Metacognition, Emotional Intelligence, Aggression and Passionate Love Among Women
Metacognition, Emotional Intelligence, Aggression and Passionate Love Among Women
Submitted by
JOSE KURIEN
I, Jose Kurien, hereby declare that the study presented in this dissertation was conducted by me
under the supervision of Dr. Saroj Arya, Professor, Institute of Mental Health, Sweekaar
I also declare that no part of this study has either been previously published or submitted as a
Date:
CERTIFICATE
Aggression and Passionate Love among Women” is a bonafide work carried out by Jose Kurien
This is to certify that this work submitted by the candidate as a dissertation in partial fulfillment
of the requirements for the M. Phil in Clinical Psychology has not formed the basis for the award
of any degree or diploma to any candidate. This is a bonafide record of the candidate’s personal
effort.
Guide:
Professor
Institute of Mental Health
Sweekaar Academy of Rehabilitation Sciences
Secunderabad-03
Forwarded:
Place: Secunderabad
Date:
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
I would like to thank the Chairman Dr. P. Hanumantha Rao of Sweekaar Academy of
I am extremely grateful to Dr. Bhasi Sukumaran, The Head of Department, Institute of Mental
I express my heartfelt gratitude and sincere thanks to Dr. Saroj Arya, my guide and Professor,
encouragement and co-operation, and contributed her treasured time and knowledge to the positive
development of this project. I admire her unconditional positive regard, ability to listen
endeavor.
I also express my sincere thanks to all the participants of this study for their valuable co-operation.
I am thankful to all the faculty members and non – teaching staff of Institute of Mental Health for
I would also like to thank my parents and family members for their support throughout the
endeavor.
CONTENTS
Page No.
Introduction…………………………………………………………………………… 2
Chapter 1 Metacognition……………………………………………………………... 9
Chapter 3 Aggression…………………………………………………………………. 30
Chapter 4 Love………………………………………………………………………… 35
Chapter 5 Methods……………………………………………………………………. 42
Chapter 6 Results……………………………………………………………………… 56
Chapter 7 Discussion………………………………………………………………….. 71
Chapter 8 Summary…………………………………………………………………… 87
References 96
Appendices 109
INTRODUCTION
1
INTRODUCTION
The concept of love and understanding of women always have been an enigma from time
immemorial despite the huge amount of thought put on it by several cultures over centuries and
the systematic research done during the modern times. Research on metacognition and emotional
intelligence has changed the field of psychology to a great extent and how we look at the concept
of human mind. Aggression always been a topic of interest from the very beginning of the field of
psychology or even before that when the field of psychology got detached from that of philosophy.
Metacognition
The knowledge and regulation of one’s thought processes is often referred to as “metacognition”.
As Veenman, Van Hout-Wolters and Afflerbach (2006) note, the domain of metacognition seems
to lack coherence. However, practically all the literature on the subject of metacognition refers to
the pioneering work of Flavell. He introduced metacognition as “knowledge and cognition about
cognitive phenomena” (1979), which is also referred to in terms such as “thinking about thinking”
or “higher order cognition of cognition” (e.g. Alexander et al., 1995; Hacker, 1998; and Veenman
et al., 2006). Flavell defined metacognition as consisting of both a component of knowledge, and
knowledge about the nature of people as cognizers, about the nature of different cognitive tasks,
and about possible strategies that can be applied to the solution of different tasks. It also includes
Emotional Intelligence
Emotional Intelligence cuts across the cognitive and emotional systems and is at one time unitary
and multidimensional, being sub-divisible into four branches. The first branch, emotional
2
perception/identification, involves perceiving and encoding information from the emotional
system. The second branch, emotional facilitation of thought, involves further processing of
emotion to improve cognitive processes with a view to complex problem solving. The third branch,
emotional understanding, is in some ways the obverse of the second: it concerns cognitive
processing of emotion. The fourth and final branch, emotion management, concerns the control
and regulation of emotions in the self and others (Mayer, Salovey, et al., 2000).Emotions are
contextualized, emergent phenomena, such that there are no right or wrong responses—no
accuracy in an absolute sense. However, some responses are better than others. Usually, judgments
about the desirability of a response are culturally and temporally situated. Thus, it is sensible to
consensual knowledge.
Aggression
Aggression acquires personal meaning for each individual as their aggressive impulses emerge
and take shape in the context of their relationships. In other words, our destructive feelings acquire
meanings from the ways others evoke, receive and understand our aggressive initiatives and
responses. Destructiveness is evoked when the sense of self feels endangered. In particular, the
If direct aggression is discouraged in females, it is likely that they instead resort to indirect forms
of aggression. The common feature of this type of aggression is that the aggressor can stay
unidentified and thereby avoid both counterattack from the aggressor and disapproval from the
rest of the community. According to Buss (1963), "indirect aggression may be verbal (spreading
nasty gossip) or physical (a man sets fire to his neighbor's home)." Archer and Coyne (2005) have
3
argued that a fuller understanding of the significance of indirect aggression can only be gained by
appreciating that it has different functions than direct aggression. Instead of putting the other
aggression may serve to remove a competitive rival from the arena of competition by excluding
them from a social group. A second major function of indirect aggression is to adversely influence
Love
Love is a form of value, and value takes many forms. It begins with drive in the unconscious core
of the self. Core value is bound up with drive in relation to the self-preservative instincts, such as
hunger. For micro-genetic theory, the core category and the drive do not come together but are
fused from the beginning in a single construct. Specifically, every affect has a conceptual frame
and every concept has an affective tonality. The broader implications of this view are that feeling
is the process of becoming and concepts (objects) are the substance of being (Brown, 2005).
The debate in psychology whether love is volition or need, that is, intentional or involuntary, or
what role decision plays in falling in love, reflects the proximity of the dominant segment to drive
or to desire. The closer to drive, the more love is like unconscious need. The closer love is to
desire, the more it is like wish, want, and intentional feeling. Volition does not precede and
motivate action or emotion but is part of the same phase to which emotion develops. More
precisely, the phase of desire is also one of volition, choice, and intentionality (Brown, 2012).
