemble the cocktail of drugs used with many cancers that kill
both healthy and cancerous tissues” (p. 209). American Psychological
Association initiatives to develop clinical practice guidelines (Hollon
et al., 2014) based on the most robust accumulated evidence should assist
the public, behavioral and mental health consumers, policymakers, and
other scientific disciplines in being assured that psychology is indeed a sci-
ence and one that may be moving to a unifying framework (Magnavita &
Anchin, 2014).
Who would not like to improve their clinical outcomes or assist stu-
dents and trainees in attaining the best results? The National Institute of
Mental Health (1999) identified the incorporation of patient and provider
decision making as an important research agenda for improving mental
health interventions. In an effort to improve quality of mental health
care, the Institute of Medicine (2001) suggested that the study of decision-
making theory (Roberto, 2009), concepts, and preferences is imperative.
Probably one reason you are reading this volume is that you want to maxi-
mize your clinical expertise or assist others in doing so by adding to your
knowledge base, thus increasing your effectiveness in your own and your
students’ treatment of behavioral and mental health disorders. Clinical
decision making is the foundation of behavioral and mental health prac-
tice, yet most clinicians receive very little formal training in this complex
activity. Clinical decision making requires a depth and range of knowledge
that requires years of education, training, and supervision, yet this critical
activity is not systematically taught. I hope this volume serves as an agent
of change by bringing decision making to the forefront of science, practice,
and education. Decision making has characteristics of both intuitive and
deliberative modes (Stanovich, 2010). Both are necessary, and neither can
operate without the other. The most profound responsibility a clinician
has is discerning which interventions are beneficial and which may prove
inert or even harmful for those who seek our assistance. A series of “correct”
decisions can have a potentially profound impact on improving the lives
of those in our care. A series of “incorrect” decisions can have dire results:
Patients may fail to improve or will deteriorate, and in extreme cases the
outcome may be death. The science and theory of clinical decision making
is the focus of this volume.
12 JEFFREY J. MAGNAVITA
DECISION MAKING: THE KEY TO EVIDENCE-BASED PRACTICE
I have already discussed the evolutionary basis for and some of the neural
circuits involved in decision making. Our brains have evolved to perceive,
filter, organize, and respond to a potentially overwhelming amount of envi-
ronmental stimuli. Without strategies to organize this incoming information
and develop internal patterns or templates, we would be rendered helpless. We
encode information and store it for later use maintaining internal schemata,
which are prototypes we use to compare current situations with past situations
and look for a match. We learn by encoding and storing new information and
then decode what comes in by seeing how it matches our internal representa-
tion. Most decisions are made automatically without having to strain our men-
tal resources: This is the fast thinking mode. “The mental work that produces
impressions, intuitions, and many decisions goes on in silence in our mind”
(Kahneman, 2011, p. 4). This type of decision making represents what is often
experienced as intuitive and real but as prone to error. We activate the slow
thinking system to prevent the fast one from ruling our lives. We help patients
learn how and when to apply slow thinking with respect to their therapeutic
endeavors, regardless of the approach and what it is termed.
14 JEFFREY J. MAGNAVITA
SHARED DECISION MAKING
DECISION ANALYTICS
Health care as a whole (with mental health soon to follow) will be seeing a
greater reliance on the use of aggregated data to inform decision making, which
will certainly have an economic impact in terms of who will get reimbursed
and at what level. This trend is being fueled by computer technology, which is
allowing us to mine data as never before.
Technological advances have led to new possibilities for using big data
for making decisions, which will inevitably change how behavioral and mental
health care is delivered. This trend is in large part being driven by the expan-
sion of the Internet along with powerful computer processing to change the
fundamental ways we gather and process information. “The fruits of the infor-
mation society are easy to see, with a cellphone in every pocket, a computer
in every backpack, and big information technology systems in back offices”
(Mayer-Schonberger & Cukier, 2013, p. 8). Since the beginning of the era
of information technology, humans have generated more information than
had been heretofore produced in the cumulative history of our species (Lehrer,
2009). “From sciences to healthcare, from banking to the Internet, the sectors
may be diverse yet together they tell a similar story: the amount of data in the
world is growing fast, outstripping not just our machines but our imaginations”
(Mayer-Schonberger & Cukier, 2013, p. 8). This has important implications
that are beyond the scope of this volume, so I suggest that those interested read
Big Data by Viktor Mayer-Schonberger and Kenneth Cukier (2013), which
presents an in-depth review of the trend and its implications.
This volume offers a range of topics that will introduce the reader to
the field of decision making in mental health practice. Decision making
influences every aspect of clinical practice and is increasingly important for
behavioral and mental health clinicians, as well as all health care providers,
because of the inherent uncertainty in many aspects of clinical science.
Many decisions are made that do not derive from an empirical evidence base
and that necessitate a comfort with uncertainty. The topics covered in the
remainder of this volume provide a sample of some of the important areas
with which clinicians, researchers, and educators should be familiar. Decision
theory and decisional research are far ranging and rapidly expanding into
exciting areas such as big data and data mining that will influence all aspects
of health care in the future. There is also an accelerating trend in all areas of
health care to develop guidelines that practitioners, patients, policymakers,
and others can use to optimize treatment of physical and behavioral health
disorders.
The current chapter, Chapter 1, has introduced the topic of decision
making in an effort to provide a brief overview of the subject of this volume
and to highlight some of the essential constructs, common biases in deci-
sion making, and the five pillars of effective decision analytics. In Chapter 2
16 JEFFREY J. MAGNAVITA
(“Clinical Expertise and Decision Making: An Overview of Bias in Clinical
Practice”), my coauthor Scott O. Lilienfeld and I present an overview of
decision analytics and the biases and traps of which clinicians and researchers
should beware. This chapter provides a solid foundation in some of the
important topics related to decision making, along with a brief compendium
of common traps or errors to which we are subject when making clinical
decisions. Decision-making theory is included in a robust body of literature
that emanates from research from many disciplines. The fundamentals of
decision analytics and the biases that present danger are critical aspects of
optimal clinical practice. In Chapter 3 (“A Dual Process Perspective on the
Value of Theory in Psychotherapeutic Decision Making”), Jack C. Anchin
and Jefferson A. Singer explore the importance of theory in decision making,
an essential tool that has been evolving and becoming more sophisticated
over the course of clinical science history. Theory presents a way of organiz-
ing and understanding clinical phenomena, offering a road map for decision
making if used appropriately. In Chapter 4 (“Clinical Practice Guideline
Development and Decision Making”), Lynn F. Bufka and Erin F. Swedish
introduce the science and processes that go into the development of treat-
ment guidelines. These cutting-edge developments include both exciting and
somewhat controversial trends and the safeguards that ensure that clinical
practice guideline development is as scientifically valid and transparent as
humanly possible. After reading the chapter by Bufka and Swedish, the next
question you might ask is what to do with this information. Clinical prac-
tice guidelines are all well and good, but we want to know how they help us
treat behavioral and mental health disorders. In Chapter 5 (“Developing
Clinical Practice Guidelines to Enhance Clinical Decision Making”),
Steven D. Hollon explains how using an evidence base can enhance clini-
cal decision making and how clinicians can maximize their effectiveness by
referring to practice guidelines. In Chapter 6 (“Using Technology to Enhance
Decision Making”), Franz Caspar and coauthors show the importance of
technology in clinical decision making. Technology is fundamentally chang-
ing who we are as a species and providing us with multiple options to enhance
our decision making. The number of technological developments available to
us seems to steadily increase, and having knowledge of these can increase our
decision making skills. In Chapter 7 (“Clinical Decision Making When the
Stakes Are High”), I present a model of collaborative decision making that
was developed while working in close collaboration with a psychiatrist treat-
ing complex clinical presentations. Clinical decision making is most chal-
lenging when the stakes are high, when cases may not have a simple solution
and hence involve a high level of risk and potential harm. In Chapter 8 (“Use
of Empirically Grounded Relational Principles to Enhance Clinical Decision
Making”), Ken L. Critchfield and Julia E. Mackaronis show how relational
SUMMARY
18 JEFFREY J. MAGNAVITA
fast and the slow systems of thinking must flexibly interact and can balance
each other to maximize outcome. There are a number of biases that occur
without our knowledge when we rely too much on the fast response system.
These errors can be avoided by developing knowledge of various types of
biases that are common to problem solving.
The chapters that follow will take you along a fascinating path that
you will find has direct bearing on just about every aspect of your clinical
practice, regardless of the setting or the types of decisions that you must
make. Enhancing our decision-making skills and knowledge will advance the
practice of behavioral and mental health treatment and allow us to adapt and
thrive in the new era of health care that is emerging in this country and the
world. Thank you for deciding to read this chapter, and I hope you will decide
that this volume is worth your investment.
REFERENCES
20 JEFFREY J. MAGNAVITA
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and
biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.185.4157.1124
van der Kolk, B. (2014). The body keeps the score: Brain, mind, and body in the healing
of trauma. New York, NY: Viking.
von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1944). Theory of games and economic behavior.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Walfish, S., McAlister, B., O’Donnell, P., & Lambert, M. J. (2012). An investigation
of self-assessment bias in mental health providers. Psychological Reports, 110,
639–644.
Wills, C. E., & Holmes-Rovner, M. (2006). Integrating decision making and
mental health interventions research: Research directions. Clinical Psychology,
13, 9–25.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14711-002
Clinical Decision Making in Mental Health Practice, J. J. Magnavita (Editor)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
23
when making decisions, and how can we avoid these? Clinicians and research-
ers alike routinely fall prey to errors in this type of thinking and many others
that can have dire consequences, but as clinical experts we have a responsi-
bility to avoid these cognitive traps. Clinicians have a “duty to know,” which
refers to an “epistemic duty . . . best enacted through a critical knowledge of
the scientific method in psychology and the relevant scientific literature”
(O’Donohue, Lilienfeld, & Fowler, 2007, p. 4). For example, we must try
to avoid missing a high-risk marker for self-destructive behavior, pushing a
patient too hard or fast in treatment, failing to corroborate information with
other family members, selectively including and excluding certain studies in
a meta-analysis, and so forth. Complex decisions are often influenced by hid-
den biases that can lead to faulty reasoning. These decision biases bear major
implications for almost every aspect of clinical practice and research, and it
is thus imperative for clinicians to be knowledgeable about them.
There are some “great” ideas and guiding principles every mental health
practitioner should know (Lilienfeld & O’Donohue, 2007). Advances in clini-
cal science over a century have led to a substantial body of information, includ-
ing robust theoretical constructs, empirical evidence in a variety of domains,
accumulated clinical experience, and evidence from related disciplines, which
serve as the foundation for and as a guide to decision making in clinical practice
(Magnavita & Anchin, 2014). However, accumulated experience or knowledge
alone does not necessarily result in expertise (Tracey, Wampold, Lichtenberg,
& Goodyear, 2014). “Expertise in any endeavor requires, among other things,
a considerable amount of dedicated practice” (Ruscio, 2007, p. 40). The
10,000-hour rule suggests that to become expert at a complex endeavor, one
must practice at least 10,000 hours to achieve mastery (Gladwell, 2008).
And yet, clinical expertise requires more than this extensive knowledge base
and much practice; we also must be wise enough to know how, and when,
to apply what we know. “To be effective clinical scientists, we must base our
actions and decisions on reliable knowledge. We should not simply guess or
believe, but instead know how nature, in this case human nature, actually
operates to influence behavior” (O’Donohue et al., 2007, p. 4). Accumulated
knowledge does not necessarily determine how to proceed in the consulting
room. Expertise requires training and experience in making complex deci-
sions, which in part derives from training and clinical experience, combined
with effective supervision and feedback. The empirical evidence on whether
more experienced psychotherapists get better outcomes is equivocal. There
is some evidence that experienced psychotherapists do have an advantage
in the face of complexity over less experienced ones (Oddli & Halvorsen,
2014), but other evidence suggests that expertise does not play a role in out-
come (Beutler, 1997; Okiishi, Lambert, Nielsen, & Ogles, 2003). Expertise
does not arise solely from experience because there is no guarantee that we
Data are discrete units of information that are not yet organized on the
basis of observations or facts. For example, a list of names and telephone
numbers and discrete observations from a research study are classified as data.
There is no inherent usefulness to data because they become useful only when
they are coherently and systematically organized. Computers rely on enormous
quantities of nonprocessed bytes encoded as 0 or 1 units, or data, that when
organized into code become useful. Never before in the history of our species
have we had the capability to amass such voluminous amounts of data. These
“big data” that most often comes from the Internet is a new frontier for infor-
mation technology. “Today, every e-mail, instant message, phone call, line of
written code and mouse-click leaves a digital signal” (Lohr, 2013, p. 4). This
constitutes a revolution in data gathering that is beginning to fundamentally
transform the world. Data patterns are being mined and used to enhance
innovations in health care and business. “Digital technology also makes it
possible to conduct and aggregate personality-based assessments, often using
online quizzes or games, in far greater detail and numbers than ever before”
(Lohr, 2013, p. 4). This emphasis on mining big data is leading to an ever
greater reliance on data-driven decision making. The future of clinical deci-
sion making will almost certainly be shaped by innovative uses of big data.
Information
When data have been endowed with some meaning and have been orga-
nized into a useful form, they are called information. A phone book is an exam-
ple of information because the data (names and phone numbers) are organized
in a way to be easily accessed. The development of clinical practice guidelines,
discussed in Chapter 4 in this volume, exemplifies what should eventually be
useful information that when accessed by clinicians and the public will serve
to reduce uncertainty. Anyone will be able to read the guidelines, a source of
information, but applying them will require more than simply assimilating
the information provided—expertise is necessary.
Knowledge
Clinical decision making begins at the point of first contact with the
patient, when we initiate our pattern-detection process. During the first con-
tact, the clinician has to make multiple decisions that involve comparisons
with internal schema. The clinician rapidly matches internal templates when
considering decisions such as who should attend the initial session (patient,
couple, family, significant others), proposed duration of the session, how much
should be discussed on the phone, and many other issues that immediately
must be contemplated and decided on. Science in large part is about finding
patterns in nature. One way we make clinical decisions is to draw from the
depth and range of information and clinical experience we have amassed and
compare external reality with internal patterns that we have constructed.
Specifically, clinicians utilize prototypes as a form of template to help
compare cases and organize decision making. Essentially, we use these heu-
ristics to make predictions. When we make a diagnostic formulation, we do
so in part to predict the course of a disorder, the client’s response to treat-
ment, and therapeutic strategies that optimize outcome. Each strategy we
employ in treatment is based on an implicit or explicit decision to achieve a
therapeutic goal. For example, the regulation of anxiety is a central aspect of
the treatment process (Faust, 2007). We are continually matching patterns
derived from past experience with our current ones, and thereby compar-
ing and evolving our mental maps. If anxiety is too high, a patient may be
flooded, whereas if it is too low, there may be little motivation to change and
treatment may stagnate. This process of anxiety management often occurs
with the fast processing of System 1, but anxiety regulation should be based
on extensive knowledge and drawn from clinical experience. Groopman
(2007) described this as occurring “within seconds, largely without any con-
scious analysis; it draws most heavily on the doctor’s visual appraisal of the
patient” (pp. 34–35). This is not a “linear, step-by-step combining of cues”
but the mind acting like a magnet, assimilating data from many domains of
the patient (pp. 34–35).
Anchoring Bias
Attributional Bias
Availability
Confirmation Bias
Gambler’s Fallacy
Gambler’s fallacy refers to our tendency to see a link between past and
present events when in fact they are independent (Stanovich, 2010). For
example, if you flip a coin once and get heads, the chance that you will flip
Endowment Effect
This bias in thinking may explain why practitioners often strongly advocate
and employ a specific approach to treatment, even if this treatment is not
compellingly supported by research.
