Embedded Matrix 1
Embedded Matrix 1
0HWHU
*
7KHSOXVVLJQDGGVRIWKHYDOXHRIWKHQRWHLWIROORZVLH ; P ; ;
% *
7KHGRXEOHGRWDGGVDQDGGLWLRQDOWKHYDOXHRIWKHVLQJOHGRWLH ; ; ;;
MATRIX OF EMBEDDED RHYTHMS
Among the most frequent requirements of newer rhythmic practice is the task to perform some
number of attacks within the space of some different number of beats, the attacks and beats not
being related to each other in the usual binary fashion i.e.
which is spoken of as " three triplet-quarter-notes in the space of two regular quarter-notes"
(we will later omit the word "regular", but for now we must fix in our minds that these are
two very different species of quarter notes!).
But even for these familiars we do not always know the exact placement of the individual
attacks (other than for the coincident beat – which is usually the downbeat).
The above matrix provides patterns for the exact placement for 64 varieties of what I refer to
as embedded or combination rhythms. These are also more commonly called composite
rhythms, but I firmly believe that term is far better reserved for the more general problem of
the totality of all rhythmic attacks that occur within a given musical context.
On the left side, descending the page, are meters. Each meter applies for that entire row of the
matrix. The is the basis for all given meters. If one wants to employ the patterns for
meters, add an additional set of beams to the notation within the cells of the matrix. For
meters, for each cell, remove a set of beams, or if no beams, multiply the note values by two.
Across the top of the matrix are the number of divisions within a column. These divisions
represent the embedded meter or value, except (of course) in those cases where the number of
divisions in a column equals the number of beats of a meter. In these cases, the notes are in the
lightest possible shade of grey, so as to differentiate.
2
As an example, in order to find the exact placement for 3:2 (in this context the : is read as
"in the space of", so for this situation one would say "3 in the space of 2 quarter notes") find
If, on the other hand, one wanted the composite for 2:3 (2 in the space of 3 quarter notes)
one would look for the intersection of the row labeled , and the column labeled 2 (with 2:3
in the upper left corner of the specific cell), the result being:
There are many ways to practice these rhythms. The simplest is to mentally remove the ties of
the notated values, and sing or tap the result of combining the embedded (columnar) value
with the required meter. For example, if one were to be studying 4:3 (4 in the space of 3), one
would tap:
Another way of studying would be to count or tap out all the internal subdivisions,
emphasizing those points that coincide with the embedded values.
3
or more easily
i.e.
or more easily
or in rhythmic notation
Another way of studying these rhythms is to have one hand tap the embedded portion, and the
other hand tap the required meter. For example, if one were to be studying 5:3, the hand
distribution might be:
4
When practicing, note that it is also invaluable to reverse the hand distribution. Having the
embedded portion in the right hand with the required meter in the left is not the same problem
as the reverse, nor is it the same as reversing the metric ratio. As explanation, consider the 2 X
2 matrix below:
Clearly the patterns in the 5:3 column are identical save for the fact that the hand-tasks are
reversed, but note that this physiological reversal is not immediately automatic, or "simple";
i.e. for many, it feels a somewhat different problem. Similarly, the patterns in the 3:5 column
are also identical except for the handedness, but again the switch is not always autonomic (in
the motor-skills sense of the word).1 In regards the rows, upon consideration it is clear that
the patterns of row A are arithmetically and physiologically equivalent, but they are far from
psychologically equivalent. Furthermore, if we assume that the of the 5:3 column = the
of right 3:5 column, the patterns of row A should purportedly also be temporally identical;
yet clearly the mental task required for the production of the members of row A remains very
different, and all this is in addition (perhaps) to the feelings regarding different handedness
that we previously remarked upon. In addition, please note that everything just said regarding
row A applies separately to row B.2
One need not reserve a special time to practice these patterns. As long as they are memorized,
they can be practiced as you are walking, counting the embedded portion against your paces, or
if sitting on a bus or subway, you can tap them using one finger from each hand. Indeed, it
may prove advantageous to practice these patterns in a public space, for if you do so with
5
supreme concentration, combined with a Harpo-Marx-like Gookie, you may find yourself with
far fewer people surrounding you than otherwise might have been the case.
These rhythms may also be distributed between two persons, or even two groups of people –
i.e. in the case of X:Y one person would attempt to count and perform the X against the Y of
the other person. The participants should then reverse roles.
It is advantageous to not only practice these patterns one by one, but to also practice them in
chains, one immediately following the other without pause. For example, one might take a
single row (meter) and create a succession of patterns that follow each other (as might happen
in an actually composition), and one should do this (in principle) for each row. Similarly one
could concentrate on a single column, varying the meter for each new pattern. Ultimately one
should create arbitrary successions of patterns across rows and columns. For all of these
practice situations, it is probably wise to at first repeat each pattern an agreed-upon number of
times before switching (without pause) to another pattern, but the goal is to switch patterns
each "bar".
All of these rhythms are self-retrogradable. In other words, each rhythm reads the same
backwards as forwards. If you copy out these rhythms, or ever need to create a version of a
similar rhythm which is not on the chart, and the result is not exactly symmetrical and self-
reversible, you have made an error in your calculations or copying.
All of these types of rhythms follow a simple arithmetic rule, which may be used to create
exemplars not found on the chart:
X always represents the number to the left of the ratio (:) sign
Y always represents the number to the right of the ratio (:) sign
B = the beat value in question
RULE
1) first divide B into X parts – this provides the smallest subdivision one needs to count.
2) take the result of Step 1) and count-off Y subdivisions, repeating this process until all the
subdivisions of 1) are finished – this process will provide the exact placement of the attack
points for the embedded rhythm. Note that there will always be X groups of these Y
subdivisions.