Researchers have observed relationship between cognitive knowledge and cognitive monitoring.
As literature suggests metacognitive experiences that allow one to monitor and regulate one’s
cognition play a major role in the development and refinement of metacognitive knowledge.
Emotion-related self-regulation which refers to monitoring and regulating the impact of emotions
4
and motivational states on one’s performance parallels the regulation of cognition involved in the
motivational states, including concepts such as effortful control and inhibitory control.
Emotional Intelligence cuts across the cognitive and emotional systems and is at one time unitary
to improve cognitive processes with a view to complex problem solving. Understanding when and
why we will feel pride, love, jealousy, and rage is a key component of emotional intelligence.
Metacognitive awareness draws on emotion concepts and is necessary for labelling an emotion,
for putting the emotion in a broader context, and for understanding an emotion. Negative emotions
have the potential for functional utility because there are times when the effects of negative
emotions are useful. Recognizing this potential for benefit is the key to understanding why it is
neither odd nor self-contradictory to claim that negative emotions can be useful or even desirable.
As early as 3 years of age, and across many cultures, females found to have greater ability than
Aggression in females is found to be less stable than in males. Female perpetrators use a form of
indirect aggression termed “social manipulation” more often than male perpetrators do. Indirect
aggression is typically a lower cost form of aggression than direct forms are, achieved either by
hiding the aggressor’s identity or by enabling them to deny aggressive intent. Indirect aggression
is perhaps most well developed in the social networks of female adolescence and young women,
and this fits with its suggested function, to reduce the social standing of potential rivals. Similar to
dieting, anorexia nervosa has been proposed to be a direct consequence of female intra-sexual
competition.
Love is the ultimate valuation of a thing, to which all desires are subordinate. There is nothing
more important than what is loved, which is to say that love, regardless of whether or not it is
5
implemented, realizes a value of immense signification. Core value is bound up with drive in
relation to the self-preservative instincts, such as hunger. The debate in psychology whether love
is volition or need, that is, intentional or involuntary, or what role decision plays in falling in love,
reflects the proximity of the dominant segment to drive or to desire. The closer to drive, the more
love is like unconscious need. The closer love is to desire, the more it is like wish, want, and
intentional feeling. Sadism is pleasure in pain inflicted on others, often linked to the sexual drive.
Masochism may be the enjoyment of such pain, but it is also a way of offering one’s self for
reciprocity or engagement. One can love or need a person so much that he/she accepts punishment
for interest. Some people require abuse and put up with constant rejection, even brutality, to satisfy
a need or in the hope of gaining love in return. Sadism and its opposite are not fully sexual nor are
they part of love but are related to aggression and defense as vectors for the drives. When giving
becomes sexualized, and reciprocity becomes punitive and the delicate balance of mutual surrender
that love requires is exploited in carnality or enslavement. The drive to individuation is opposed
to that for community, which is manifest in the security that family affords, not only for the child
but in the shared labor and mutual aid essential to marriage or its equivalent. Community reaches
its limit in abdication, individuation in alienation. Love resolves these competing pressures, one
to an extreme of independence, the other to possible loss of self. Possessiveness in love is closer
to hunger than sexuality, even with a sexual character, since the signature of true love is surrender,
not ownership. A desperate need for a beloved or a pathological jealousy may appear as great
passion, but it derives from the transition of predation to sexual capture. An intense love can vanish
with a word, evaporate on a misunderstanding, or insensitive remark. How fragile is a love that
cannot withstand a foolish jest, a jealous glance, a moment of detachment, a slight coolness, an
6
In this context, establishing a statistical correlation/ association between the variables of
metacognition, emotional intelligence, aggression and love among women would be desirous.
correlation between them has already been established. Both direct aggression & indirect
aggression together representing the total aggression and passionate love is selected to represent
The dissertation is divided into 9 chapters. The present introductory chapter is intended to provide
context and background for the study. Chapter 1 provides a review about the concept of
metacognition and the variable meta-awareness. Chapter 2 provides a review about the concept of
emotional intelligence. Chapter 3 provides a review about the concept of aggression. Chapter 4
provides a review about the concept of Love. Chapter 5 outlines the methodology adopted for the
current study. Chapters 6 and 7 presents the results and discussion of the findings in the context
of existing literature. Chapters 8 and 9 presents the summary and conclusion of the research.
References and appendices are provided in the last segment of the dissertation.
7
CHAPTER -1
METACOGNITION
8
CHAPTER -1
METACOGNITION
The knowledge and regulation of one’s thought processes is often referred to as “metacognition”.
pertains to one’s knowledge about how one’s cognition operates (“I know that I do not memorize
names well”), whereas metacognitive control pertains to how one controls one’s cognitive
operations. Nelson and Narens (1990,) conceptualized metacognition as operating at two levels:
the objective level and the meta-level. The objective level carries out cognitive operations, whereas
The triarchic theory of Intelligence by Sternberg (1994) supports the view that metacognitive
As Veenman, Van Hout-Wolters and Afflerbach (2006) note, the domain of metacognition seems
to lack coherence. Practically all the literature on the subject of metacognition refers to the
cognitive phenomena” (1979), which is also referred to in terms such as “thinking about thinking”
or “higher order cognition of cognition” (e.g. Alexander et al., 1995; Hacker, 1998; and Veenman
et al., 2006). Flavell defined metacognition as consisting of both a component of knowledge, and
experiences and regulation (1979). Building on the contributions of Flavell, Brown shifted the
focus to the monitoring and regulatory aspects of metacognition in her articles of the late seventies
(e.g. Brown, Campione & Barclay, 1979). On the other hand, Kluwe’s articles, published in the
eighties, focus on executive control as a key concept within the domain of metacognition in which
he makes the distinction between declarative and procedural knowledge (e.g. Kluwe, 1982).