Fixed Frames
Narrative Fallacy
Overconfidence Bias
Recall Bias
Recency Effect
This mental shortcut operates by the rule of “like goes with like” (Tversky
& Kahneman, 1974). There are “virtues” to judging information on the basis
of our impressions. In many cases, reliance on stereotypes (e.g., the extent to
which a client reminds us of our prototype of the modal patient with schizo-
phrenia) is more accurate than sheer guessing (Kahneman, 2011, p. 151).
Nevertheless, this tendency can lead to errors in the clinical context. We may
assume that a client with bipolar disorder who reminds us of a previous client
with borderline personality disorder has the latter condition; as a consequence,
the client may receive inappropriate or inadequate treatment. However, it is
tempting to predict unlikely events even when the base rates are low. This error
can lead to the phenomenon of base rate neglect, a corollary of the overuse of
the representativeness heuristic. For example, if a client who acts differently on
different occasions reminds us of our prototype of dissociative identity disorder,
we may jump too readily to assign this diagnosis, forgetting that the prevalence
of this condition in the general population is probably extremely low.
Sunk-Cost Effect
Selection Bias
Where and from whom we gather our data influences our analyses. Most
psychologists have been trained to recognize the inherent problems when
samples are not obtained by random selection. This bias can be introduced
in many ways. For example,
A survey of consumers in an airport is going to be biased by the fact that
people who fly are likely to be wealthier than the general public; a survey
Survivorship Bias
Another factor that can bias our thinking is the fact that in datasets
from clinical and research settings, dropout rates are often high. Those who
remain in treatment may not be representative of the population. Wheelan
(2013) wrote, “If you have a room of people with varying heights, forcing the
short people to leave will raise the average height in the room, but it doesn’t
make anyone taller” (p. 123). In psychotherapy, the clients who remain in
treatment are typically those who are benefiting from it. If we do not take
survivorship bias into account, we may overestimate our effectiveness as
therapists.
Publication Bias
The access to information that is available with the Internet and search
engines is fundamentally changing human social memories and transcend-
ing some of the limitations of human cognition (Wegner & Ward, 2013).
Most clinicians now have access to high-quality information that has never
been so readily available and can be easily used to enhance decision making
by using Boolean operators—search commands—to access relevant data-
bases (Norcross et al., 2008). In Chapter 5 in this volume, Hollon discusses
the steps involved in generating structured questions in the development
of clinical practice guidelines. These are called PICOTS, a mnemonic that
stands for populations, interventions, comparisons, outcomes, time, and
settings.
SUMMARY
REFERENCES
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14711-003
Clinical Decision Making in Mental Health Practice, J. J. Magnavita (Editor)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
61
The clinician is also confronted with decisions pertaining to the process
of psychotherapy. For example, as treatment unfolds, what ways of relation-
ally responding to and interacting with the patient will best establish, build,
and maintain the therapeutic alliance? At any given point when engaging in
inquiry, should an open-ended or a closed-ended question be used? How long
should silence be maintained before the therapist intercedes with a statement
or question? At a particular juncture in treatment, what degree of balance
between support and confrontation is most responsive to the patient’s cur-
rent treatment needs and clinical status? And what are the indicators that it
is appropriate to begin addressing treatment termination, and what is the best
way to implement termination with this specific patient?
Although these and innumerable other questions operate as decision
choice points for the psychotherapist, one facet of psychotherapy about
which the therapist does not need to make a decision is the overarching
purpose of the enterprise: Psychotherapy is an intrinsically goal-directed pro-
cess intended to help the patient move toward greater mental health and
self-understanding in his or her day-to-day functioning and subjective expe-
rience. This metagoal frames the entire endeavor, and therefore the practice
of psychotherapy presupposes that the therapist’s judgments and decisions
throughout the treatment process are motivated by this prevailing desire.
This motivation is of course crucial, but it is not sufficient. The therapist
must also have actual tools that, yoked to this essential motivation to help,
facilitate and guide his or her specific decisions in therapeutic directions.
In this chapter we discuss the centrality of theory as one such tool—as
vital to the psychotherapist as an architect’s blueprints are to builders or
a musical score is to musicians in a symphony orchestra. Just as blueprints
or a musical score stipulate the components of their ultimate product, they
also presuppose a particular endpoint—a skyscraper or a symphony. They are
created with a teleological purpose—to yield a particular “good outcome.”
Theory for psychotherapists works in the same way. It stipulates not only the
working parts of the process that will unfold but also the particular endpoint
of “good health” that is being pursued. Psychotherapy is always promoting a
particular vision of the good life, and theory is the medium through which
this message is constructed.
Theory contributes to psychotherapeutic decision making, and its utili-
zation can be maximized for the good. In the chapter’s first section we crystal-
lize key features of theory in psychotherapy, emphasizing that both explicit
and implicit theories inhabit the mind of the psychotherapist. In the second
section we elucidate the centrality of dual process conceptions of the human
mind by describing attributes of automatic and deliberative processing, their
differential approaches to decision making, and their interactivity. We then
integrate these two lines of discussion in the third section by presenting and
Explicit Theory
Implicit Theory
Over the past 3 decades, an array of dual process models of the human
mind have been developed in cognitive and social psychology. These models
and their associated cross-disciplinary research programs have extended their
reach to the study of judgment and decision making. Illustrating the value
of these concepts through their application to medical diagnostic reasoning
and decision making, Croskerry (2009b) suggested that “a variety of lines
of evidence from philosophy, psychology, neurology, neuroanatomy, neuro-
physiology, and genetics in recent years provides support for the view that
decision making might best be represented by dual process theory” (p. 29).
Lying at the heart of this body of formulations is the view that the human
mind is characterized by two “qualitatively distinct forms of processing”
(Evans & Stanovich, 2013, p. 226), one characterized by automatic and the
1Other theorists and researchers have characterized the distinction between these two forms of infor-
mation processing as, for example, automatic and controlled (Schneider & Chein, 2003), associative and
reflective (Beevers, 2005), experiential/intuitive and rational/analytic (Epstein, 2010), reflexive and reflec-
tive (Lieberman, 2007), System 1 and System 2 (Kahneman & Frederick, 2002), and Type 1 and Type 2
(Evans & Stanovich, 2013). Each such dualistic characterization captures major differentiating features
encompassed by dual process theories. For present purposes, the automatic–deliberative terminology has
been chosen to descriptively highlight a core distinguishing feature of each type of processing on which
other features of each system seem to conceptually load, as it were. It must also be noted that whatever
terminology is used, the dual process distinction is not without controversy (see, e.g., Keren & Schul,
2009; Newell & Shanks, 2014).
2Indeed, it is important to note that among major responsibilities of institutional review boards in evalu-
ating proposed research is identifying and weighing the possible risks and benefits posed to participants
in the proposed study (a risk–benefit analysis).
EXHIBIT 3.2
Sources of Bias and Error in Deliberative Decision Making
• Selective attention to or overfocus on particular stimulus characteristics of the
decision situation (e.g., those that are most accessible or easily articulated) at
the expense of other information more relevant to the decision (Evans, 2007;
Bodenhausen & Todd, 2010)
• Gaps in the “mindware” (Stanovich, 2011) necessary to engage in rational thinking
(e.g., missing declarative knowledge in a particular domain pertinent to the deci-
sion situation)
• Incorrect information (Hammond, Hamm, Grassia, & Pearson, 1987)
• Premature closure on considering alternative causal hypotheses by virtue of
assuming an initial hypothesis is accurate (Croskerry, 2002)
• Reasoning that is quick and sloppy (Stanovich, 2011)
• Distraction (Croskerry, 2009b)
• Fatigue (Croskerry, 2002)
3This approach to interpersonal psychotherapy, also characterized as the interpersonal tradition, interpersonal
paradigm, interpersonal system, and interpersonal nexus (Pincus, 2010), is rooted in Sullivan’s (1953) seminal
interpersonal psychiatry and ensuing elaborations by such notables as Leary (1957), Carson (1969, 1982),
Wiggins (1979), Benjamin (1996), and Kiesler (1996). It is also important to note that Sullivanian inter-
personalism, synthesized with British object relations theory and self-psychologies rooted in Kohutian
theory, has been integral to the development of relational psychoanalysis (Aron, 1996; Mitchell, 1988;
Singer, 2005; Wachtel, 1997), and hence, not surprisingly, conceptual and technical–procedural parallels
between the interpersonal approach described in this section and relational psychoanalysis are consider-
able (see Anchin, 2002).
4In cognitive therapy theory, automatic thoughts are immediate cognitions that consciously run through
a person’s mind in response to a situation and arise from the core belief activated in that situation
(Clark & Beck, 2010).
SUMMARY
REFERENCES
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14711-004
Clinical Decision Making in Mental Health Practice, J. J. Magnavita (Editor)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
105
development and the efforts undertaken to reduce potential forms of bias
(see Chapter 2, this volume, for an in-depth presentation of these forms)
is critical for their acceptance and uptake. In this chapter, we discuss how
CPGs enhance clinical decision making. We also provide an overview of the
development of CPGs and the efforts expended to reduce biases.
Over the past 2 decades there has been an increasing call for evidence-
based practice in all of health care, including treatment for behavioral
and mental health disorders. Both internal and external pressures, such as
demands for accountability, marketplace changes, technological advances,
and the explosion of research literature, have resulted in increased attention
to the quality and content of psychotherapy (Goodheart, 2010). Currently,
numerous empirically supported treatments (ESTs) for individuals with men-
tal health problems have been shown to be efficacious. More specifically,
sophisticated methodological designs have shown robust effects for various
ESTs for anxiety, depression, posttraumatic stress disorder, and bipolar dis-
order, to name a few (Barlow, Levitt, & Bufka, 1999; Barrett & Ollendick,
2003). Large-scale efforts to disseminate ESTs have occurred as the state of
science in psychology advances (Chorpita, 2002). However, other widely
practiced psychotherapy approaches have not yet accumulated corresponding
supportive bodies of research. Although lack of evidence does not indicate
lack of efficacy (APA Presidential Task Force, 2006), the lack of evidence
is problematic when payers and other third parties determine practices or
otherwise demand certain clinical treatment approaches.
CPGs are one tool to help link research and practice. CPGs as defined
by the IOM are “systematically developed statements to assist practitioner
and patient decisions about appropriate health care for specific clinical cir-
cumstances” (Field & Lohr, 1990, p. 38). The development of quality CPGs
for the treatment of behavioral and mental health disorders has the potential
to help patients, mental health practitioners, policymakers, and administra-
tors make better decisions about how to proceed with care.
Evidence-based CPGs are vital for effective clinical decision making
and efficient patient care. The three-circle model for evidence-based clini-
cal decisions (see Figure 4.1) was developed to promote a clear process for
decision making to inform clinicians’ treatment decisions (Haynes, Sackett,
Gray, Cook, & Guyatt, 1996). Separately, the three circles illustrate that
evidence-based clinical decisions use the best available research; clini-
cal expertise; and patient values, characteristics, and circumstances. The
Pa!ent
Characteris!cs, Clinical
Culture, Exper!se
Preferences, &
Values
Figure 4.1. Three-circle model of evidence based practice in psychology. Data from
Haynes, Sackett, Gray, Cook, and Guyatt (1996).
In the United States, unlike some other countries, the federal govern-
ment has not assumed responsibility for creating CPGs. Typically, professional
associations or health care organizations either singly or collaboratively cre-
ate these guidelines in the United States. The U.S. Agency for Health Care
Policy and Research was established in 1989 with responsibilities that included
outcomes research and practice guideline development (Gray, Gusmano, &
Collins, 2003). In the mid 1990s, the agency faced criticism over its work.
Although the agency ultimately survived, it was renamed the Agency for
Healthcare Research and Quality, and its focus shifted to comparative effec-
tiveness research rather than guideline development. It is in this context
that guideline development by organizations and professional associations
increased, and standards for guideline development emerged in 2011.
Although the development of CPGs flourished in the 1990s and 2000s,
guidelines had strong opponents (Abrahamson & Saakvitne, 2000; Reed,
McLaughlin, & Newman, 2002). Criticism of existing guidelines included
lack of generality or objectivity, suggesting potential bias in their content and
therefore application or concerns regarding the presentation of the science
(Craske & Zucker, 2001). For instance, guidelines from government entities,
such as the VA, are focused on specific populations and therefore might not be
applicable to other populations. Additionally, guidelines developed by profes-
sional societies were seen as potentially slanting toward the interests of spe-
cific guilds, and guidelines funded by industry were potentially biased toward
industry interest, whether that funding and interest might be pharmaceutical,
EXHIBIT 4.1
Institute of Medicine Standards for Developing
Trustworthy Clinical Practice Guidelines
• Transparency in development and funding
• Disclose, manage and resolve conflicts of interest
• Panels multidisciplinary, balanced and include patient/community involvement
(adversarial collaboration)
• Systematic reviews of literature are basis for guideline recommendations
• Rate quality of evidence and recommendation strength
• Recommendations framed as actionable statements
• Submit draft guideline for public review and comments
• Update guideline periodically as necessary
Note. Data from Institute of Medicine (2011a).
Transparency
Conflicts of Interest
External Review
Updating
SYSTEMATIC REVIEWS
SUMMARY
REFERENCES
The clinical situation is complex, and no two clients are ever exactly
alike. At times only limited empirical evidence exists to guide the decision-
making process, and what evidence exists is often open to dispute. Clinical
practice guidelines (CPGs) are intended to help guide the decision-making
process. They are not a substitute for clinical judgment but when well crafted
can provide a basis for the decisions to be made. Clinicians need to under-
stand the basics underlying how these guidelines are developed in order to
get maximum benefit from their use.
The American Psychological Association (APA) has decided in recent
years to begin developing CPGs. That decision was not without some contro-
versy and represents an evolution of the position that the APA has taken over
the years (see Chapter 4, this volume, for a discussion of that evolution). It
formed an advisory steering committee (ASC) to guide the process, and I was
fortunate enough to be selected as its initial chair. The issues I describe in this
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14711-005
Clinical Decision Making in Mental Health Practice, J. J. Magnavita (Editor)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
125
chapter represent my own perspective on the questions that we confronted
and do not necessarily represent the views of the ASC or the larger APA.
In this chapter I describe the evolving science that underlies the devel-
opment of CPGs and its implications for the clinical decision-making process.
Guideline development has moved from a process based largely on unsystem-
atic clinical consensus to a more deliberative process in which a systematic
review (SR) of the scientific data is conducted and the resultant evidence
presented to a panel of individuals with diverse perspectives and expertise
(including service utilizers to provide input concerning patient preferences).
The goal is to ground the discussion in the best science currently available as
filtered through the differing perspectives and expertise of the panelists in a
manner that balances out whatever biases and blind spots each may possess.
Not long after the APA steering committee was formed, the Institute
of Medicine (IOM) published two sets of recommendations for generating
CPGs. These reports had been in process of development for several years and
did (in my opinion) a marvelous job of summarizing the current state of the
knowledge regarding how to generate guidelines that the public could trust.
The first report described the process of forming the guideline development
panels (GDPs) charged with generating the treatment recommendations
(IOM, 2011a), and the second dealt with the execution of the SRs on which
those recommendations would be based (IOM, 2011b). The reports them-
selves are available to the public and summarized in recent articles (Hollon
et al., 2014) and chapters (Chapter 4, this volume). Therefore, I highlight
only certain aspects of the reports (and the larger process of guideline devel-
opment) that I think are most relevant to their subsequent use.