3) After completing Step 2) be certain (by using multiples of Y, or if those multiples go past a
beat, by using ties) to rewrite the results of Step 2) in terms of B.
6
As a simple example of this procedure, consider 4:3
In this case
Step 1) above tells us to divide B (in this case a ) into X (in this case four) parts, resulting in
Step 2) above tells us to take the result of Step 1) (in this case a )
Now we know that so to the greatest extent possible we should write the
When you have worked through this process, and have rewritten the results of Step 2) so as to
conform to the underlying meter, the final result is:
Now there are those who propound a common denominator approach to solving these
problems. As an example, in the aforesaid problem of 4:3 one would multiply the two
numbers (i.e. 4 * 3) to arrive at a result of 12, and one would then be told to think, and/or
count, 12.
7
The problem with this approach is that it does not answer the two questions:
a) 12 what?
b) how many subdivisions per beat?
To continue the argument, consider 4:3 vs 3:4 . For both of these, the common
denominator answer would be 12 (i.e. 4 * 3 = 3 * 4), but 4:3 requires us to think and/or
Another way of saying this is that 4:3 requires us to divide each beat into 4 parts, whereas
In short, the common denominator solution is not a good one, as it does not provide enough
information (i.e. what type of note value should one be thinking).
Finally, there is the matter of performing these types of rhythms. It is true that, for all of these
types of patterns, one must begin the learning process by attempting to have the attacks as
arithmetically correct as possible. However, arithmetic exactitude is only a means to an end. It
is not, and cannot be, the end itself. Arithmetic exactitude will not provide the shaping
necessary to convey the sentiment of a waltz, let alone a waltz embedded in a context.
Arithmetic exactitude will not allow one to shape an embedded rhythm so that one can
maintain the same trajectory of phrase across different tempi which happen to be written out
using so-called irrational rhythms (i.e. think Babbitt). Arithmetic exactitude in-and-of-itself as
a goal will rarely convey any meaning, other than an attempt at exactness.
For the sake of argument, consider that cell within the matrix which exists at the intersection
Count (preferably out loud – but it will also work silently) these five to yourself, doing so at
a comfortable tempo. Note how there is a shape, a trajectory, to the way the notes group
8
together. For the purposes of this discussion let us inelegantly call this quality of grouping
“five-ness”, or if you prefer, “quint-essence”. “Five-ness” is part and parcel of the concept of
performing groups of five. “Five-ness” is what distinguishes a group of five from, shall we
say, a group of four plus one.
Now observe every cell in the column marked 5. Each cell in that column has five attacks, just
up and down all cells in the column, especially when you look at , and , but
nevertheless, there are five attacks per cell. Indeed, if one were to perform the proper metric
modulations, there is no reason why all these differently notated avatars of five attacks could
not be performed at exactly the same speed. That the five attacks in each cell have not been
vertically uniformly aligned is due not only to the restrictions of the typesetting program, but
also to the fact that visually we would reject the image, and/or fail to comprehend how many
beats it fills. Furthermore, this column shows only eight different ways in which to write
"FIVE". All I would need to do is to change the denominator (the number on the bottom) of
the time signatures to 2, 8 or 16, and there would be 32 different ways of attempting to portray
five nominally equal and equidistant values across some slice of time, and note that I have not
While this enormous dichotomy between the underlying unity of the patterns (they all possess
and partake of "five-ness"), and the epidemic of notational possibilities, is endemic to our
system of rhythmic notation, there is a distortive aspect whose virulence we cannot, must not,
minimize.
Remember the architectural adage: form follows function. Applied here, it implies that the form
of the notation must follow the need (the function) to convey a group of five. It is the five
that contains the "quality" we have remarked upon and which is the important factor, not the
notational garb it must assume so that we can accurately do our job. In addition, just as the
column of five has the quality of “five-ness”, so does every other column have its own quality
(or should) – and it is those qualities, no matter how horrifying the precise notation, that are of
paramount importance.
9
None of the above means that one has license to distort beyond recognition. What it does imply
is that one begins by doing as well as one can, and trains to be as accurate as is humanly
possible, and part of that training is knowing these rewritten patterns, and more importantly,
how to rewrite them – but thereafter, after you have tried to understand why the composer is
using such rhythms, and after you have given thought to what you think needs to be aurally
conveyed, so that the underlying purpose and structure of the idea is clarified, and especially
after the trajectory of the embedded pattern is “in your blood”, then shape the pattern – very
slightly – so as to better convey its intrinsic meaning and motivation.
PZ
7/2003
FOOTNOTES
1: At least three broad reasons come immediately to mind as to why handedness might interact with the
performance of these patterns.
a) One possibility could be a result of the habitual use of each hand. For example, string or brass players,
who are over trained to use each hand differently when playing, may have greater problems switching
hands than keyboard players, whose hands tend to perform the same kinds of tasks.
b) Another possibility is that there may be a psychological or physiological preference to assign the
tapping of the basic meter to either (a) a person's dominant hand, which may require less mental
supervision, or (b) the reverse preference of assigning the basic meter to the non-dominant hand,
precisely because one feels more secure with the underlying meter, and can therefore fairly safely engage
the non-dominant hand for the "simpler" task.
c) Yet another possibility could be that one has a preference to assign (to either the dominant or non-
dominant hand) whichever rhythm has the fewest taps, without regard to whether the fewest number of
taps represents the meter, or the embedded rhythm. Sadly, I have never had the time and/or opportunity
to perform the experiments that might tease apart the answers to these questions.
2: It is not only handedness that is worthy of detailed study. All of these different aspects, and their
interactions, could reveal much about how the brain organizes the production and perception of rhythm, and/or
timing.
10