9
Along with the discussion about the concept of metacognition, many terms have been developed
over the years, for example: metacognitive beliefs, metacognitive awareness, metamemory,
executive skills, self-regulation, metacognitive skills and so on (Veenman et al., 2006). Though
the diverse conceptualizations have differential focal points, the most common distinction seems
(exhibited in metacognitive skills concerning the regulation of, and control over cognitive
processes and learning) (Veenman, et al., 2005; 2006). Shraw and Graham (1997) for instance,
make the distinction between metacognitive knowledge and metacognitive control. In their view,
control processes consist of skills that help a person use that knowledge to regulate cognitive
processes. Alexander et al. (1995) also use a comparable distinction between declarative
metacognitive knowledge and a more procedural aspect to which they refer to as cognitive
monitoring. Besides these two factors, they have chosen to add a third factor: “strategy regulation
and control” which they use as a separate indicator of the metacognition referring to a strategy or
task. Yore and Craig (1992) use a definition of metacognition as consisting of the awareness and
Alexander et al. (1995) have not only assessed the development of metacognition tied to age, but
in addition have proposed four possible models for the development of metacognition in relation
to intelligence. They elaborated multiple possible hypotheses concerning this development. One
is the model of metacognitive abilities that develop in a monotonic fashion, parallel to the increase
of intellectual abilities over the years; “the monotonic development hypothesis”. In this model,
children of high intelligence will maintain monotonic superiority over children of lower
intelligence throughout life. Another possible model is provided by the “acceleration hypothesis”
10
in which we see effects of higher intelligence becoming larger with age. The third hypothesis posed
is the “ceiling hypothesis” that states that highly intelligent children develop metacognitive skills
faster, but effects diminish over time with lower intelligence children catching up eventually.
Finally, if there were no coherence between intelligence and metacognitive skills, Alexander, 1995
et al. state, no separate theory would be needed. The review of empirical evidence by Alexander,
intelligence and metacognition. The monotonic hypothesis seems to be the explanation most in
line with the ideas of general intelligence theorists, but most previous research lending support for
this hypothesis was done on average and retarded children, and much less on children of high
ability.
According to Freeman factors such as positive and supporting interactions, meaningful stimulation
of learning, a variety of experiences, freedom and materials to play and experiment and real
emotional support will be positive factors in the development of both abilities and metacognitive
skills (Freeman, 1993). Moreover, another factor of interest that may influence metacognitive
skills, as well as intellectual abilities, is “‘culture”. Culture is a package concept that comes as a
whole. It influences patterns of relationships, undermining the universality of certain theories and
models. In the assessment of intelligence, school performance or any other concept in different
cultural settings the validity of score comparisons has been - and still is - the focus of ample
scientific research and debate (e.g., Jensen, 1985; Helms-Lorenz, Van de Vijver & Poortinga,
2003). Subject related factors that can influence test performance of natives and migrants are:
verbal abilities, cultural norms and values, test-wiseness, and acculturation strategy. Most tests
call for high verbal abilities and skills. Natives and migrants differ in mastery of the native
language and cultural knowledge. Test instructions and item phrasing can unintentionally cause
11
bias and reduce the validity of inferences drawn from score comparisons (e.g., Van de Vijver &
Poortinga, 1997).
The central problem related to metacognition research is its estimation in the individual's cognitive
process while solving a problem or learning. This is a practical obstacle caused by the fact that
metacognition is an inner awareness or process rather than an overt behaviour (White, 1986), and
because individuals themselves are often not aware of these processes (Rowe, 1991).
In measuring metacognitive skills, most often verbal report techniques are used. These techniques
comprise off-line and on-line methods. Off-line methods frequently used in research on
metacognitive skills are questionnaires or interviews (compare Chan, 1996; Jakobs, & Paris, 1987;
Malpass, et al., 1999; Minnaert & Janssen, 1999; Perleth, 1992; Swanson, 1992; Yore & Craig,
1992). These off-line methods have the advantage that the collection of data is simple and takes
relatively less time. Interviews have the additional advantage that it is possible to continue
questioning until the meaning of the answer is clear. Disadvantages of both methods however, are
the possibility that learners may have forgotten, or simply might not mention, several relevant
learning activities. Also, the effects of reflection-skills of students and social pressure may
influence the responses. Questionnaires appear to be less suitable for elementary school pupils
(Van-Hout-Wolters, 2000).
in which the students are asked to think aloud during the performance of a task (compare Swanson,
1990; Veenman et al. 1997; 2000; 2004; 2005). The thinking aloud of the subjects is usually
recorded and transcribed. These transcriptions are then judged on metacognitive verbalizations.
This method is typically used in research but not often in educational practice. Assessment by
means of thinking aloud protocols, in which information is gathered during the learning activities,
12
has the advantage that little information is lost (Van Hout-Wolters, 2000). Disadvantages of
thinking-aloud protocols are found in the fact that the gathering and analyses of data is relatively
complex and time-consuming. Therefore, this on-line method is not frequently applied to large
groups of subjects. Besides, these measures may also be influenced by social desirability (Van
The teaching of metacognitive skills have seen to be a great stimulating factor for the development
of learning and abilities as evidenced in the Thinking Actively in a Social Context (TASC)
program in Africa. This TASC program proceeds from different conceptions of intelligence from
Vygotsky and Feuerstein to Sternberg’s theory of intelligence. Children are taught how, and when,
to use strategies, and are given ample practice in the application of skills and strategies that are
significant and relevant to the learners. Several studies have shown that training metacognitive
skills can benefit the working method and performance of students. For example, in the
(1999), the results point to a significant positive effect of a learning environment aimed at the
increase in the upper elementary school pupils’ problem solving skills. Furthermore, the
Maverech and Kramarski (1997) showed that seventh grade students who had been working with
mathematics achievement, particularly in mathematical reasoning. The students using this method
were instructed in new concepts, metacognitive questioning, previewing, verification and reducing
difficulties, and were supported in practice, obtaining of mastery, and enrichment. Veenman et al.