It is important to note in that regard that both the IOM (2001) and the
APA Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice (2006) explicitly
adhere to a tripartite model of clinical decision making in which the best
available evidence (represented by the CPG) is filtered through the expertise
of the treating clinician to arrive at the best decisions consistent with the pref-
erences and values of the individual patient. In essence, the goal of guideline
development is to put the best available evidence before the patient and treat-
ing clinician, but the clinician trumps the guideline and the patient trumps
the clinician.
What this means to me is that I have an obligation to provide my
patients with the most complete and accurate information possible (drawing
when possible on the CPG in the process) and to make my recommendations
The IOM also laid out a series of steps for conducting SRs. The details
of these steps are rather technical in nature and will not be reviewed in any
detail, but several aspects of the process deserve to be highlighted. First, con-
ducting an SR is broader and more inclusive than simply conducting a meta-analysis,
and in some instances (when studies are weak or few) conducting a meta-
analysis might not even be indicated. Meta-analyses provide a quantitative
summary of the empirical literature and can serve as a real guide to the recom-
mendation development process, but like all summaries they often obscure
relevant information. It is important for the GDPs to know just how many
and how strong the studies are that contribute to a given body of the litera-
ture and whether systematic limitations exist that ought to be reflected in the
nature and the strength of the resultant recommendations.
Second, publication bias is often a concern and tends to inflate the
apparent efficacy of the given interventions. This is true not only for medi-
cation treatment (as previously noted) but also with respect to psychother-
apy, although likely for noncommercial reasons (Cuijpers, Smit, Bohlmeijer,
Hollon, & Andersson, 2010). There is a tendency for small studies with weak
effects to not find their way into the literature, whereas small studies with large
effects (often a consequence of chance) are much more likely to be published.
Therefore, it can be important to search the “gray literature” for studies that were
never published. The IOM recommends working with a reference librarian or
other personnel experienced in the conduct of SRs to ensure adequate cover-
age. What this means is that the clinician and the public will not be misled by
inflated estimates of efficacy; it is important to know not only what works but
also how well it works. Paul Meehl (1987) once said that the basic attribute
that ought to distinguish psychologists from the other helping professions is
“the general scientific commitment not to be fooled or to fool anybody else”
(p. 9). Whether we devote the bulk of our time to research or (more often)
clinical practice, the one defining aspect of psychology as a profession is that
we were all trained as scientists and in its application.
Most current reviews start with the formulation of an analytic framework
that lays out the kinds of questions that the SR is intended to address. This
might involve questions such as, “Do the interventions produce the desired
changes in outcomes of interest (positive or negative)?” and if so, “Do they
do so through the mechanisms specified?” We have opted to ask the GDPs
to meet with the SR teams in the beginning of the process to generate the
analytic framework that they want the review to address. The reason that
this is important is because the GDP can focus the review on any aspect of
clinical change that seems important. It is not necessary to limit attention
to symptom change (although that will likely be one component), and most
HIERARCHY OF EVIDENCE
There are at least two ways that the field has tried to determine what
works with respect to treatment, and they tend to result in somewhat differ-
ent answers. With respect to medications, the Food and Drug Administration
requires at least two well-conducted trials conducted by different research
groups that each produce positive findings (relative to pill-placebo) before it
will let a new medication go to market. That is, we require evidence of speci-
ficity (which presumes efficacy) before we will allow a novel medication to be
sold to the general public. (We also require evidence of safety as well but that
is another story.) A similar approach (albeit one that only requires that a treat-
ment is better than its absence to say that it is efficacious and that adds the mod-
ifier specific when it exceeds a nonspecific control condition) has been applied
to the psychotherapy literature (Chambless & Hollon, 1998). Application of
these criteria results in support for the efficacy of the cognitive and behav-
ior therapies, along with interpersonal psychotherapy, and has earned them
the appellation of “empirically supported treatments” (ESTs) with respect to
depression (DeRubeis & Crits-Christoph, 1998). This approach tends to be
favored by research scientists (who developed the ESTs) and viewed with
suspicion by the majority of practicing clinicians (who tend to practice more
traditional treatments).
SUMMARY
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association. (2010). Practice guideline for the treatment of patients
with major depressive disorder (3rd ed.). Washington DC: American Psychiatric
Press. Retrieved from http://www.psychiatryonline.com/pracGuide/pracGuide
Topic_7.aspx
Angell, M. (2005). The truth about the drug companies: How they deceive us and what
to do about it. New York, NY: Random House.
APA Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice. (2006). Evidence-based
practice in psychology. American Psychologist, 61, 271–285. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1037/0003-066X.61.4.271
Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1963). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs
for research and teaching. Chicago, IL: Rand McNally.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14711-006
Clinical Decision Making in Mental Health Practice, J. J. Magnavita (Editor)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
147
Internet, even English essays can be graded by computers without human
English teachers. Just a few years or decades ago, who would have expected
that all this could work, apart, maybe, from Jules Verne? The world has
changed, and in a dramatically changing world clinical psychology/
psychotherapy does not have the choice of standing still. (p. 222)
Some of the applications are really new, and some are extensions and
continuous developments of preexisting technological and nontechnological
approaches. Ten years after Caspar’s comment, as technology in general and
computers in particular increasingly permeate all domains of life, it would be
astonishing if they were not used for clinical decision making. The task of
decision making includes many subtasks with which technology can help. In
this chapter, we go through these tasks, depicting what technology can do
and which technology is actually available and in use or in development, and
we give a tentative evaluation of how each is utilized. As we all know, in the
domain of technology, predictions about what will turn out to be useful in the
long run can be terribly wrong, so the best we can do is provide perspectives
and arguments.
In line with our view of decisions we have presented on earlier occasions,
the term is not reserved for big, conscious, deliberate decisions. Decisions are
often made in an implicit process of accumulating premises, which ultimately
determine a decision, or rather, a course of action, even though a deliberate
decision is never made (see Chapter 2, this volume). It is plausible that tech-
nology could be used in support of more deliberate decisions, but this should
not make us forget the less deliberate processes that determine our actions
as clinicians and psychotherapists. These less deliberate processes dominate
all the more in adaptive treatment tuning processes (adaptive Indikation in
German), in which small adaptations are made to details in the process of
the actual procedure and in which selective treatment (selektive Indikation) is
fine tuned and set into action.
We focus here on decision making in the everyday clinical situation
and do not address decision making in politics and administration, although
obviously it affects clinical practice and vice versa. For example, decisions
against providing sufficient face-to-face psychotherapy is one factor that may
increase the demand for Internet therapy, and vice versa—that is, the use of
technology for therapy, as in Internet therapy, might influence to what extent
face-to-face therapy needs to be provided. It is obvious that the aggregation
of information for political and administrative decisions can take advantage
of technology. If technology is used professionally, this should contribute to
better informed decisions and less dependency on information provided by
lobbyists who might not work in the interest of high-quality service for those
who need it. An optimistic view is thus that technology works in favor of
patients on this level as well.
For how many disorders and problems do you currently have all available
information that can have an impact on decisions made for an actual patient? It
can be assumed that the honest answer from most clinicians (as well as clinical
scholars!) is at best three or four. How would the gaps be filled when one of the
other disorders or problems showed up? If the clinician has good professional
connections she or he may ask a knowledgeable colleague, most probably by
phone. The probability is high that information will be gathered through the
Internet, or, if the clinician is more thorough, by books selected and/or ordered
via Internet. Even a clinician who does not use technology-provided information
him- or herself has to deal with the fact that patients access these sources. Maggio
(2008) described how practitioners can expand their knowledge of evidence-
based practice by gathering background information, searching filtered resources,
and tracking down original studies through unfiltered databases. Some of the
reported resources and affiliated advances are especially worth mentioning:
! Online textbooks. In contrast to traditional paper textbooks, so
called e-texts, for instance Goodman and Gilman’s Pharmaco-
logical Basis of Therapeutics (Brunton, Chabner, & Knollman,
Feedback Systems
Computers can be used not only for single assessments but also to keep
track of the process and progress of an ongoing therapy, providing feedback to
clinicians. Bickman, Kelley, and Athay (2012) designated such applications
measurement feedback systems (MFSs), and according to them, “the primary pur-
pose of an MFS is to provide feedback that is used to inform clinical practice”
(p. 277), which, of course, means a use for decision making (See Chapter 9,
this volume). What are the benefits of MFSs? One argument mentioned by
Duncan (2012) is their potential to reduce the high dropout rates usually
found in routine practice through the identification of at-risk patients.
They are also being discussed as tools for quality assurance and for opti-
mizing cost effectiveness of therapies (Strauss et al., 2015). Additionally,
MFSs may enhance patients’ active participation in an ongoing therapy
(Sexton, Patterson, & Datchi, 2012).
Feedback systems can be used to predict the course of an individual
treatment by using data from patients who are similar to the current patient
to make assumptions about how this patient can be expected to develop
(Lambert, 2007; Lutz, Tholen, Kosfelder, Grawe, & Schulte, 2005; Percevic,
Lambert, & Kordy, 2004). Some trials have demonstrated several benefits,
including a reduction in the duration of treatments, reduced failure rates for
at-risk patients, greater success rates of clinically significant improvement,
and reduced operating costs (Coyle et al., 2007); other studies did not find
superiority (Strauss et al., 2015).
In sum, feedback systems show a considerable capacity to improve out-
come, especially for problematic therapies, if the therapist is supported in
interpreting and utilizing the feedback (see the section Support of a Decision-
Making Process in Therapy later in this chapter) and if the patient is also
informed (De Jong et al., 2014). It is important to note that technology does
not do the job of improving the procedure but that it helps provide extrinsic
feedback (feedback that is not naturally and without particular efforts pro-
vided by the therapeutic procedure), which can be an essential input for a
therapist trying to understand a patient and an ongoing procedure.
It has long been shown that therapist written records of therapy ses-
sions are highly unreliable (Levenson & Strupp, 1999). Although there are
still big differences between private practice and training or research set-
tings, it has become common to video-record single psychotherapy sessions
or even entire therapies. Not only research protocols but also some training
programs require recording on a regular basis, and more and more therapists
are finding that video is a useful source of information for regular practice
as well. Video records can be used to complete the information a thera-
pist has about a session in which his or her capacity for watching, listen-
ing, and interpreting was limited, or they can be used to give others insight
into a therapy (for supervision or peer consulting; see the section Online
Supervision later in this chapter). For patients, it has become common to
be video-recorded, and hardly any of them ask anymore whether this will be
on the TV news.
Manring, Greenberg, Gregory, and Gallinger (2011) described how
to set up a video-recording system and which issues should be considered.
In principle, only a computer with a webcam and a microphone (usually
built in on a laptop) are needed for reasonably good records. Video-recording
has become so inexpensive that the cost of a reasonably good picture and
sound recording system is no longer an argument against installing and using
video-recording systems. In advanced video systems using a server, as we have
installed in our training clinic, video recordings are stored on a secured server
As Comer and Barlow (2014) argued, for problems that do not occur
frequently it would be inefficient to train a large number of clinicians so that
they would have all necessary information available at all times. Technology
can help to quickly provide the information needed to make a clinical deci-
sion, and it also enables well-informed decisions for rare disorders. Providing
conceptual and empirical information has been an issue, as described earlier.
Here we discuss more direct support of decision making.
A branch of decision-making research has proposed basing clinical deci-
sions on explicit probability calculations, even by a general practitioner on a
home visit. An “actuarial” or “statistical” way of making decisions is claimed
to be superior to “clinical” decision making, which includes soft information,
qualitative decisions, and intuitive information processing. There is no doubt
that statistical decision making would be a field for the use of technology. We
are convinced, though, that although the inclusion of probability informa-
tion in clinical decision making is highly recommended, primary reliance on
it in mental health and psychotherapy-related clinical decision making is
not a viable approach. We therefore do not detail here how technology and
computers could be used in such an approach.
Beutler and Harwood (2004; Beutler, Williams, & Norcross, 2011;
Harwood et al., 2011) described a web-based systematic treatment selection
system approach that is based on an Aptitude × Treatment Interaction design,
providing information about adequate interventions or therapeutic styles.
Before intake, certain patient characteristics (e.g., the patient’s therapist
preferences, most dominant symptoms, demographic data) are assessed via
a web-delivered self-report measure, resulting in a tailored self-help tool for
the patient and a more detailed report for the clinician. During the treatment
process, a patient’s symptoms are continuously being assessed, and the result-
ing change trajectory is compared with a projected change trajectory, showing
the expected development according to initial information. This corresponds
largely to the feedback systems described earlier. Harwood et al. (2011) fur-
ther described how certain patient characteristics, such as functional impair-
ment, coping style, subjective distress, social support, reactance–resistance
level, problem complexity–chronicity, and stages of change–readiness are
being used by systematic treatment selection to assist treatment planning.
Videoconferencing
Webinars
Blended Treatment
TRAINING
textbooks
video tape
trainee’s
view
possibilities of
experts’ view in
exploration
free language
Figure 6.1. Feedback based on latent semantic analysis; see explanation in the text.
Online Supervision
Virtual Reality
Virtual reality has been used for the assessment of medication manage-
ment skills (Kurtz, Baker, Pearlson, & Astur, 2007). Beutler and Harwood
(2004) proposed a virtual reality–based training in their empirically sup-
ported and multifaceted systematic treatment selection model (see also the
section Support for Decision Making earlier in this chapter). As far as we
know, only a pilot model has been developed.
Comer and Barlow (2014; Rogers, 2003) pointed out the importance
of four factors for the acceptance of new approaches: (a) limited complex-
ity; (b) trialability, which means the possibility of trying out something new
before a decision for or against its permanent use is made; (c) compatibility
with existing approaches; and (d) the testability of results. Acceptance will
also depend on financial compensation; for example in telemedicine, face-
to-face contact is replaced by virtual contact, but where technology comple-
ments human activity, the tools and procedures must also be financed.
Beyond acceptance, the skillful use of technology is an issue. Depending
on how close the use of the technology is to the everyday use, it may be very
demanding in terms of resources and motivation to introduce and effectively
use technology. Dedication as well as communicative and didactic skills of
personnel may become crucial, along with employee retention in the interest
of not losing knowledge and experience with turnover on the job (Comer &
Barlow, 2014). As a basis for appropriate training, requirements for competent
use must be defined by the developers of applications (Comer & Barlow, 2014).
RISKS
Abbass, A., Arthey, S., Elliott, J., Fedak, T., Nowoweiski, D., Markovski, J., &
Nowoweiski, S. (2011). Web-conference supervision for advanced psycho-
therapy training: A practical guide. Psychotherapy, 48, 109–118. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1037/a0022427
ACES Task Force. (2011, January 18). Best practices in clinical supervision: ACES
Task Force report. Retrieved from http://www.saces.org/resources/documents/
aces_best_practices.doc
Axelson, D. A., Bertocci, M. A., Lewin, D. S., Trubnick, L. S., Birmaher, B.,
Williamson, D. E., . . . Dahl, R. E. (2003). Measuring mood and complex behav-
ior in natural environments: Use of ecological momentary assessment in pedi-
atric affective disorders. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 13,
253–266. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/104454603322572589
Bartle-Haring, S., Silverthorn, B. C., Meyer, K., & Toviessi, P. (2009). Does live
supervision make a difference? A multilevel analysis. Journal of Marital and Fam-
ily Therapy, 35, 406–414. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1752-0606.2009.00124.x
Bauer, S., Percevic, R., Okon, E., Meermann, R., & Kordy, H. (2003). Use of text
messaging in the aftercare of patients with bulimia nervosa. European Eating
Disorders Review, 11, 279–290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/erv.521
Berger, T. (2004). Computer-based technological applications in psychotherapy
training. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 60, 301–315. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
jclp.10265
Berger, T., Caspar, F., Richardson, R., Kneubühler, B., Sutter, D., & Andersson, G.