(2006) remarked that in supporting students by providing metacognitive instruction, the instruction
13
will have to be embedded in the content matter, must inform learners about the usefulness of
metacognitive skills and the training should be of considerable duration to guarantee thorough and
maintained appliance. They argued that all successful instructional programs should attend to
explaining: What to do, When to do it, Why do it, and How to do it, the so-called “WWW&H
rule”. Bearing in mind that metacognitive skillfulness appears to positively affect the performance
of minority students, similar metacognitive training programs can thus be useful in the stimulation
of performance among children with different cultural backgrounds, provided that possible cultural
*** ~~~~~~~***
14
CHAPTER -2
EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE
15
CHAPTER -2
EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE
Despite its historical roots, the concept of Emotional Intelligence (EI) is one of the most recently
defined categories of intelligence in the field of psychology, appearing at the beginning of the
concept of intelligence (Pfeiffer, 2001). Mayer and Salovey (1997) suggest that 18th century as
the time of origin of the concept of emotional intelligence, when scientists divided the brain into
three parts dedicated severally to cognition, affect, and motivation. The concept of emotional
intelligence aroused general interest because of the efficacy of its practical applications in
improving the individual’s skills in facing the new-age challenges, combined with the
dissemination of both concept and basic components through a number of new books and articles
Mayer and Salovey (1990) are credited with originating the term “emotional intelligence,”
considering it a form of social intelligence entailing the individual’s ability of both self-control
and of influencing emotions and feelings of others; the ability to distinguish different feelings and
emotions and use these skills to guide and influence ways of thought and action. According to him,
Emotional Intelligence cut across the cognitive and emotional systems and is at one time unitary
and multidimensional, being sub-divisible into four branches. The first branch, emotional
system. The second branch, emotional facilitation of thought, involves further processing of
emotion to improve cognitive processes with a view to complex problem solving. The third branch,
emotional understanding, is in some ways the obverse of the second: it concerns cognitive
processing of emotion. The fourth and final branch, emotion management, concerns the control
and regulation of emotions in the self and others (Mayer, Salovey, et al., 2000).
16
Cooper (1996) has conceptualized emotional intelligence as comprising emotional awareness of
self and others, interpersonal connections, resilience, creativity, compassion, and intuition (to
name but a few abilities) and developed an operational index: the EQ Map. Goleman’s (2001)
cognition, personality, motivation, emotions, neurobiology, and intelligence, rather than on this
Goleman (1995, p. 132) suggests that ‘‘Women, on the average, experience the entire range of
emotions with greater intensity and more volatility than men—in this sense, women are more
emotional than men’’. In contrast, citing his normative sample, Bar-On (2000) notes no differences
between males and females regarding overall emotional and social competence, though both
gender groups do show slight differences (in their favor) in some domains. (Females score higher
on interpersonal skills, men score higher on stress management and adaptation). In fact, existing
personality data (e.g., Feingold, 1994) suggest that women score higher on some of the personality
traits linked to EI, especially agreeableness and its components such as trust and tender-
mindedness. Men, on the other hand, score higher on others, such as emotional stability, and there
is no gender difference on still other relevant traits, such as conscientiousness and impulsivity.
As for gender differences, the available information on ethic differences in EI is both scant and
contradictory. Goleman’s (1995) conceptualization clearly implies that citizens of diverse, racial
and cultural origins can possess EI, in equal measure. Supporting this proposition, Bar-On (2000),
in examination of a North American sample, claims that there were no significant differences on
social and EI between various ethnic groups administered the EQ-i. However, using the Multi-
factor Emotional Intelligence Scale (MEIS), Roberts, Zeidner, et al. (2001) reported a rather
17
unusual phenomena. Using consensual scores there appeared no difference between ethnic groups,
and yet when expert scoring was used Whites outperformed minority groups on many of the sub-
scales.
Roberts, Zeidner, et al. (2001) reported that the types of investigation carried out to examine age
differences has been both meager and contradictory. Bar-On (2000), for example, claims relatively
small differences across the life-span, whereas Mayer, Salovey, et al. (2003) claim that increasing
age differences are required of the MEIS, if it measures a legitimate form of intelligence. In
providing preliminary data, they compared a sample of adolescents with a population of university
Among the earliest documented of the various group differences studied, it was the observation
that children of ‘‘superior’’ social class outperformed children of ‘‘inferior’’ social class on
measures of general intelligence (see, e.g., Terman & Merrill, 1937). These social class differences
appear to have remained with us to the present point in time (see Mackintosh, 1998; Mascie-Taylor
& Gibson, 1978; Waller, 1971). Roberts, Zeidner, et al. ( 2001) reports that despite extravagant
claims to the contrary, it remains entirely possible, given close links that we have already alluded
Cognitive emotion theory is compatible with biological and basic emotions approaches in
attributing broadly adaptive functions such as communication to emotion. It goes beyond such
approaches in specifying and differentiating multiple functions of emotion and linking them to
different components of the cognitive architecture. One of the distinctions often made (e.g.,
18
Ketelaar & Clore, 1997) is between emotion as information and emotion as motivation. The former
refers to the capacity of emotion (or, rather, the signals associated with emotion) to provide
information distinct from language like codes. The Oatley and Johnson-Laird (1996) model
describes the coordinative function of emotion signals. Emotion as information may influence
processing at different levels simultaneously, serving as input both to automatic biasing of discrete
processors and to higher-level executive function. Ketelaar and Clore (1997) make the intriguing
suggestion that emotions may signal the likely long-term outcome of encounters, based on evolved
routines for evaluation of the cues provided by a situation. Emotion as motivation refers to the
power of affect to elicit behaviors that increase happiness and prevent negative affect. Again, the
time factor may be important: Ketelaar and Clore (1997) argue that emotions regulate the
a long-term goal. As indicated above, it is uncertain whether emotions are rigidly tied to
evolutionary imperatives. The more pertinent aspect of Ketelaar and Clore’s (1997) position is that
emotions may be linked to processes for weighing up the value of response choices that are distinct
from conscious ratiocination (though still cognitive). The idea resembles Damasio’s (1994) view
that emotions facilitate social-problem solving, but Ketelaar and Clore’s formulation makes more
contact with the behavioral-research literature. For example, they describe how experimental
inductions of guilt influence behavior in a ‘‘prisoner’s dilemma’’ game in which the person has to
decide whether to behave cooperatively or antagonistically toward (in this case) a computer
opponent. Guilt (or whatever cognitions underpin guilt) tended to restrain people from taking
There is converging evidence from both neuroscience and cognitive science that EI may be a
quality of an executive control system for emotion regulation, supported by sites in the frontal
19
cortex (Rolls, 1999). Lesions to areas such as orbitofrontal cortex lead to substantial deficits in
social problem-solving (Bechara et al., 2000). More general control systems for attention and
decision-making also reside in frontal cortex (Shallice & Burgess, 1996). From the cognitive
perspective, the problems of modularity may be avoided if it is supposed that EI relates to some
superordinate executive system, of the kind established by experimental studies (Wells &
Matthews, 1994). The idea is also compatible with current theory linking EI to aspects of self-
regulation (Mayer et al., 2000), or to effective coping (Bar-On, 2000; Salovey et al., 1999). EI may
describe an executive system that makes adaptive selections of evaluative and action-oriented
processing routines. Nevertheless, the executive hypothesis faces significant difficulties. Like any
other cognitive system, it is supported by multiple components, and it is unclear which components
are critical. Furthermore, much of the wisdom of the executive resides in its store of procedural
knowledge in long-term memory (Wells & Matthews, 1994), whose quality reflects a variety of
factors including exposure to supportive learning environments and transferability from past to
present circumstances.
According to appraisal theorists, emotions are tightly coupled with specific computational
operations. Analyzing a stimulus as threatening implies anxious emotion, and vice versa. The
implication is that EI may be a quality of these computations of the personal relevance of the
stimulus. Hence EI can be conceptualized as either the efficiency or positive bias of these
computations. The emotionally intelligent person might have more accurate evaluations of
significant stimuli, or they might be biased towards evaluation of stimuli as positive rather than
negative, leading to qualities such as optimism, happiness, and positive self-beliefs (see Bar-On,
2000).
20
Research on Emotional Intelligence and Coping
A modicum of research has been conducted linking EI components to effective coping strategies.
Bar-On (1997) reports that the EQ-i correlates moderately with high task-focused coping and low
emotion-focused coping. However, exactly this result would be expected based on the high
correlation between EQ-i and neuroticism, which is similarly related to coping (Endler & Parker,
1990, 1999). Ciarrochi, Chan, and Bajgar (2001) report a study of the Schutte et al. (1998) in which
EI scale in adolescents that did not address stress directly but provided some mixed findings. On
the positive side, EI was related to perceived social support. The authors also found that a subscale
generated by participants, depending on the type of mood previously induced, an effect attributed
to use of mood-regulation strategies such as mood repair (negative mood induction) and mood
maintenance (positive induction). On the negative side, EI failed to moderate emotional response
to negative and positive mood inductions used in the study, so the mood management strategies
attributed to EI did not appear to be effective in regulating mood in this context. Results of studies
using the Multi-factor Emotional Intelligence Scale (MEIS) have been somewhat confusing.
Ciarrochi, Chan, and Caputi (2000) suggest that the MEIS indexes individual differences in mood
management but the evidence for this claim is somewhat indirect, and EI had no effect on negative
mood response. A study on a female sample found that skill at mood repair was not meaningfully
associated with active coping but rather associated with less trait and state passive coping (Salovey,
Stroud, Woolesy & Epel, in press). These differences may well be attributed to gender differences
Emotional Disorders
The mood disorders and anxiety disorders describe a group of conditions in which mood must be
altered for the diagnosis to be made. Various anxiety disorders (e.g., generalized anxiety, panic
21
disorder, phobia) require the presence of unusual or intense fear or anxiety. Negative emotions
must be accompanied by other criteria for a clinical diagnosis to be made. For a major depressive
episode to be diagnosed, the patient, in addition to meeting other criteria, must experience either
depressed mood for most of nearly every day or marked decrease in interest or pleasure in nearly
all activities. Positive moods may also be a sign of pathology. Bipolar disorders are characterized
by alternating episodes of depression and mania, i.e., abnormal, persistent elevation of mood.
For more than half the days in at least 6 months, the patient experiences excessive anxiety
Other anxiety and worry symptoms include feeling restless, tired, and irritable. May have
Symptoms cause clinically important distress or impair work, social or personal functioning.
2. Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder
Patient must have obsessions or compulsions (or both) which cause severe distress, and
awareness and cause marked distress or anxiety. The patient tries to ignore or suppress these
Compulsions. The patient feels the need to repeat physical behaviors (checking the stove to
be sure it is off, hand washing) or mental behaviors (counting things, silently repeating
words). These behaviors aim to reduce or eliminate distress, but they are not realistically
22
3. Panic Disorder (may be diagnosed with or without agoraphobia)
The patient experiences recurrent panic attacks, defined as the sudden development of a
severe fear or discomfort that peaks within 10 minutes. During this discrete episode, 4 or
Chest pain or other chest discomfort, Chills or hot flashes, Choking sensation, De-
lightheaded, faint or unsteady, Fear of dying, Fears of loss of control or becoming insane,
Heart pounds, races or skips beats, Nausea or other abdominal discomfort, Numbness or
For a month or more after at least 1 of these attacks, the patient has had 1 or more of:
Ongoing concern that there will be more attacks, Worry as to the significance of the attack
or its consequences (for health, control, sanity), Material change in behavior, such as
The patient has experienced or witnessed an unusually traumatic event that involved actual
or threatened death or serious physical injury to the patient or to others, and the patient felt
Patient repeatedly relives the event through, e.g., intrusive recollections, distressing dreams,
Patient repeatedly avoids thinking about the event, and situations that recall the event
Patient has numbing of general responsiveness; e.g., feels detached from other people
23
5. Social Phobia
The patient strongly, repeatedly fears at least one social or performance situation that
involves facing strangers or being watched by others. The patient specifically fears showing
anxiety symptoms or behaving in some other way that will be embarrassing or humiliating.