(2011). Internet-based treatment of social phobia: A randomized controlled
trial comparing unguided with two types of guided self-help. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 49, 158–169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.12.007
Berger, T., Hohl, E., & Caspar, F. (2010). Internetbasierte Therapie der sozialen Pho-
bie: Ergebnisse einer 6-Monate-Katamnese [Internet-based therapy of social
phobia: Findings in a 6-month follow-up]. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie
und Psychotherapie, 39, 217–221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1026/1616-3443/a000050
Beutler, L. E., & Harwood, T. M. (2004). Virtual reality in psychotherapy training.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 60, 317–330. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jclp.10266
Beutler, L. E., Williams, R. R., & Norcross, J. N. (2011). Science and research.
Retrieved from http://innerlife.com
Bickman, L., Kelley, S. D., & Athay, M. (2012). The technology of measurement
feedback systems. Couple and Family Psychology, 1, 274–284. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1037/a0031022
Brunton, L., Chabner, B., & Knollman, B. (2010). Goodman and Gilman’s the
pharmacological basis of therapeutics (12th ed.). New York, NY: McGraw Hill
Professional.
Buchanan, T., Ali, T., Heffernan, T. M., Ling, J., Parrott, A. C., Rodgers, J., &
Scholey, A. B. (2005). Nonequivalence of on-line and paper-and-pencil psy-
This model of collaborative treatment for high stakes cases was developed and practiced with John
Santopietro, MD.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14711-007
Clinical Decision Making in Mental Health Practice, J. J. Magnavita (Editor)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
175
situations may be life threatening and the outcome possibly catastrophic in
terms of harm to self or violence toward others. There are far too many exam-
ples of these high-stakes situations; for example, in recent years our nation
has struggled to understand and prevent school shootings and other cases of
mass murderers who have slipped through the treatment net. Most clinicians
who have been in practice for some time have run into similar cases.
In this chapter, some of the important issues that are inimical to mak-
ing decisions when the stakes are high are presented. A model is presented
of collaborative–shared decision making, developed in collaboration with a
psychiatrist colleague, that has been found to be effective. This model is dif-
ferent from most decision-making models in that it expands the traditional
dyadic configuration (i.e., between patient and clinician) by utilizing a tri-
adic configuration (i.e., among patient; two clinicians; treatment team; and
other significant parties such as families, community representatives, etc.).
This model is anchored in system theory, which we used as a framework for
unifying and orienting our clinical work (Magnavita, 2012; Magnavita &
Anchin, 2014). Human behavior and mental and relational processes can
be understood by incorporating complexity or chaos theory, which are recent
iterations of system theory. Using chaos and complexity theory allows one to
see patterns in systems as they emerge and/or are repeated. These patterns rep-
resent attractor states, which are essentially convergent processes such as we
might see in a severely eating-disordered family member who organizes the
family system around the management of his or her illness (Magnavita, 2005a).
Recurrent processes have been explained as follows:
The strength of an attractor is measured by the depth of its basin. A deep
attractor is well-developed and behavioral repertoires are frequently
drawn to this attractor. As behavioral patterns are repeated, the attrac-
tor is strengthened. In any given interaction, there are likely to be several
available attractors in the state space, with some presenting a stronger
pull than others. (Stanton & Welsh, 2012, p. 21)
Complex systems operate in a nonlinear holistic fashion, viewed through
the lenses of the various levels and domains of the biopsychosocial model
(Magnavita & Anchin, 2014). The use of theory in decision analytics is cov-
ered in Chapter 3 in this volume.
Some cases give rise to fear in behavioral and mental health clinicians;
most often these are cases where the stakes are high and the outcome uncertain.
Most clinicians can easily recall those experiences in which decision making
was risky and the potential for disaster loomed large. Such cases are generally
Information
Values
Preferences
QUADRATIC DECISION-MAKING
As has been discussed earlier in this volume, the way a question is framed
influences the answer. Quadratic modeling can guard against some of these
framing risks.
An example is a case of a married woman in her early 30s who, prior to
being seen by one of us, was hospitalized and diagnosed with bipolar disorder
for which she was placed on a mood stabilizer. She was referred for pharma-
cological treatment and medication management. My colleague wondered
if the diagnosis was accurate. The patient wanted to come off her medica-
tion because she was considering having a child. Quadratic decision making
helped to assess the risks involved in weaning her off medication. The first
question we sought to assess was the veracity of the diagnosis, and the ques-
tion was, “Is the patient suffering from bipolar disorder?” Second, we wanted
to decide if she should continue to be treated for bipolar disorder. The ques-
tion we posed was, “Should she be supported in coming off her medication
when she is trying to conceive a child?”
We addressed our questions in as systematic a fashion as possible.
We wanted to know what evidence showed that the patient suffered from
a bipolar disorder. The evidence we had on intake was that she was diag-
nosed with bipolar during a brief hospitalization. More information was
gathered through a history and clinical interview and then a recommen-
dation that she be further evaluated with psychometric testing for more
data. A Millon Multiaxial Clinical Inventory–III (Millon, Millon, Davis,
& Grossman, 2009) was administered and did not endorse bipolar dis-
order. Instead, from history and interview, it appeared that the patient
suffered, at least in part, from an undiagnosed posttraumatic stress disorder
(Magnavita, 2005b).
DECISION-MAKING AIDS
Team Approach
A team approach can sound like an overused cliché. Respect and trust
are essential to a functional team. Part of the benefit of a multispecialty team
approach is that the perspectives offered by various members can be a strong
corrective to the bias of one’s model and the underlying beliefs that inform
one’s clinical work. Our goal was to establish open and regular lines of com-
munication with other health care providers who were involved in the cases
we were treating.
One of the most helpful experiences for many families was the oppor-
tunity to have both of us present at collaborative care meetings. These were
often necessary in situations where there was deterioration in a patient,
couple, or family’s condition. These downward spirals are the situations that
in the past would have resulted in a hospitalization. Instead, with a review
of treatment and modification of the treatment plan we were often success-
ful in averting hospitalizations or in keeping them limited except in cases
of extreme necessity where stabilization required a longer episode of care.
RISK MANAGEMENT
Treating high-risk patients can cause high levels of anxiety for clini-
cians because of the chaos and uncertainty involved. Continual monitoring
of the therapeutic alliance is critical for risk management. Shared collabora-
tive decision making is one way to tolerate the uncertainty and risk. Anxiety
in the care team can at times run rather high, and on occasion vicarious trau-
matization of the team or a team member is not uncommon. Many high-risk
patients have suffered from a variety of traumatic experiences and develop-
mental traumata. The clinicians who treat high-risk cases should be trained
extensively in trauma and be alert to the possibility of suffering from compas-
sion fatigue and vicarious traumatization.
Personality Systematics
MANAGING COUNTERTRANSFERENCE–
TRANSFERENCE REACTIONS
SUMMARY
REFERENCES
Ahn, W. K., Proctor, C. C., & Flanagan, E. H. (2009). Mental health clinicians’
beliefs about the biological, psychological, and environmental bases of
mental disorders. Cognitive Science, 33, 147–182. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
j.1551-6709.2009.01008.x
Betan, E., Heim, A. K., Zittel Conklin, C., & Westen, D. (2005). Countertransfer-
ence phenomena and personality pathology in clinical practice: An empirical
investigation. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 890–898.
Gottman, J. M., & Gottman, J. S. (2013). Difficulties with clients in Gottman
method couples therapy. In A. W. Wolf, M. R. Goldfried, & J. C. Muran (Eds.),
Transforming negative reactions to clients (pp. 91–112). Washington, DC: Ameri-
can Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/13940-004
Heatherington, L., Friedlander, M. L., & Escudero, V. (2013). Managing negative
reactions to clients in conjoint therapy: It’s not all in the family. In A. W. Wolf,
M. R. Goldfried, & J. C. Muran (Eds.), Transforming negative reactions to clients
(pp. 113–137). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Institute of Medicine. (2001). Crossing the quality chasm: A new health system for the
21st century. Washington, DC: Author.
Kahneman, D., & Klein, G. (2009). Conditions for intuitive expertise: A failure to
disagree. American Psychologist, 64, 515–526. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016755
Magnavita, J. J. (Ed.). (2004). Handbook of personality disorders: Theory and practice.
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Magnavita, J. J. (2005a). Systems theory foundation of personality, psychopathology,
and psychotherapy. In S. Strack (Ed.), Handbook of personology and psychopathology
(pp. 140–163). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Magnavita, J. J. (2005b). Using the MCMI–III for treatment planning and to
enhance clinical efficacy. In R. J. Craig (Ed.), New directions in interpreting the
Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory—III (MCMI–III) (pp. 165–184). Hoboken,
NJ: Wiley.
Magnavita, J. J. (Ed.). (2010a). Evidence-based treatment of personality dysfunction:
Principles, methods, and processes. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/12130-000
Magnavita, J. J. (2010b). When the evidence base is scant: Some considerations
in the ethical treatment of personality dysfunction. In J. J. Magnavita, K. N.
Levy, K. L. Critchfield, & J. L. Lebow, Ethical considerations in treatment of
personality dysfunction: Using evidence, principles, and clinical judgment. Profes-
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14711-008
Clinical Decision Making in Mental Health Practice, J. J. Magnavita (Editor)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
193
Levy, Critchfield, & Lebow, 2010) has emphasized that use of principles is
essential for ethical intervention in areas where there is little empirical evi-
dence, such as decision making for patients with comorbid disorders; patients
with understudied disorders; patients with unusually severe or chronic dis-
orders or unusual cultural experience; or patients who have shown little benefit
from previous treatment trials.
A focus on principles represents a return to the conceptual origins of
most manualized approaches, which are premised on guiding principles about
the etiology, maintenance, and change processes governing psychopathology.
For example, the psychodynamic “blank screen” of neutrality—a technique—
is intended to make transference patterns more available for interpretation,
insight, and change. Similarly, in cognitive behavior therapy (CBT) for anxi-
ety disorders, graduated exposure protocols are intended to inhibit avoidance
responses and stimulate new learning. In each approach, specific techniques
serve as tools for engaging a deeper change principle. Presumably, if a given
principle is validly linked to problems and change processes, any technique
that activates it in a specific context will be effective.
Psychotherapy research has shown that many therapy approaches have
efficacy for a wide variety of disorders. Generally equivalent results, however,
have been found across protocols for the same disorders (Wampold, 2001).
One possible explanation for equivalent results is that these protocols may
use techniques that activate the same or similar underlying mechanisms. For
example, cognitive restructuring, eye movement desensitization and repro-
cessing, and psychodynamic–interpersonal approaches all impact trauma
symptoms to some degree (Chard, Schuster, & Resick, 2012; Gallagher &
Resick, 2012; Nayak, Powers, & Foa, 2012; Schottenbauer, Glass, Arnkoff,
& Gray, 2008). This may be because they all require some degree of “being
with” traumatic material in the context of a safe, therapeutic “secure base”
(Cloitre, Stovall-McClough, Miranda, & Chemtob, 2004; Keller, Zoellner,
& Feeny, 2010; Kinsler, 2014).
Research to establish treatment efficacy has primarily emphasized the
randomized controlled trial (RCT). RCTs frame the primary research ques-
tion as, “Do a set of specific techniques applied in a systematic way produce
an average benefit for a specified population?” Internal controls are strong,
largely owing to random assignment to groups and clear operationalization of
techniques. Evidence produced from RCTs is powerful for answering whether
average effects can be attributed directly to the impact of the treatment
package. However, RCTs tell us relatively little about how individuals might
respond to the treatment, especially those differing significantly from RCT
samples, such as those with comorbid diagnoses or any number of cultural
differences (Holt et al., 2013). RCTs also do not aid clinicians in select-
ing or tailoring treatments to best match individual strengths, weaknesses,
Emancipate Separate
Active Reactive
Attack Recoil Love
Love
Control Submit
complements
Figure 8.1. Structural analysis of social behavior simplified cluster model illustrating complementary behaviors. Behaviors focused on others are boldface,
and behaviors focused on the self are underlined. In this adapted version, the focus on Other and Self are shown in separate “surfaces,” and lines and
arrow are added to illustrate complementary conceptual pairings. From Interpersonal Diagnosis and Treatment of Personality Disorders (2nd ed., p. 55),
by L. S. Benjamin, 1996/2003, New York, NY: Guilford Press. Copyright 1996/2003 by Guilford Press. Adapted with permission.
are opposites (e.g., Blame and Affirm; Protect and Ignore). Similarity and
opposition are predicted to be unstable. The antithesis or antidote refers to a
position that is maximally different from another behavior on all three SASB
dimensions.
Ted entered individual and group treatment with a female trainee super-
vised by clinic psychology staff as his therapist for a convicted sexual offense.
He was a youthful-looking 40-year-old, and his offense involved having sex
with a teenage girl he had pursued after seeing her be turned away from a
bar. He was of average intelligence with a stable job in finance. He demon-
strated strong tendencies toward grandiosity, exertion of control over others,
and use of seeming compliance and indirect resistance to demands placed
on him, combining features of both narcissistic and passive–aggressive PDs.
Ted’s interactions with others, particularly women, were often marked by
contempt or objectification. During treatment, he began dating a reportedly
“young-looking” 18-year-old woman (Heidi), which greatly concerned his
probation officer. When confronted in therapy, Ted insisted that his therapist
was “making a big deal out of nothing.” He received feedback from group
therapy members that his choice seemed reckless at best and a return to his
major problem pattern at worst. A week later Ted reported to the group that
he had told the young woman that he could not see her again. He said he
made this decision to make his treatment as smooth as possible, and she was
not mentioned again. On his last night in group, Ted told each member what
he perceived to be their greatest flaws, and he said to his therapist, “If you
weren’t my therapist, I would have asked you out a long time ago.” He then
failed to schedule any of his planned individual follow-up sessions with the
clinic director.
2. Former therapist:
Text: “Hi Ted—I am very glad to hear
Separate you are doing well. Congratula!ons on
your marriage! Thank you for the
invita!on to coffee with you and your
Affirm wife, as well. I will have to decline the
invita!on, though. There are strict
guidelines for therapist client contact,
even when therapy has ended. S!ll,
thank you for the thought, and please
know I wish you both the best.”
4. Former therapist: Text: “Hi Ted. I was sorry to see that you seem
upset and may be in need of support … I am no
Separate longer employed at the clinic … Dr. Smith
[supervisor] is now your point of contact … I
Ignore Disclose
shared your texts and voicemail message with
him, and we agreed that it might be helpful for
you to talk with him … I will no longer be
communica!ng with you about these issues,
and if you try to contact me, I am sorry to say
that I will need to block your number. Again,
Dr. Smith would be happy to discuss your
concerns with you, and can be reached at … ”
Control
Sulk
potentially hazardous. As seen in Figure 8.2, the therapist’s final text empha-
sized interpersonal distance in several forms but also included interpersonal
control worded to further enforce the distance (contact the clinic, not me;
if you contact me, I will block your number). Ted’s final communication
retained a hint of hostile enmeshment but appeared to largely follow the pull
of complementarity, and he distanced himself from the entire conversation.
Ted has not contacted the therapist again over an interval of several years.
The next example further demonstrates how interpersonal principles of
complementarity can be used with a case formulation to enhance adaptive
and discourage maladaptive patterns.
(Sulk) Trust
(Trust)
4. Therapist:
Phone conversa!on: Collabora!vely
discussed that part of her feels that
suicide is inevitable, and it can be hard
to see why not to just “get it over
(Ignore) Affirm with.” Pa!ent agreed that the part of
her that is !red is the part of her that
has been figh!ng to live, and cares
deeply about her sisters.