The phobic stimulus almost always causes anxiety, which may be a cued or situationally
The patient either avoids the situation or endures it with severe distress or anxiety.
Either there is marked distress about having the phobia or it markedly interferes with the
6. Major Depression
At least one major depressive episode, i.e., in the same 2 weeks, the patient has had 5 or more
of the following symptoms, occurring for most of nearly every day. Either depressed mood
Mood. Patient reports depressed mood or appears depressed to others; Interests. Interest or
pleasure is markedly decreased in nearly all activities; Eating and weight. Marked change in
appetite or actual weight; Sleep. The patient sleeps excessively or not enough; Motor activity.
The patient’s activity is agitated or retarded; Fatigue. There is fatigue or loss of energy; Self-
worth. The patient feels worthless or inappropriately guilty; Concentration. The patient is
indecisive or has trouble thinking or concentrating; Death. The patient has repeated thoughts
Symptoms cause clinically important distress or impair work, social or personal functioning.
24
Non-Emotional aspects of Emotional Intelligence in Mental Disorders
Many of the defining characteristics of EI refer not directly to emotional state, but to qualities
related to awareness and management of emotion. In some disorders, dysfunction of these qualities
is more apparent than any overall mood disturbance. Impaired self-control is one such quality.
Disorders associated with poor impulse control fall under several DSM headings, including
personality disorders (e.g., antisocial personality), and bipolar disorder (during manic episodes).
Severely disorganized behavior is an important criterion, though not a necessary one, for
and flat or inappropriate affect must be present. Impulse control disorders, which refers to
trichotillomania (persistent hair-pulling and extraction). Often, these conditions are associated
with anxiety before the impulsive act and relief afterwards. In children, impulsivity is a common
Another feature of low EI is interpersonal difficulties due to lack of insight into others’ feelings
and motives. Difficulties in relating to others are common in various mental disorders, including,
personality disorders. Pervasive developmental disorders in which the child fails to develop social
skills, notably autistic disorder, whose diagnosis requires at least 2 indicators of impaired social
relationships, and lack of social or emotional reciprocity. Social dysfunction due to inaccurate
appraisal of others may also be experienced by mood and anxiety patients, and by schizophrenics.
Several disorders are not overtly associated with emotion or emotion regulation criteria, but are
nevertheless linked to emotion by research. At the diagnostic level, it is not uncommon for various
25
medical and psychiatric disorders to be comorbid with anxiety and/or mood disorder (Kroenke,
Jackson & Chamberlin, 1997). Carson et al. (2000) studied 300 patients referred to a general
neurology clinic. Of these patients 140 met criteria for at least one DSM-IV mental disorder. These
patients presented with poorer physical and somatic function, worse somatic symptoms, and more
pain. Consistent with these results, neurotic personality (high N) tends to be linked to DSM-IV
somatoform disorders, in which the patient complains of recurring physical symptoms for which
no organic basis is found. Somatization disorder is defined entirely in terms of somatic symptoms
clinically significant distress resulting from symptoms, rather than the symptoms themselves,
together beliefs that one has or is developing a serious disease. Even in nonclinical samples, N is
associated with number of medical symptoms reported (Costa & McCrae, 1985) and with measures
of hypochondriasis and health anxiety (Cox et al., 2000; Wells, 1994). N is also linked to some
specific somatoform complaints, such as globus pharynges, feeling a lump in the throat, in the
absence of any physical cause (Deary, Wilson & Kelly, 1995). Likewise, N is also linked to
psychosomatic illnesses (Kirmayer et al., 1994), in which psychological factors contribute to actual
diseases such as ulcer, and it is often difficult to distinguish the psychological and medical
Another major classification in DSM is for substance-related disorders. Again, criteria for
substance use make no overt reference to emotion, but N tends to be elevated in chronic users of
various legal and illegal substances, including alcohol (Martin & Sher, 1994) and opiate drugs
(Doherty & Matthews, 1988). The causal role of N is uncertain, however, in that N scores tend to
drop as alcoholism is treated, implying an effect of the disorder on personality (Shaw et al., 1997).
Conversely, other evidence suggests that substance use may sometimes be a coping strategy
adopted by stress-vulnerable individuals intended to neutralize the impact of some threat (Riskind,
26
Gessner & Wolzon, 1999). Other groups of DSM-IV conditions known to be linked to higher N
include eating disorders (Goldner et al., 1999), sexual disorders (Eysenck, 1976; Kennedy et al.,
1999), and sleep disorders (Dorsey & Bootzin, 1997). Broadly, these findings suggest that the
that are not explicitly emotional, although in some cases the causal association between negative
disorders are linked to direct or indirect expression of negative affect, or to aspects of EI, such as
self-control and interpersonal relationships. However, the diversity of mental disorders linked to
emotional disturbance mitigates against any clear-cut relationship between low EI and
somatoform disorders, substance abuse, and eating, sexual and sleep disorders into a superordinate
EI category makes little sense clinically. Despite overlaps, these conditions have distinct etiologies
and require different treatments. The position that low EI is one of many factors associated with
vulnerability to a range of clinical pathologies is perhaps tenable, but low EI fails to emerge from
the absence of compelling evidence that EI exists independent of diagnoses that themselves have
varying degrees of reliability. Furthermore, a more coherent set of diagnostic categories may be
primarily a quality of the anxiety and mood disorders, there is considerable comorbidity between
these disorders and those in which negative emotion appears to be expressed indirectly, e.g.
through somatoform disorder. Anxiety and depression are also often comorbid with impulse-
control disorders such as ADHD (Biederman et al., 1991), and with disorders related to poor social
27
skills such as autism and Asperger syndrome (Green et al., 2000; Kim et al., 2000). N is elevated
in children with ADHD (White, 1999), though there has been little work on autism and personality
traits. Generally, it appears that N operates as a generalized vulnerability factor that may interact
with other diathesis and stressor factors to produce a range of more specific pathologies, although
there may be some mutual, reciprocal influence between personality and pathology over time
(Widiger & Trull, 1992). However, it is difficult to make the case that N should be re-
conceptualized as low EI, despite the substantial correlations between personality-like measures
for the two constructs. The conclusions are tentative because of difficulties with DSMIV and the
similar World Health Organization ICD-10 scheme. There are continuing problems with validating
the different diagnostic categories, as separate, unitary entities due to lack of conclusive evidence,
and conceptual disagreements on how mental disorders should be distinguished from each other,
and from normal functioning (Widiger & Clark, 2000). According to Farmer and McGuffin (1999),
no classificatory scheme may claim validity, because the causes of most disorders are uncertain.
In addition, empirical data on specific groups of disorders, notably the personality disorders,
conflict with the distinctions made in DSM-IV (Widiger, 1997). Although anxiety and depression
appear to be clinically distinct, they are frequently comorbid, and scales for the two syndromal
conditions are highly correlated (Watson & Clark, 1997). It may be that a hierarchical model is
required, such that anxiety and depression are lower-order dimensions linked to an overarching
general negative affect factor (Mineka, Watson & Clark, 1998; Steer et al., 1995).
*** ~~~~~~~***
28
CHAPTER -3
AGGRESSION
29
CHAPTER -3
AGGRESSION
The first comprehensive theoretical account of the etiology of aggression which assigned a major
role to learning theory was the monograph, Frustration and Aggression by Dollard, Doob, Miller,
Mowrer, and Sears (1939), a group of psychologists at Yale University. The Yale group's
hypothesis about instigation to injure the frustrator finds a close parallel in Freud's (1915)
statement that "if the object is a source of unpleasant feelings ... " this can eventually lead to "an
aggressive inclination against the object ... an instigation to destroy it" (p. 137). The premise thus
of the frustration-aggression hypothesis is that when people become frustrated (i.e., when their
goals are thwarted), they respond aggressively. The basic premise that frustration was a necessary
precursor to aggression, was questioned by a number of researchers (Buss, 1963; Cohen, 1955;
Pastore, 1952). Neal Miller (1941) had denied the inevitability of aggression as a response to
frustration with the statement that, "Frustration produces instigations to a number of different types
of responses, one of which is an instigation to some form of aggression" (p. 338). New and
aggression than it did on inherent or drive factors (Bandura, 1973; Eron, 1987).
Bandura (1973) proposed that aggressive behavior is learned and maintained through
any new behaviors (Bandura, 1973, p. 57). Bandura (1973) points out that according to a social-
contrast, from a drive model perspective, aggression is usually mediated by negative reinforcement
or the escape from an aversive situation. The model that Rowell Huesmann (1977, 1980, 1982,
1988) and Eron (Eron, 1982, 1987; Huesmann & Eron, 1984) proposed to explain the development
30
of aggression was based on the models of human cognition which had been elaborated in the 1960s
of behavior. These positive consequences can include tangible rewards (e.g., desired objects),
psychological benefits (e.g., control or dominance over others), self-evaluations (e.g., increased
feelings of self-worth), and social reactions (e.g., status among peers). Studies of preadolescent
children have shown that aggressive children are more likely than their less aggressive peers to
predict that aggressive behavior will result in tangible rewards and termination of aversive
behavior toward them by others (Perry, Perry, & Rasmussen, 1986), as well as psychological
reinforcement based on control over peers (Boldizar, Perry, & Perry, 1989).
Traditionally, men and boys have been regarded as more aggressive than women and girls. This is
supported by the fact that, with few exceptions, males are more aggressive than females in most
animal species. A review of these issues was presented by Moyer (1977). In humans, there is
evidence for a higher level of physical aggression in males than in females. Criminal statistics
show that men outnumber women as perpetrators of physical violence in all societies. Reviews
have usually found males to score higher on measures of aggression than females (Frodi,
Macaulay, & Thome, 1977; Frost & Averill, 1982; Huesmann & Eron, 1984; Huesmann, Eron,
Lefkowitz, & Walder, 1984; Lambert, 1985; Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974; Rauste-von Wright, 1989).
In an international follow-up study in six countries (Huesmann & Eron 1986), the self-ratings of
boys correlated positively with their peer rated aggression in most countries and samples, but for
girls, the correlations between self-ratings and peer-ratings were generally poor. This result may
reflect the difficulty a girl has in admitting that she is aggressive, and in thinking about and
31
Direct aggression with females as perpetrators may depend, in relative terms, more than its male
counterpart on the situation than on norms of behavior. This suggestion is based on findings that
females are likely to react aggressively, similarly to males, if the setting seems to call for it.
Females will be aggressive, for instance, if they are provoked, or if the aggression is seen by them
to be justified in the situation, but typically they will not show as much unprovoked aggression as
males do (Frodi et al., 1977; Lambert, 1985). This fact may well be due to culturally determined
sex roles in regard to aggression. Pulkkinen (1987) suggests that the behavior of males tends to be
more of the offensive type, whereas that of females is confined more to the defensive type. The
distinction between offensive and defensive aggression is, however, problematic due to the fact
that what should be regarded as a provocation is not self-evident. There is also evidence suggesting
that women do not become provoked as easily as men. According to Zillman (1979), women are
more effective in minimizing the provocations and in handling annoyance in other ways than by
counter aggression. Zillman thinks that the conflict solution strategies used by women are often
more efficient than those used by men, since the latter often lead to an escalation of the conflict.