Protect
Figure 8.3. Interactions with Annie. Behaviors in parentheses show interactions with
the Regressive Loyalist (RL) in contrast to interactions with the Growth Collaborator
(GC) parts of Annie.
5. Annie: Text: “About ready to give up....I can’t
do this. I might text but I don’t want to
talk.”
(Wall Off)
Trust
6. Therapist:
(Blame)
(Sulk)
8. Therapist:
Protect
and make the decision to let them go in favor of healthier ways. The many
implications of an IRT approach to treatment is beyond our present scope;
primary emphasis here is how relational principles can optimally be used
in concert with a clearly articulated case formulation to address severe
clinical problems. In IRT, relating with the patient can often be thought
of as a relationship not with one unitary patient but two separate persons:
one showing RL patterns, another showing GC patterns. Thinking of the
patient as having separate parts allows for more targeted use of relational
interventions.
Figure 8.3 shows a sample of postsession text exchanges initiated by
Annie to her therapist, with the relationship process with the GC in standard
text and the relationship with the RL in parentheses. The sequence illus-
trates how Annie’s therapist responded to differentially reinforce adaptive
versus maladaptive patterns as defined in Annie’s case formulation. Given
Annie’s strong withdrawal and sense of abandoned alienation, her therapist’s
struggle is often to simply create and maintain a collaborative alliance in pur-
suit of a healthy goal; interpersonally, this means to increase friendly forms
of enmeshment in the therapeutic relationship (so that the same themes can
also be inspected in her self-concept and relationships with others). Annie’s
old RL pattern, however, is to pull for her therapist to recapitulate her history
of abandonment by withdrawing and attempting to frustrate her therapist’s
attempts to connect and provide help.
In the first text, Annie places her therapist in a bind, expressing sui-
cidality and hopelessness while also requesting that her therapist give per-
mission for suicide and confirm that her family will not be affected by her
loss. This is clearly a message from Annie’s RL and contains elements of
trust in the therapist (albeit maladaptively directed), coupled with neglect
of her therapist’s feelings and role and clear walling off (“don’t try calling
me”). Annie’s pattern in the past had been simply to implement a suicide
attempt, so there is a hint that the GC was also present by the fact of her
reaching out to her therapist in any form (“I don’t know why I’m telling you
this . . .”). Although small, it suggests trust and the potential for greater
collaboration in the future.
One relatively common option in crafting a response to the first text
in health care settings might be along the lines of: “I’m sorry to hear you are
upset. But, it is after hours. If you’re suicidal I need you to call the crisis center
per our safety contract.” Annie had received this sort of response before. It is
kind, professional, and appropriate in many respects but would very likely be
taken by her as further evidence that she is burdening her therapist and will
be abandoned by her sooner or later. The therapist could have responded in
an attempt to block suicidality by directly engaging the RL. But any attempt
A patient I’m seeing now doesn’t like [the manualized approach I’m
using] for specific reasons, and it also hasn’t worked for her in the past.
But, how can I respond to my patient’s needs and still be evidence
based? Isn’t it unethical to deviate from the manual if it is empirically
supported?
Ahmed, M., Westra, H. A., & Constantino, M. J. (2012). Early therapy interpersonal
process differentiating clients high and low in outcome expectations. Psycho-
therapy Research, 22, 731–745. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2012.724538
APA Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice. (2006). Evidence-based
practice in psychology. American Psychologist, 61, 271–285. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1037/0003-066X.61.4.271
Benjamin, L. S. (1979). Structural analysis of differentiation failure. Psychiatry, 42,
1–23.
Benjamin, L. S. (1996). Interpersonal diagnosis and treatment of personality disorders
(2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Benjamin, L. S. (2006). Interpersonal reconstructive therapy: An integrative, personality-
based treatment for complex cases. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Benjamin, L. S., & Critchfield, K. L. (2010). An interpersonal perspective on ther-
apy alliances and techniques. In J. C. Muran & J. P. Barber (Eds.), The thera-
peutic alliance: An evidence-based approach to practice and training (pp. 123–149).
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Benjamin, L. S., Rothweiler, J. C., & Critchfield, K. L. (2006). The use of struc-
tural analysis of social behavior (SASB) as an assessment tool. Annual
Review of Clinical Psychology, 2, 83–109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.
clinpsy.2.022305.095337
Carson, R. C. (1969). Interaction concepts of personality. Oxford, England: Aldine.
Castonguay, L. G., & Beutler, L. E. (2006). Principles of therapeutic change that work.
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Chard, K. M., Schuster, J. L., & Resick, P. A. (2012). Empirically supported psychological
treatments: Cognitive processing therapy. In J. G. Beck & D. M. Sloan (Eds.), The
Oxford handbook of traumatic stress disorders (pp. 439–448). New York, NY: Oxford
University Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195399066.013.0030
Cloitre, M., Stovall-McClough, K. C., Miranda, R., & Chemtob, C. M. (2004).
Therapeutic alliance, negative mood regulation, and treatment outcome in
child abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clini-
cal Psychology, 72, 411–416. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.72.3.411
Coady, N. F. (1991). The association between client and therapist interpersonal pro-
cesses and outcomes in psychodynamic psychotherapy. Research on Social Work
Practice, 1, 122–138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/104973159100100202
Critchfield, K. L. (2012). Tailoring common treatment principles to fit individ-
ual personalities. Journal of Personality Disorders, 26, 108–125. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1521/pedi.2012.26.1.108
Critchfield, K. L. (2013, May). Interpersonal reconstructive therapy: Methods, mech-
anisms, and findings to date. Presented at the symposium Interpersonal Foun-
dations of Adjustment, Health, and Change: Current Perspectives on Lorna
In the search for the best psychological methods, models, and services
for helping clients, it has become clear that effective psychological services
are those that include the strongest available research evidence delivered with
clinical expertise and that are in line with patient values (APA Presidential
Task Force, 2006; Hollon et al., 2014; Sackett, Straus, Richardson, Rosenberg,
& Haynes, 2000). Yet, debates have continued about the role of research and
the mechanisms needed to integrate it into the practice of psychotherapy.
These debates are represented in the vibrant series of publications examin-
ing the strengths and weaknesses of the research and its potential impact on
practice (Hollon et al., 2014; Sexton, Alexander, & Mease, 2003; Sexton
& Coop Gordon, 2009; Westen, Novotny, & Thompson-Brenner, 2004).
There is considerable outcome and process research that can guide practice in
individual, couple, and family treatment (Sexton, Datchi, Evans, LaFollette,
& Wright, 2013). However, for many practitioners research remains distant
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14711-009
Clinical Decision Making in Mental Health Practice, J. J. Magnavita (Editor)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
223
and broad and includes nonspecific findings with clients and problems that
are not familiar. It is no surprise that those in practice frequently see research
as having little impact on daily clinical decision making. Although both
research and clinical experience are important, we still know little about
how they can be successfully integrated into the daily practice of clinical
professionals.
One thing we do know is that the therapist is a critical element in the
delivery of any type of psychological treatment. Growing empirical support
shows that treatment outcomes are systematically related to the provider, above
and beyond the specific treatment, which suggests the importance of under-
standing clinical decision making in enhancing positive client outcomes (Crits-
Christoph et al., 1991; Huppert et al., 2001; Kim, Wampold, & Bolt, 2006;
Wampold & Brown, 2005). These findings should not be a surprise. In a real-life
clinical setting it is the clinician who must make the ongoing decisions about
adapting treatment on the basis of whether a client is improving, remaining sta-
ble, or deteriorating. Sexton (2007) described the role of the therapist as one of
a translator between the assumptions and mechanisms of the treatment model
and the interactions with the client necessary to promote change. It is through
the clinical decision-making process, conducted by the therapist, that the trans-
lation of treatment models (whether general or specific) to client occurs. The
APA Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice (APA Presidential
Task Force, 2006) suggested that the clinical expertise needed for good decision
making is the skillful and flexible delivery of treatment with the highest prob-
ability of success. As such, clinical decision making plays an important part of
the clinical expertise needed to integrate research, theory, and experience into
the complex formula of successful psychological treatments.
Our understanding of the components and processes of clinical deci-
sion making is still in its early stages. We do know that effective clinical
decision making requires more than clinical experience and individual ther-
apist judgment. When reviewing the relationship between clinician expe-
rience (based on years of experience and amount of training) and clinical
outcomes, the conclusions of the major reviews are mixed at best (Beutler
et al., 2004). Clinicians have not demonstrated high levels of reliability
at the tasks of diagnosis, prediction, and case formulation (Garb, 2005).
Clinical judgment alone seems vulnerable to the same sources of error that
often distort ordinary human judgment, including confirmatory bias, self-
enhancement bias, the availability heuristic, and a greater emphasis on per-
sonal experience than general information. Clinical decision making requires
more than just experience. Shapiro, Friedberg, and Bardenstein (2006) sug-
gested that clinical reasoning consists of therapists’ informal analysis, deci-
sion making, and planning based on a wide variety of inputs. These include
research findings, observations of the client, assessment of etiology, theories
ILLUSTRATIVE EXAMPLES
OQ-Analyst
Clinical
Feedback
Alerts Status
(requiring immediate attention)
(intended for a “quick look”)
Figure 9.1. Information domains in the functional family therapy–clinical feedback system.
Treatment Planning
(progress notes & session plans) Clients & Clinical Measures
Therapist
Information
Treatment History Deomographic Information (Therapist)
Service Delivery Profile
FFT
INTEGRATING ONGOING MEASUREMENT
Critical Events
Session
Information
session and activity management Session Type, Time, Location, Participation
& scheduling
Figure 9.2. Domains of the functional family therapy (FFT)–clinical feedback system.
list. This prompts clinicians, in a very user-friendly way, to complete the needed
material in a timely manner. The Therapist Information section includes adher-
ence rates, which consist of service delivery information that can be compared
with other cases within the therapist caseload, or to others in the same agency
or to the entire FFT database. These comparisons give content to the feedback.
Session Information includes basic data about sessions (date, length, session
goals, focus, critical incidents) as well as rating of client improvement and
session success. Integrating these three domains of information into a platform
for the primary work functions of the clinicians (scheduling, therapist informa-
tion, and session information) allow for these major functions of the clinician
to be represented so that the FFT-CFS is thus designed to move the MFS into
the center of all clinical activities (see Figure 9.2).
Therapist Input
(Progress Notes)
-Assessment
-Anticipated Session Goals
- Observed Session Progress
Individualized
Session Next Session Plan
(type of session/ Clinical Feedback updated treatment goals
contact/time/treatment
INTEGRATING ONGOING MEASUREMENT
Client:
Completed by: Date:
INSTRUCTIONS:
Please answer the following questions as honestly as you can based on your individual experience of
the counseling session that you have just attended. Your answers to these questions will NOT be used
to evaluate the therapist.
Completely Completely
1 2 3 4 5
Disagree Agree
Family &
0 1 2 3 4 5 6
struggles are.
Youth reported
5. I think what is going on in counseling is
0 1 2 3 4 5 6
important and I am taking part.
Youth reported
This Youth’s Behaviors, Thoughts
and Feelings SFSS-Weekly-Youth
Symptom
INSTRUCTIONS:
Please answer the following questions as honestly as you can based on your experience of your ado -
Never Hardly Ever Sometimes Often Very Often
lescents behavior.
1. ...feel unhappy or sad? 1 2 3 4 5
Treatment
Never Hardly Ever Sometimes Often Very Often
Severity
4. ...disobey adults (not do what adults told
you to do)?
1 2 3 4 5
SIS-EM 5. ...threaten or bully others? 1 2 3 4 5
6. ...feel afraid that other kids would laugh 7. ...have a hard time waiting your turn? 1 2 3 4 5
1 2 3 4 5
at you?
Progress
Client: 8. ...feel nervous and/or shy around other
7. ...have a hard time waiting your turn? 1 2 3 4 5 Completed by: Date: people?
1 2 3 4 5
8. ...feel nervous and/or shy around other 9. ...have a hard time sitting still? 1 2 3 4 5
1 2 3 4 5
people? INSTRUCTIONS:
Below are several statements about the session or meeting you just had with your counselor. 10. ...cry easily? 1 2 3 4 5
9. ...have a hard time sitting still? 1 2 3 4 5
11. ...annoy other people on purpose? 1 2 3 4 5
10. ...cry easily? 1 2 3 4 5
Not at all Only a little Some A lot Entirely 12. ...argue with adults? 1 2 3 4 5
11. ...annoy other people on purpose? 1 2 3 4 5
1. I feel blamed for the problems in my 13. ...drink alcohol (beer, wine, hard
12. ...argue with adults? 1 2 3 4 5 family.
1 2 3 4 5
liquor)?
13. ...drink alcohol (beer, wine, hard 2. I feel negative toward other members of 14. ...want to runaway (go AWOL)? 1 2 3 4 5
1 2 3 4 5
liquor)? my family.
14. ...throw things when he/she was mad? 1 2 3 4 5 3. I understand better something about
others (my parents/child, brothers/sis -
15. ...interrupt others? 1 2 3 4 5 ters).
INSTRUCTIONS:
ONGOING TREATMENT
Figure 9.3. Clinical decision-making process in functional family therapy (FFT)–clinical feedback system.
237
information about the session itself (e.g., its length, type, focus) and infor-
mation from clients (about their status and the change trajectory they are
experiencing) and therapist (from their case and session planning notes).
At the end of a session, client measures are taken by each family member
either by paper and pencil (to be entered into the system later) or electroni-
cally through tablet or computer devices that allow the client to directly
enter information. Once entered, the MFS translates the data into useful
and digestible feedback on the initial baseline status of the youth and family
based on their level of family functioning and youth symptom levels. The
multidimensional nature of this MFS makes two levels of feedback available
to review after each treatment session. First, clinicians can quickly review
the status of each family member on the level of youth symptoms, the impact
of treatment, and the level of progress they experience. This is intended to
alert the clinician to areas that need immediate attention. There is also client-
specific feedback that details each measure, subscale, and question answer for
detailed case planning. In treatment planning the system allows the clinician
to look broadly at data (using the at-a-glance status indicators) or more specifi-
cally through the extensive feedback reports that show client score and how
that score compares with the norms of the instrument. Once the clinician
has reviewed the client feedback on the family’s functioning level and the
youth symptom level, the clinician completes a session planning guide that
integrates client feedback on level of youth symptom and family functioning
into the next session plan. Over time that process continues, resulting in a
focused and tailored approach to matching the treatment intervention with
the specific client.
Client, therapy, and session information can also be useful at the specific
phases of treatment. For example, assessment at baseline or intake allows the
clinician to gather broad and general diagnostics to help in deciding how
to deliver treatment. Baseline assessments provide an opportunity to systemati-
cally understand the client variables that might be important to consider
in planning the types of therapeutic interventions needed. In the FFT-CFS
system, there are two baseline measures used to ensure fit for the intervention
(family and symptom) and to identify areas of functioning that might be targets
of understanding the problems or change (family and symptoms—internalizing
and externalizing). Two specific domains are represented: family functioning
and the other youth symptom level. Both are important because they help
adjust treatment if it is not working and results are not apparent, and they also
serve as clinically relevant measures of treatment outcome.
In the early phases of treatment (the engagement and motivation
phase) the FFT model focuses on the within-family risk factors, systematically
addressing within-family blame, negativity, problem alignment, and alliance.
Changing these within-family relational interactions is intended to result
SUMMARY
REFERENCES
Any letter, e-mail, or contact from your licensing board certainly grabs
your attention. For most, the contact is generally associated with license
renewal. However, receiving notification from your licensing board that a
complaint by a current or former patient or client has been filed against you
surely gets the heart pumping at near panic rate. Making good clinical decisions
and using good judgment, providing good standards of care, and using sound
risk management procedures will keep those latter types of letters either to a
minimum or nonexistent. Obviously, some patient populations who are seen
in a psychotherapy practice—such as those involved with custody issues and
evaluations, parent coordination, and forensic evaluations as well as high-
risk mental health patients/clients (those with personality disorders)—may
involve more contentious interactions. These patient population groups
have a higher risk of licensing board complaints than others. However, most
psychologists, especially those in private practice, adhere to ethical and
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14711-010
Clinical Decision Making in Mental Health Practice, J. J. Magnavita (Editor)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
245
professional standards of care and have an understanding of proper risk
management. If the standards are followed, the potential of interacting
with a licensing board in a negative way becomes greatly reduced.
There are many reasons why we chose to become psychologists. Those
of us who have chosen to go into private practice, whether independent
or group, have dedicated ourselves to providing high-quality care to those
we serve. But there are events (external or internal stressors) that seem to
close the door to good judgment and decision making. So, let’s jump into
the deep end of the pool right at the outset of this chapter with a case study
to begin illustrating what we mean by clinical decision making and risk
management.
A well-known and well-liked psychologist (Dr. A.) accepted a new patient
into his practice. The 37-year-old woman (Ms. Z.) presented on time for
her appointment, was appropriately attired, was pleasant in mood, and
seemed comfortable in the office as she engaged in small talk about the
amount of rain they were having during the first few moments of the intake
session. As the patient and psychologist settled in for the intake session,
the psychologist followed his normal procedure of providing written
information on the Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act
(HIPAA), his office policies (to include confidentiality, emergencies, and
even social networking), and his fees, and then he verbally explained
issues related to confidentiality and further explained his “fee for ser-
vice” policy, which included some narrative on how he does not belong
to any insurance panels but would be happy to provide the patient with
an invoice or statement of office visits that she could submit for possible
reimbursement. Ms. Z. then told him that she would check with her hus-
band’s insurance carrier although she didn’t believe her insurance carrier
provided for “out of network” providers and therefore her sessions with
him would not be covered. She indicated that she would pay by check or
credit card for each session. When she was asked whether she had any
questions, the patient, who worked in a hospital setting, indicated that
she was well aware of HIPAA and issues related to confidentiality, billing,
and so on and signed an informed consent form.
After the paperwork and business of psychology was put behind him,
Dr. A. began the task of developing a therapeutic alliance as he carefully
and empathically listened to what brought the patient to his office. At
the end of the first session, Dr. A. summarized what he had heard and
developed a treatment plan with the patient for future sessions. They
both agreed that his understanding of her situation and his approach for
working with her seemed appropriate.
Dr. A. and Ms. Z. for all intents and purposes were engaged in an
appropriate therapeutic relationship. The good news is that they seemed
well suited for each other in that he felt very comfortable dealing with her
issues and she was active in responding to new insights and perspective on
STANDARD OF CARE
CLINICAL JUDGMENT
RISK MANAGEMENT
The Trust, a major malpractice carrier for psychologists, has defined risk
management as “prospective assessment of retrospective evaluation” (Knapp,
Younggren, VandeCreek, Harris, & Martin, 2013, p. 34). Although to some
this might be seen as just another cliché, the guidance it gives is actually the
essence of good risk management. Effective risk management requires prac-
ticing psychologists to be aware of how their conduct today might be evalu-
ated at a later date by another person or entity, like a licensing board, that has
full knowledge of the outcome of that past conduct. What is key here is that
when the decision was made by the psychologist to do what he or she did, that
psychologist did not know what the outcome of that service was going to be.
Yet, when the appropriateness of that professional service or conduct is later
evaluated retrospectively, the outcome will be a part of the evaluative pro-
cess. Consequently, it is very easy for the evaluator to engage in hindsight bias
when determining not only what could have been done to avoid the outcome
in question but, and more dangerously so, to determine what should have
been done. This retrospective analysis, often a key component of licensing
investigations and civil actions against psychologists, clearly runs risk of fall-
ing prey to this hindsight bias, making the whole process potentially unfair.
Because of their fears about being sued or having a licensing board com-
plaint filed against them, practicing psychologists have dealt with the reali-
ties of professional risk in both maladaptive and adaptive ways. Maladaptive
solutions to reduce risk can be found among those who simply refuse to treat
populations of people who present a risk to them. Psychologists who do this
can frequently be heard saying, “I just don’t treat people who . . .” Other
maladaptive solutions include those who have adopted a series of “rules”
that they believe will protect them and, consequently, reduce their risk.
Psychologists who do this can frequently be heard saying, “You never . . . with
a patient.” Both of these risk management styles are, in reality, bad and reflect
a black-and-white approach to a world that is filled with gray. In addition,
each of these is a least-common-denominator solution to risk management
that is designed only to protect the professional, with little focus on patients
and their needs. Psychologists who utilize either of these approaches can
SUMMARY
REFERENCES
When you look for it, it is everywhere—it permeates almost every aspect
of professional practice. Whether one is setting up one’s office, consulting on
a referral, deciding what assessment instrument to use, meeting a client for
the first time, reviewing and assessing the literature, or advocating for a par-
ticular treatment approach for a particular case, one is engaged in a form of
it. The “it” can be termed clinical decision making, and it’s not too much of a
stretch to say that the fundamental goal of doctoral training in professional
psychology or training in any advanced mental health discipline is to produce
budding clinicians who have the knowledge, skills, and attitudes that enable
them to make and carry out good clinical decisions.
Despite the centrality of this concept, professional psychologists are
generally less likely than some other health professionals, such as nurses and
physicians, to deliberately frame their work and teach their craft in terms of
clinical decision making, although there are exceptions (e.g., O’Donohue &
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/14711-011
Clinical Decision Making in Mental Health Practice, J. J. Magnavita (Editor)
Copyright © 2016 by the American Psychological Association. All rights reserved.
273
Henderson, 1999). The failure to explicitly frame our training in this manner
may be a function of the old debate about clinical versus empirical judgment
or the fact that “clinical” is still used to denote one of the three broad prac-
tice areas (with “counseling” and “school” being the other two) or the fact
that conflict remains between the romantic and empirical visions of profes-
sional psychology. Whatever the reasons, it is my hope that this volume will
change the current state of affairs. Emphasizing clinical decision making is
apt because it encourages a deliberate, reflective, and intentional stance with
regard to how to go about one’s work as a professional psychologist.
Because clinical decision making is such a broad term, it has, not surpris-
ingly, many facets and can be approached from many different angles. For
example, Magnavita and Lilienfield (Chapter 2, this volume) offer a powerful
analysis that deconstructs the key elements of the clinical decision-making
process from the vantage point of cognitive psychology (Kahneman, 2011).
Part of that analysis includes a review of how people make decisions in gen-
eral. Perhaps most central to understanding general decision making is that
human’s process information via two related but separable streams of menta-
tion. The first stream is a fast, relatively automatic, perceptual, holistic, affec-
tive system of processing that sizes up a situation via “thin slicing” (Gladwell,
2005) and forms quick, intuitive judgments. The second system is a slower,
more explicitly self-conscious and deliberate form of thought, mediated largely
by processes of verbal justification. From an educator’s perspective, this is a
basic and central feature of the human mind of which students of profes-
sional psychology should be very aware. For example, a training exercise that
I find useful when the class is viewing video is to stop the tape as soon as the
patient (or client) appears on the screen and ask students for their report of
their immediate perceptions, feelings, and intuitions about the client. Often
trainees are initially reticent to say anything, generally because they don’t
want to appear as though they “judge a book by its cover.” But once they are
given permission, the associations flow, and we see that many impressions are
formed almost instantaneously. They first notice the obvious demographics
of the patient. Then they will notice how attractive they perceive the client
to be and the manner of dress, hygiene, and body position, all which serve as
indicators of socioeconomic status. Following that, a host of more imagina-
tive wonderings will begin. These impressions are examples of thin slicing,
an inevitable aspect of being human, and students need to be aware that they
will then begin to form narratives and expectations on the basis of this very
brief exposure.
Building on this basic formulation of the human mind, Magnavita
and Lilienfeld (Chapter 2, this volume) further articulate how individuals
develop heuristics, the general rules of thumb that are acquired over time that
help consolidate the massive amounts of incoming information into relatively
One of the most perplexing challenges for the field of professional psy-
chology has been its struggle to navigate the tensions between the cold logic
of science and the moral necessities of humanism. Indeed, in a seminal article,
Kimble (1984) empirically documented the split between science and human-
ism in the broader field. It is the obligation of professional psychologists to
understand the historical and epistemological issues that have contributed to
this split and to be informed by both scientific and humanistic lenses when
engaged in professional practice. First, by virtue of a core institutional identity,
professional psychology is grounded in science, which means that it embraces
the epistemic values and methods associated with science (Henriques &
Sternberg, 2004). As such, it is crucial that a scientific attitude is instilled in
budding professional psychologists. Some of the key ingredients of this atti-
tude are skepticism and critical thought, a worldview that frames cause and
effect with certain assumptions based on scientific plausibility, and reliance on
evidence acquired in a systematic way (see Lilienfeld & O’Donohue, 2012).
Although a scientific attitude is crucial, it is not all there is to being a
professional psychologist. Indeed, the primary identity of professional psychol-
ogy is as an applied health service profession, and this means that the primary
charge of professional psychology is prescriptive (Henriques & Sternberg,
2004). Ultimately, the function of professional psychologists is to change an
existing state. This can be conceived as having the goal to move individuals
or systems toward more valued states of being, which requires having a broadly
philosophical—some might say metaphysical (O’Donohue, 1989)—position
regarding the values that are guiding one’s actions. The ethical code offered
by the APA prescribes some of the key values that all psychologists need to
consider in their professional behavior but, although essential, leaves much
ambiguity in the details of how to be an ethical, values-driven practitioner.
Because an individual psychologist has the potential for great influence over
When asked how he defined science, Robyn Dawes, well known for his
work on fostering empirically based decision making, answered,
I would define it as testing hypotheses through the systematic collection
and analysis of data whether via what are called “randomized trials,”
where we randomly assign people to be given a vaccine or not or to a
placebo group, all the way to informed observation. These are really the
two essences of science. (Gambrill & Dawes, 2003; cited in Lilienfeld &
O’Donohue, 2012, p. 59)
Dawes captured the methodological view of science. This view is
embraced by many psychologists, both researchers and practitioners alike.
Indeed, some argue that grounding psychology in the scientific method is
the defining and unifying feature of the discipline (see, e.g., Stam, 2004).
However, from the vantage point of a broad scientific humanistic philosophy,
the purely methodological view of science is inadequate. In isolation, the
scientific method (i.e., generating hypotheses and conducting studies) yields
data and information. However, the professional psychologist needs to oper-
ate first from knowledge and wisdom. The incompleteness of method is obvi-
ous on reflection. Consider the question of why we engage in the scientific
method in the first place. It generally is not solely for the specific data it yields
about the specific phenomena under investigation. Indeed, if the data gath-
ered were not generalizable at all, they would be largely irrelevant because
scientific findings from specific studies—in the absence of a nomological net-
work of scientific understanding—are essentially meaningless. The data and
information from scientific studies become meaningful only when they are
linked with data from other investigations and then placed within a network
of understanding. Thus, science must include attention to the conceptually
grounded meaning-making schema that organizes scientific knowledge.
In this approach, concepts and theories are the bridges that link
data and information gleaned from the scientific method to wise practice.
Consequently, a major goal I have as a trainer of budding clinicians is to pro-
vide them with a broad framework that effectively maps the discipline, clears
up the current psychotherapy tower of Babel, and allows the key insights from
myriad perspectives and traditions to be coherently integrated into a whole.
Directly related to clinical decision making in a wide variety of contexts is
the approach to conceptualizing people based on analyzing five systems of
character adaptation and the biological, learning and developmental, and
sociocultural contexts in which the individual is immersed (Henriques, 2011;
see Figure 11.1). The systems of character adaptation refer to the hierarchical
arrangement of mental systems that enable an individual to respond to the
current situation. The character adaptation system theory (CAST) approach
refers to the hierarchical arrangement of mental systems that enable an indi-
vidual to respond to the current situation. From the most basic to the most
advanced, the five systems are as follows: (a) the habit system, which refers to
the basic procedural processes shaped by learning and stimulus control; (b) the
experiential system, which refers to the core of experiential consciousness that
is organized by the flow of perception, motivation, and emotional reactions;
(c) the relationship system, which is an outgrowth of the experiential system
Sociocultural Context
Macro,
Meso,
Micro
Language based beliefs and values; Public to Private Filtering;
Justification System Attributions; Identity and Self Concept; Existential Meaning Making
Biological Context
Figure 11.1. The five systems of character adaptation. From A New Unified Theory of Psychology (p. 230), by G. Henriques,
2011, New York, NY: Springer Science+Business Media. Copyright 2011 by Springer Science+Business Media. Reprinted
with permission.
that tracks self–other exchanges in an intuitive way on the dimension of rela-
tional value and social influence; (d) the defensive system, which refers to the
ways the individual manages psychic equilibrium in the form of experiential
avoidance, dissonance reduction, and defense mechanisms; and (e) the justifi-
cation system, which refers to the verbally mediated explicit beliefs, values, and
attributions people use to make sense of themselves and others.
As articulated by Henriques and Stout (2012), the five systems of char-
acter adaptation provide a framework for assimilating and integrating the key
insights from major traditions in psychotherapy, placed in a biopsychosocial
context. For example, behaviorists have historically tended to think and focus
on habits, whereas humanistic and experiential practitioners have focused on
core emotions and experiences; psychodynamic practitioners have focused on
underlying relationship patterns and psychological defenses, and cognitive
and narrative therapists have emphasized semantic meaning making in vari-
ous ways. The CAST approach provides a way to understand how these are
all component systems of adaptation that can be effectively woven together
in to a more coherent whole.
How does this system influence clinical decision making? As O’Donohue
and Henderson (1999; cited in Lilienfeld & O’Donohue, 2012) pointed out,
“choosing appropriate treatment methods involves knowing and instanti-
ating causal relations” (p. 51). To do this, a clinician needs to be able to
understand the key variables and their hypothesized causal relations, and the
CAST approach guides students on how to accomplish this. To see this, let’s
continue with the example that was introduced earlier, that of a college stu-
dent who receives a referral for assessing the presence of ADHD and possible
accommodations. Let’s add the following background to the formulation and
then apply the CAST approach to fostering a conceptualization:
Tina is a 19-year-old college freshman. She grew up in a small rural
town in southwestern Virginia. She is a first-generation college student
and entered college with hopes of being a physician. She did extremely
well in high school and has always been very driven and conscientious.
However, her first semester at college did not go very well. She expe-
rienced difficulty making friends, and she was uncomfortable with the
drinking and party atmosphere. She focused a lot on her studies and
studied several hours a day, but she struggled to get the As she expected
(her first semester grade point average was 3.2). Now she is reporting
problems taking tests and staying focused and is worried that she has
ADHD. She is starting to have trouble sleeping; she can’t fall asleep
because she is constantly worrying about what she needs to do the next
day. She is also having nightmares about failing out of school. She
also is reporting frequent stomachaches, and she is now considering
whether she should transfer to a different college because it is closer
to home.
Professional psychology has long been torn between two visions, the
practice of psychology as an art versus an empirically based science. The artis-
tic vision promotes the image of the master clinician as a wise and insightful
healer guided by a deep intuitive knowledge. A prototype of such a clinician
was offered by Caldwell (2004; cited in Garb, 2005), who, on receiving an
award for his work in personality assessment, gave the following example of
successfully interpreting a Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory:
We got a severe 4-6-8 profile on a young woman. I looked at the tortured
implications of the pattern and somehow said, “She will have something
like cigarette burn scars on her hands, where her father prepared her to
steel herself to the suffering of life.” The round burn marks were on her
hands and extended a little way up her arms. (Caldwell, 2004, p. 9)
In contrast to the vision of the master practitioner as a wise artisan, the
empiricist vision cautions psychologists against such ideals (Garb, 2005) and
emphasizes judgments and decision making based not on intuition and the
like but on existing empirical evidence. The practitioner’s skill is in know-
ing how to acquire, interpret, and apply good empirical data to the question
at hand. Empirically trained practitioners tend to dismiss with skepticism
anecdotes like the one in the previous paragraph and point out the incred-
ible biases of the human mind in seeing spurious patterns in nature. As a
consequence, proponents of the empirical tradition argue that there is a great
need to ground assessments and treatments in those validated by the scien-
tific method. As alluded to earlier, the empirical tradition is now explicitly
represented in “clinical science” training programs (Baker et al., 2009) that
The argument laid out so far is that if students are going to be informed
consumers of scientific research applied to professional practice, they must
operate from a broad scientific humanistic philosophy of the field. Such a
view will enable them to consolidate findings into meaningful information
that guides their decision making. Without such a framework, the field and
its practitioners are destined to endless debates because of foundational dis-
putes about assumptions that are not resolvable at the level of scientific data
gathering. Earlier I described how a broad scientific humanistic philosophy
is necessary to reflect on and make decisions about diagnoses and develop
holistic conceptualizations that elucidate key variables and their causal inter-
relations in a way that leads to informed clinical decision making. In this
section of this chapter, the focus turns to therapy and how the unified system
offers a new way to approach the field of psychotherapy, one that is quite dif-
ferent from other approaches.
The field of psychology in general and the practice of psychotherapy in
particular have been “pre-paradigmatic,” meaning that there was no avail-
able broad framework from which professional psychologists could operate.
This is apparent when one considers that the emergence of the major schools
of thought were generally through a master practitioner gaining insights
based on useful techniques in the therapy room. Although they were all
students of human nature, the founders of the great therapy endeavors like
Freud, Rogers, and Beck largely started with observations about the thera-
peutic process and generalized from there about insights for the field of psy-
chology. These gurus then generated a following of individuals who tried to
apply their insights and argue for the best approach to psychotherapy on the
basis of this process.
The unified approach advocated for here works in the opposite direc-
tion. It specifically concerns itself with the construction of an integrative
metatheoretical framework that then can be used to assimilate and inte-
grate key insights and findings from both the science of human psychology
(e.g., personality, cognitive, affective, developmental, neuroscience, social,
A basic principle stemming from the argument thus far is that, as phi-
losophers often point out, “facts are theory laden.” Professional psychologists
must be aware that humans do not perceive the world directly as it truly is
(whatever that might mean), but we have perceptual and conceptual catego-
ries that enable us to actively make meaning out of the patterns in the world.
This is the first meaning of the word theory here. Because the background
conceptual structure “frames” what the practitioner sees in making decisions,
it is crucial that the practitioner be as fully aware of those structures as pos-
sible. This starts at the level of broad philosophy and worldview and includes
the views the professional psychologist has for how the world works, his or
her religious and political perspectives, beliefs about the nature of human
nature, and beliefs about humanity’s place in the universe at large. If these
sound deeply philosophical, they are. This is central because we relate to our
clients at the level of meaning and inevitably hear their stories through a
particular lens defined by our worldview.
Applied to Tina, consider how a Christian psychological practitioner
might hear and respond to her story differently than a secular skeptical prac-
titioner. To do so, let’s make the reasonable assumption, on the basis of Tina’s
story and the demographics of southern rural Virginia, that she was raised in
a socially conservative, Christian home. If so, it follows that some of her cur-
rent anxiety and confusion likely would stem from the potentially conflicting
messages she has received in the context of her transition from a socially and
religiously conservative environment where she felt comfortable to one that
is more secular and has looser mores regarding drinking and sexual activity. If
so, then it is highly likely that a socially conservative Christian psychological
practitioner will hear Tina’s story differently than a purely secular practitio-
ner. This is the case even when both practitioners are engaged in “secular”
psychotherapy and are appropriately ethical and sensitive about imposing
Relationship
SUMMARY
The following list offers some of the key principles that guide effec-
tive psychotherapy. This attempts to breakdown the elements of TEST RePP
in a way that is congruent with the empirical literatures in psychology and
psychotherapy.
1. Set an appropriate, ethical frame. Psychotherapy is a relationship
that is grounded in professional obligations and constraints,
and it is crucial that all stakeholders involved understand the
purpose and function of the relationship; issues of confidenti-
ality; financial reimbursement; general focus of the work; and,
where appropriate, expected time frame.
2. Begin to foster a strong therapeutic relationship. It is crucial that
the psychologist exhibit a level of competence and respect
toward the client such that the client feels valued and heard
and believes the therapist can help where appropriate.
3. Identify cultural context variables. Consideration of the social
construction of identities is crucial for both the therapist and
the client. When the client and psychologist do not share the
same broader cultural background, particular attention should
be paid to how the influence of cultural context might lead to
differences in communication patterns, expectations of roles,
core values, and so forth.
4. Identify client values and hopes. The central goal of psychother-
apy is to enhance adaptive ways of living, a central element of
which is the client’s value states of being.
5. Identify risk of harm. A fundamental tenet of practice is to min-
imize the risk of harm. A practitioner must be reflective about
the possible ways an intervention might have unintended side
effects. If there is anticipated possible harm, all parties should
be informed, and that must be carefully weighed against prob-
able benefits.
6. Begin to formulate an ongoing case conceptualization. A compre-
hensive assessment includes a general categorization of the
major symptoms, character, key developmental factors, rel-
evant biological and sociocultural variables, current relational
context, and major stressors and affordances in the environ-
ment. In addition, a systematic approach to assessing rela-
tional style, identity, and presenting problem (i.e., diagnosis)
should be included.
7. Begin to identify realistic, adaptive treatment outcomes. The ther-
apist should work with the client to identify therapy goals in
REFERENCES
309
bias, continued errors commonly influencing, 39–50.
selection, 5, 48–49 See also Bias
survivorship, 49 and evolutionary basis of decision
in System 1 thinking, 34 making, 8–9
Bickman, L., 153, 225–229, 232 and expertise. See Clinical expertise
Big Data (Viktor Mayer-Schonberger & fast vs. slow thinking in, 33–36
Kenneth Cukier), 16 heuristics for, 3–4
“Big data,” 16, 32 knowledge of, 12
biofeedback, 152 pillars of effective, 9–12
biology, 8–9, 11 pragmatics of, 27–28
blended treatment, 157 rationality and emotionality in,
Blink (Malcolm Gladwell), 30, 35 29–31
Bloom, Benjamin, 261 risk management in. See Risk
Board of Psychology v. Tom Spencer management
Allison, 267 schemata and prototypes in, 14
Bodenhausen, G. V., 78 shared, 15
borderline personality disorder, 5, 40, sunk costs in, 4–5
44, 47, 48, 189, 196, 197, 210 uncertainty of analytics in, 28–29
Bordin, E. S., 291 clinical expertise, 23–27
Borkovec, T. D., 196 definitions of, 25–26
Breda, C., 228–229 development of, 4, 10–11
Buchanan, T., 152 in three-circle model of evidence-
based practice, 106–107
care, standard of, 255–257 training and experience required for,
case conceptualization, 160–162, 24–25, 34
205–216, 303 clinical judgment, 257–259
case management models, 187–188 clinical practice guidelines (CPGs),
Caspar, F., 147–148, 162, 167 105–121, 125–142
Castonguay, L. G., 195, 196 advantages of decision making with,
Caudill, C. O., 256 118–119
CBTs. See Cognitive and behavioral and American Psychological Asso-
therapies ciation, 11, 105, 108–110,
CFS (contextualized feedback 116, 125–126
system), 228 and bias, 109, 116–118
chaos theory, 176 criteria for, 108–109
character adaptation system theory and hierarchy of evidence, 136–138
(CAST), 281–285, 289, 291–292 history of, 109–111
children, treatment of, 265 implementation of, 119–120
chronic fatigue syndrome, 177 Institute of Medicine standards for,
Claxton, K., 15 105, 111–115, 126–134
clinical decision making, 3–16, 23–55. limitations of, 108
See also High-stakes clinical as link between research and
decision making; specific headings practice, 106–107
and actuarial prediction, 36–38 overview, 106
analytics for, 15 selection of appropriate, 134–136
approaches for enhancing, 50–54 systematic reviews of, 108–109, 113,
basics of, 38–39 115–116
and big data, 16 and treatment efficacy, 138–142
and decision theory, 5–6 Clinical Practice Guidelines We Can Trust
elements of, 31–33 (Institute of Medicine), 111
310 INDEX
clinical prediction contextualized feedback system
actuarial prediction vs., 36–38 (CFS), 228
effectiveness of, 35 Conway, M. A., 78
clinical support tools (CSTs), 156 cost–benefit analysis, 7
clinical training, 158–164. See also countertransference–transference
Teaching clinical decision making reactions, 188–189
Clinical Versus Statistical Prediction (Paul CPGs. See Clinical practice guidelines
Meehl), 35 Creighton, L. A., 69
Clinician’s Guide to Evidence-Based Critchfield, K. L., 196
Practices (J. C. Norcross, T. P. “Criteria for Evaluating Treatment
Hogan, & G. P. Koocher), 25 Guidelines” (APA
cognitive and behavioral therapies document), 110
(CBTs) critical thinking, 249–253
for anxiety disorders, 194 Croskerry, P., 68, 72, 90, 91
patterns in, 216 Cross, G., 10, 52
processes in, 299, 300 Crossing the Quality Chasm (Institute of
psychoanalytic treatment vs., 180 Medicine report), 52
research support for, 138, 141 Csikszentmihalyi, Mihaly, 30
semantic meaning in, 283 CSTs (clinical support tools), 156
theory in, 64 Cuijpers, P., 139
cognitive bias. See Bias Cukier, Kenneth, 16
cognitive capabilities, 254
cognitive continuum theory, 77 Dahlin, M., 151
cognitive dissonance theory, 227 data
cognitive restructuring, 194 “big,” 16, 32
cognitive theory, 82–83 in clinical decision making, 226–229
Cohen, J. T., 15 defined, 32
coherence, 160 and knowledge databases, 150
COIs (conflicts of interest), 112, patient, 151–155
128–129 Dawes, Robyn, 278
collaborative problem solving, 158, 164 de Andrade, A. R., 228–229
collaborative shared decision-making decision analysis, 26
model, 178–180, 183–187 decision-making research, 253–255. See
combined–integrated approach to also Clinical decision making
training, 274–275 decision theory
Comer, J. S., 149, 155, 165 history of, 13–14
common factors in psychotherapy, 291 influence of, 9
comorbidities, patient, 177–178, 196 overview, 5–6
complexity theory, 176 study of, 12
complex trauma, 177 default-interventionism model, 77
computer-assisted testing, 151–152 deliberative processing, 69, 73–78
computer-based training, 159–162 depression, 44, 46, 65, 128, 140, 288
confirmation bias, 5, 40–41, 43, DeRubeis, R. J., 135, 152
50, 128 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
conflicts of interest (COIs), 112, Mental Disorders (DSM), 71, 287
128–129 diagnosticity, 51
Conklin, C., 189 diagnostic labels, 5
consultation, 154–155, 264, 284 dialectical behavior therapy, 159, 196,
contact and communication technolo- 197, 216
gies, 156–157 divorce, 35, 52, 260, 265
INDEX 311
dodo bird effect, 291 case examples, 205–216
dopamine, 30 in moment-by-moment therapy
Doverspike, W., 256 process, 201–205
DSM (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual norms in, 201
of Mental Disorders), 71, 287 principles of, 196–198
dual process model, 79–94 and structural analysis of social
applications of, 79 behavior, 198–201
automatic processing and decision empirically supported treatments
making in, 69–74, 76–78 (ESTs). See also Evidence-based
clinical recommendations, 89–94 practice
clinical vignette, 79–89 and clinical practice guidelines, 106
deliberative processing and decision treatment efficacy of, 138
making in, 73–78 empiricism (philosophy), 286
development of, 68 endowment effect, 42
overview, 68–78 Ensworth, Heather, 266
dual process model of explicit and Ensworth v. Mullvain, 266–267
implicit theory, 79–94. See also Epstein, S., 78
Theory e-SOFT (System for Observing Family
applications of, 79 Therapy Alliances), 158–159
clinical vignette, 79–89 ESTs. See Empirically supported
recommendations with, 89–94 treatments
Dumont, F., 92 e-texts, 149–150
Duncan, B. L., 228 ethical frameworks, 12
ethical violations, 258
eating disorders, 177 EU (expected utility) theory, 180
EBM (evidence-based medicine), 107 EUV (expected utility value), 74–75
EBP. See Evidence-based medicine Evans, J. St. B. T., 69, 76, 77
ecological momentary assessment Evans, W. J., 162–163
(EMA), 154 evidence
efficacy, treatment, 138–142 empirical, 9–10
egocentrism, 43 hierarchy of, 136–138
Ellis, E., 254 evidence-based medicine (EBM), 107
EMA (ecological momentary evidence-based practice (EBP). See also
assessment), 154 Empirical evidence
EMDR (eye movement desensitization defined, 13
and reprocessing), 194 history of, 285–290
eMedicine Clinical Knowledge for posttraumatic stress disorder, 10
Database, 150 and teaching clinical decision
emotional reasoning, 252 making, 285–290
emotions evolutionary psychology, 8–9, 280
awareness of, 254 expected utility (EU) theory, 180
in clinical decision making, 29–31 expected utility value (EUV), 74–75
empathy, intellectual, 250 experiential practitioners, 64, 283
empirical evidence, 9–10 experiential processing. See Fast
empirically grounded relational thinking mode (System 1)
principles, 193–217 expertise. See Clinical expertise
across treatment approaches, explicit theory, 64–65
195–196 external review, 114
for case conceptualization and external validity, 137
interventions, 205–216 eye contact, 4
312 INDEX
eye movement desensitization and game theory, 13
reprocessing (EMDR), 194 Gawronski, B., 69
Eysenck, H. J., 295 GDP (guideline development panels),
111–113, 126–131
fair-mindedness, 251 gender bias, xiii
“Faith in reason” (trait), 251 Gervind, E., 151
family systems therapists, 64 Gladwell, Malcolm, 30, 35, 50
fast thinking mode (System 1), 33–36 Goethe, Johann Wolfgang von, xi
biases with, 53 Goodman and Gilman’s Pharmacological
overview, 14 Basis of Therapeutics (L. Brunton,
feedback systems, 153. See also B. Chabner, & B. Knollman),
Measurement feedback systems 149–150
FFT (functional family therapy), Gosch, E., 28
232–236 Gottman, J. M., 40
FFT-CFS (Functional Family Therapy Gottman, John, 52
Clinical Feedback System), GRADE (Grading of Recommendations
232–236 Assessment, Development, and
FFT-CMI (Functional Family Therapy Evaluation), 116
Clinical Measurement Inven- Graves’ disease, 44
tory), 232 Grawe, K., 153
fibromyalgia, 177 The Great Psychotherapy Debate (Bruce
fight–flight response, 34 Wampold), 288
financial conflicts of interest, 128–129 Greenberg, R. P., 154
Finding What Works in Health Care: Gregory, R., 154
Standards of Systematic Reviews Groopman, J., 27, 28, 38, 53
(Institute of Medicine), 111 guideline development panels (GDP),
Fischer, U., 72 111–113, 126–131
Fiske, S. T., 27
Fitzgerald, F. Scott, 44 Hammond, K. R., 77
fixed frames, 43–44 Harmon, C., 228
flow, 30 Harwood, T. M., 155, 164
Foundation for Critical Thinking, Hassin, R. R., 30
249–250 Hautle, I., 162
Four Horseman of the Apocalypse Hawkins, E. J., 228
(algorithm), 52 Health Insurance Portability and
Frank, Jerome, 291 Accountability Act (HIPAA),
Frederickson, J., 163 164, 256
Friedberg, R. D., 224 Heim, A. K., 189
functional family therapy (FFT), 232–236 Henderson, D., 283, 284
Functional Family Therapy Clinical Henriques, G. R., 283
Feedback System (FFT-CFS), Henry, W. P., 196
232–236 heuristic(s), 3–6
Functional Family Therapy Clinical availability, 40, 47, 135, 224
Measurement Inventory defined, 26–27
(FFT-CMI), 232 errors with, 39
Furr, J. M., 28 function of, 71–72
representative, 5, 48
Galinsky, A. D., 258 and teaching clinical decision
Gallinger, L., 154 making, 274–275
gambler’s fallacy, 41–42 Higgs, J., 254
INDEX 313
high-stakes clinical decision making, intellectual integrity, 250–251
175–190 intellectual perseverance, 251
aids for, 183 internal validity, 137
case management models for, Internet, 149–151
187–188 Internet-based testing, 151–152
collaborative shared decision-making Internet-based training system
model for, 178–180, 183–187 (ITS), 158
and expected utility theory, 180 interpersonal psychotherapy, 65, 79,
managing countertransference– 138, 194, 216
transference reactions in, interpersonal reconstructive therapy
188–189 (IRT), 210–214
overview, 176–177 interventions, 205–216
with patient comorbidities, 177–178 intuition, 30–31, 34–35, 51, 80. See
pattern recognition in, 188 also Clinical prediction; Fast
quadratic decision-making model for, thinking mode
181–183 intuitive processing. See Fast thinking
risk management in, 187 mode (System 1)
and safeguards against cognitive IOM. See Institute of Medicine
biases, 189 IRT (interpersonal reconstructive
simplified decision-making model therapy), 210–214
for, 180–181 ITS (Internet-based training
hindsight bias, 45, 259 system), 158
HIPAA (Health Insurance Portability
and Accountability Act), joint collaborative care meetings, 186
164, 256 Jones-Smith, E., 64
Hogan, T. P., 25
Hollon, S. D., 152 Kagan, Jerome, 11
Holmes-Rovner, M., 180 Kahneman, Daniel, 6
hormone replacement therapy on clinical expertise, 26, 179
(HRT), 137 on clinical prediction, 37–38
How Doctors Think (J. Groopman), 27 on clinicians and algorithmic
humanistic psychologists, 64, 283, thinking, 52
294, 299 development of decision theory by,
humility, intellectual, 250 13–14
hyperthyroidism, 44 on evidence, 50, 53
and heuristics, 26
illusory correlations, 46 and priming, 11
ILT (instructor-led training System 1 and System 2 thinking
workshops), 159 developed by, 33–36
implicit theory, 65–68 Karlin, B. E., 10, 52
information, 32 Keillor, Garrison, 47
insight, 299 Kelley, S. D., 153, 225, 228–229
Institute of Medicine (IOM), 12, 52, Kendall, P. C., 28
105–107, 111–115, 126–134 Kiesler, D. J., 80
instructor-led training workshops Kimble, G. A., 276
(ILT), 159 Klein, G., 26, 66, 70, 179
integrative psychotherapy, 64 Knight, Frank, 13
intellectual courage, 250 knowledge, 32, 294
intellectual empathy, 250 knowledge databases, 150
intellectual humility, 250 Koocher, G. P., 25
314 INDEX
Lake Woebegone effect, 47 Minnesota Multiphasic Personality
Lambert, M. J., 33, 47, 228 Inventory—2 (MMPI–2), 46,
Lammers, J., 258 280, 285
latent semantic analysis (LSA), 161 moment-by-moment therapy process,
Lehrer, J., 29 201–205
licensing boards, 245–246, 256, 258 Morgenstern, Oskar, 13
Lilienfield, S. O., 274–275 Mosier, K. L., 72
Linardatos, E., 128 Mullvain, Cynthia, 266
Linehan, M., 196 multiple personality disorder, 40
Lohr, S., 32 multiple relationship violations,
LSA (latent semantic analysis), 161 267–268
Luhrmann, T. M., 43
Najavits, L. M., 66, 93
Maggio, L. A., 149, 150 Naked Statistics: Stripping the Dread From
Magnavita, J. J., 41, 188, 225, 274–275 the Data (C. Wheelan), 47
malpractice, 258–259 narcissism, 5, 40, 43, 196
Manring, J., 154 narrative fallacy, 46
Månsson, K. N. T., 151 narrative therapists, 283
manuals. See Treatment manuals National Guidelines Clearinghouse, 108
Maslow, A., 31 National Institute for Health and
Matthews, A. M., 128 Clinical Excellence (NICE), 127,
Mayer-Schonberger, Viktor, 16 129–130
McFall, R. M., 279 National Institute of Mental Health
measurement feedback systems (MFS), (NIMH), 12
153, 223–241 naturalistic decision making (NDM), 70
benefits of, 225 negative affect, 304
in clinical decision making process, Neumann, P. J., 15
236–239 neurofeedback, 153
in functional family therapy, Neuropsychotherapy (K. Grawe), 153
232–236 neuroscience, 11, 30–31
future directions of, 239–240 Newnham, E. A., 239
OQ-Analyst, 229–230 The New Unconscious (R. R. Hassin,
overview, 226–229 J. S. Uleman, J. A. Bargh), 30
Partners for Change Outcome NICE (National Institute for Health
Management System, 231 and Clinical Excellence), 127,
Systemic Therapy Inventory of 129–130
Change, 230–231 NIMH (National Institute of Mental
medical model of treatment, 287, 296 Health), 12
Meehl, Paul, 35, 37, 132 nonmaleficence, 258
memory, 47–48 Norcross, J. C., 25
mentalization, 31 Norman, G., 90, 91
metacognition, 254 Norsworthy, L. A., 228
metacommunication, 80–82, 84
methodological view of science, O’Donohue, W. T., 24, 283, 284
278–281 Of Two Minds: An Anthropologist Looks
MFS. See Measurement feedback systems at American Psychiatry (T. M.
Miller, K. L., 162–163 Luhrmann), 43
Miller, S. M., 162–163 online supervision, 162–164
Miller, Scott, 33 online testing, 151–152
mindfulness, 92 online textbooks, 149–150
INDEX 315
optimal decision making, 9, 13, 31–33, Psychology’s Ghosts: The Crisis in the
54, 73, 225 Profession and the Way Back
OQ-Analyst (computer feedback (Jerome Kagan), 11
program), 229–230 psychopathy, 5
outcome research, 109 psychosocial learning, 297
overconfidence bias, 6, 46–47 psychotherapy
common factors in, 291
Page, A. C., 239 definitions of, xi–xii
paranoid personality disorder, 29, 197 integrative, 64
Parkinson’s disease, 44 interpersonal, 65, 79, 138, 194, 216
Partners for Change Outcome Manage- supportive, 139
ment System (PCOMS), 231 and theory, 63–64
passive-aggressive personalities, 197 unified theories of, 280–281,
patients 290–292
data of, 151–155 Psychotherapy Training e-Resources,
values and preferences of, 159–160
106–107, 181 PTSD (posttraumatic stress disorder), 10
pattern recognition publication bias, 49, 116, 132
in high-risk cases, 188
overview, 38–39 quadratic decision-making model,
Patterson, R. E., 66 181–183
PCOMS (Partners for Change Outcome
Management System), 231 randomized controlled trials (RCTs),
peer consulting, 154–155 137, 194–195
personality disorders (PDs), 177, 196– rationalism (philosophy), 286
198. See also specific rationality, 29–31
disorders, e.g.: Borderline person- Rationality and the Reflective Mind (Keith
ality disorder Stanovich), 33
personality systematics, 188 rational processing. See Slow thinking
PICOTS (populations, interventions, mode (System 2)
comparisons, outcomes, time, RCTs (randomized controlled trials),
and settings), 133–134, 137, 194–195
137–138, 141 recall bias, 47
Pierce, B. J., 66 recency effect, 7, 47–48
Pincus, A. L., 80 recognition-primed decision making
posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), 10 (RPD), 70–72
practice parameters. See Clinical record keeping, 264
practice guidelines Reese, R. J., 228
prescriptive indices, 134 referrals, 44
priming, 11 reflective processing. See Slow thinking
probability, 42, 50–51, 155–156 mode (System 2)
prognostic indices, 134 reflexive processing. See Fast thinking
prototypes, 14, 38, 39. See also Bias; mode (System 1)
Heuristics reframing, 44
psychiatry, 43–44, 49 Reid, W. H., 256, 257
psychoanalytic treatment, 64, 180 reimbursement policies, 108
Psychodynamic Diagnostic Manual, 287 relational processes in therapy. See
psychodynamic psychologists, 64, 194, Empirically grounded relational
283, 287, 300 principles
psychoeducative information, 151 relational psychoanalysis, 79n3
316 INDEX
representative heuristics, 5, 48 Shoben, E. J., Jr., 63, 65, 67
research. See also Evidence-based Shoham, V., 279
practice Siegle, G. J., 152
components of best available, simplified decision-making model,
106–107 180–181
decision-making, 253–255 Singer, J. A., 78
outcome, 109 Skagius Ruiz, E., 151
Riemer, M., 226, 228–229 Slade, K., 228
risk-averse psychologists, 260–261 sleep disorders, 177
risk-managed psychologists, 261–264 Slone, N. C., 228
risk management, 245–269 Slovic, P., 73, 78
case examples, 246–248, 265–268 slow thinking mode (System 2), 33–36
and clinical judgment, 257–259 biases with, 53
and critical thinking, 249–253 overview, 14
and decision-making research, Smith, M., 254
253–255 social capabilities, 254
in high-stakes clinical decision Society for Psychotherapy Research, 157
making, 187 Socrates, 249
overview, 14, 259–260 Sood, E., 28
by risk-averse psychologists, 260–261 Spada, H., 164
by risk-managed psychologists, Sparks, J. A., 228
261–264 splitting, 184
and standard of care, 255–257 standard of care, 255–257
Roberto, M. A., 26 Stanovich, Keith
Rosen, C. S., 10 on Bayesian instincts, 51
Rosenthal, R., 35, 128 classification of System 1 and 2
Rothert, M. L., 180 thinking by, 33, 36
Rousmaniere, T., 163 on clinical prediction, 35, 37
RPD (recognition-primed decision and decision making errors, 42,
making), 70–72 45, 46
Rummel, N., 164 default-interventionism model, 77
Russell, E. J., 152 and dual process models, 69
and heuristics, 26, 27
Sackett, D. L., 107 on optimal decision making, 31
Sapyta, J., 226 Stanton, M., 176
SASB (structural analysis of social Stapel, D. A., 258
behavior), 198–201, 216 stepped care, 156
schemas, 82, 216 STIC (Systemic Therapy Inventory of
schemata, 14 Change), 230–231
scientific humanistic philosophy, Stout, J., 283
276–278 Strangers to Ourselves: Discovering the
selection bias, 5, 48–49 Adaptive Unconscious (Timothy
selective treatment, 148 Wilson), 30
SEUT (structural analysis of social Stricker, G., 240
behavior), 73–75 structural analysis of social behavior
severe mental illness, 177 (SASB), 198–201, 216
Sexton, T. L., 224 subjective expected utility theory
Shanteau, J., 25 (SEUT), 73–75
Shapiro, J. P., 224 Sullenberger, Chessy, 8
shared clinical decision making, 15 Sumner, W. G., 249
INDEX 317
sunk-cost effect, 4, 48 TEST RePP (Theoretically and Empiri-
sunk-cost fallacy, 5 cally Supported Treatment and
supervision, 154–155, 162–164 Relationship Processes and Prin-
supportive psychotherapy, 139 ciples), 292–301
survivorship bias, 49 “The autonomous set of systems”
System 1. See Fast thinking mode (TASS), 90
System 2. See Slow thinking mode Theoretically and Empirically Supported
systematic reviews (SRs), 108–109, 113, Treatment and Relationship
115–116 Processes and Principles (TEST
System for Observing Family Therapy RePP), 303–305
Alliances (e-SOFT), 158–159 theory, 61–68. See also specific theories
Systemic Therapy Inventory of Change and models, e.g.: Dual process
(STIC), 230–231 model of explicit and implicit
theory
Tavris, C., 252 of automatic processing, 69–74,
Taylor, S. E., 27 76–78
teaching clinical decision making, definitions of, 294
273–302 of deliberative processing, 73–78
and evidence-based practice, explicit, 64–65
285–290 implicit, 65–68
and heuristics, 274–275 role of, 11
integrative model for, 281–285 role of, in psychotherapy, 63–64
and methodological view of science, therapeutic alliance, 196–198, 291,
278–281 297–298, 303
and paradigms in psychotherapy, therapeutic ruptures, 305
290–292 Thinking, Fast and Slow (Daniel
scientific humanistic approach to, Kahneman), 14, 33
276–278 “Thin slicing” (information
TEST RePP model for, 292–301 processing), 35
technology in clinical decision making, Todd, A. R., 78
147–167 Toland, M. D., 228
acceptance of, 165 Tracey, T. J. G., 25, 50
advantages of, 149 training, clinical, 158–164. See also
ambulatory monitoring, 154 Teaching clinical decision making
and clinical training, 158–164 trauma symptoms, 194
contact and communication treatment efficacy, 138–142
technologies, 156–157 treatment guidelines. See Clinical
future developments, 166–167 practice guidelines
and health care, 15–16 treatment manuals, 28
information available on Internet, Trierweiler, S. J., 240
149–151 The Trust (malpractice carrier), 259
patient data, 151–155 Turner, E. H., 128
probability calculations in, 155–156 Tversky, Amos, 13, 26
psychoeducative and therapeutic Type 1. See Fast thinking mode
information, 151 Type 2. See Slow thinking mode
risks with, 165–166
skillful use of, 165 Uleman, J. S., 30
video recordings, 154–155, 158–159 uncertainty tolerance, 52–54, 187, 253
Tell, R. A., 128 unified theories of psychotherapy,
termination, therapy, 266, 305 280–281, 290–292
318 INDEX
U.S. Agency for Health Care Policy and Weigold, A., 152
Research, 109 Weigold, I. K., 152
U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs Welsh, R., 176
(VA), 109, 118 West, Richard, 33
Westen, D., 189
VA (U.S. Department of Veterans Wheelan, C., 47, 49
Affairs), 109, 118 Whipple, J. L., 228
videoconferencing, 157 Williams, M. H., 256
video recordings, 154–155, 158–159 Wills, C. E., 180
virtual reality, 164 Wilson, Timothy, 30
von Neumann, John, 13 wisdom, 33, 34, 78
INDEX 319
ABOUT THE EDITOR
321
research interest is decision making and the use of technology in mental
health practice. Dr. Magnavita served as a member and then vice chair of
the APA Steering Committee tasked with the development of Clinical
Practice Guidelines and is a lecturer in the Department of Psychiatry at
Yale University. His most recent volume, with Jack C. Anchin, is Unifying
Psychotherapy: Principles, Methods, and Evidence From Clinical Science.