If direct aggression is discouraged in females, it is likely that they instead resort to indirect forms
of aggression. This would show only in measurements indicating indirect instead of direct
aggression. The common feature of this type of aggression is that the aggressor can stay
unidentified and thereby avoid both counterattack from the aggressor and disapproval from the
rest of the community. According to Buss (1961, p. 8), "indirect aggression may be verbal
(spreading nasty gossip) or physical (a man sets fire to his neighbour’s home)." Measurement
techniques usually do not detect indirect aggression precisely because it is indirect, and the
perpetrator will disguise her or his aggression, or refrain completely from aggressive behaviour if
she or he is aware of being observed. If asked about one's own aggression in a questionnaire, the
person who uses indirect forms of aggression would typically also be expected to deny being
32
aggressive. A typical feature of psychological follow-up studies is that aggression in females is
found to be less stable than in males (Huesmann et al., 1984; Huesmann & Eron, 1986; Olweus,
*** ~~~~~~~***
33
CHAPTER -4
LOVE
34
CHAPTER -4
LOVE
Love is a form of value, and value takes many forms. It begins with drive in the unconscious core
of the self. Core value is bound up with drive in relation to the self-preservative instincts, such as
hunger. For microgenetic theory, the core category and the drive do not come together but are
fused from the beginning in a single construct. Specifically, every affect has a conceptual frame
and every concept has an affective tonality. The broader implications of this view are that feeling
is the process of becoming and concepts (objects) are the substance of being (Brown, 2005).
The debate in psychology whether love is volition or need, that is, intentional or involuntary, or
what role decision plays in falling in love, reflects the proximity of the dominant segment to drive
or to desire. The closer to drive, the more love is like unconscious need. The closer love is to
desire, the more it is like wish, want, and intentional feeling. Volition does not precede and
motivate action or emotion but is part of the same phase to which emotion develops. More
precisely, the phase of desire is also one of volition, choice, and intentionality. Love can begin
with need and evolve to desire or it can begin with desire and descend to need, and find its
The idea that love is an illusion, that is, a distorted perception, or even a delusion or false belief,
is ingrained in skeptical thought. This view owes to the irrationality of love, its fragility, loss,
betrayal, and the disillusionment that often follows. For some, only facts are real, for others, only
feelings. Emily Dickinson wrote that the deeper the love, the less the illusion. It is not love that is
illusory, but the common illusion of being in love when a genuine fusion of the lovers has not
occurred. A deep love is more real in the intensity of feeling, which is an outcome of the evolution
of energy that is the life of all matter. The beloved can be purely imaginary, absent, abusive, or
dead. But even with the absence of an actual other, we can truly say we are in love and that the
35
love is not delusion, but we can have the delusion we are loved in return. Even in cases of obsessive
love or homicidal jealousy, when there are no grounds to believe in the other’s love or infidelity,
we cannot say the love one feels is unreal, or no less real than any other mental phenomenon
(Brown 2012).
Freud tried to account for relation of feeling to memory, to idea, dream and object by postulating
the cathexis of memory traces by libidinal drive energy. He argued that memory traces were static
entities deposited by perceptions. For Freud, one meaning of the timelessness of the Ucs concerned
the identity of registration and retention (Leowald, 2000). The perception-memory trace was, in
cathected—by libidinal drive energy, but how circulating energy locates the trace, or the trace
attracts the energy was not discussed. The concept that sexual drive energy binds with a trace to
form an idea is untenable. How among the innumerable memories, or potential neural
configurations that correspond to memories, does drive energy find the proper trace? How does
the trace attract the energy? Is there a physical “address” on the trace that lures the energy; is
energy uniform or differentiated into distinct feelings that map to the specific ideas? In spite of the
many problems with a combinatorial theory, the notion survives to the present, for example in the
view of the brain in terms of older limbic areas for emotion and the association to memory images,
and neocortical zones for thought and ”higher” cognition. For micro-genetic theory, traces are not
static entities but consist of the entire sequence from core to surface in the arousal of a mental
content, while drive energy is derived to a succession of states of feeling and the “trace” and its
feeling-tone develop through intermediate stages. On the micro-genetic view, the “structure” of
Contemporary accounts of love tend to describe what passes between a subject and an object, a
lover, and a beloved, each “bringing to the table” some attributes, emotions, and values in a
36
relationship that is like a negotiation in that it rests on the cooperative interaction of separate
individuals. For some this negotiation is a contestation of power, the completion of an inventory
projection of the subject’s needs onto the other or, on a more rational plane, a bestowal, a judgment,
or appraisal of what is desirable or undesirable. On this view, value is transferred to the other,
much like to any other object, and a judgment is made as to whether the value is justified by the
qualities. This entails that the subject encounters the other as a mirror of the self or as a supplement
for what is lacking, as if adding the other will harmonize with, magnify or compensate for what is
present or lacking in the self. Subject and object are autonomous units that combine with greater
or lesser success. The romantic ideal of oneness gives way to shared interests, mutual benefits and
equality of labor, more like a partnership or contractual relation than a unity of souls. This way of
thinking, which can be occasioned by hard work and the pressure to survive, is now the outcome
of an analytic materialism in which couples are conceived as the interlocking parts of a machine,
a collection of autonomous units that interact through surface contacts, while the romantic ideal of
Another way to look at the nature of passionate and true love begins with subjectivity and attempts
to go from the inner life of the lover to an intra-psychic absorption of the beloved. It accepts the
other as an entity independent of the subject but apprehends the other as intra-psychic yet extra-
personal, a creation of the self’s imagination. Love and allied states such as affection, interest,
desire, compassion, or for that matter rejection and disappointment can be interpreted in relation
to their psychic infra-structure. The subjectivities of lover and beloved are not interactive but
assimilative. The authenticity of the love depends on the degree of coherence across corresponding
yet inaccessible psyches as well as the mutuality of need, desire, and genuine giving that each
offers and receives. On this view, the beloved is infused with the subjectivity of the lover, truly
37
possessed, and created in the conceptual imagination as the center of a cognized world. The
stronger is the imagination, the greater the fantasy, the more facile the formation of an ideal, the
deeper, that is, the more subjective, the love and the less the objectivity. Conversely, the more
objective the love, the less love there is. To see one’s beloved in an objective manner is to accept,
to approve, to judge, and appraise, but not to truly love. This follows if idealization is essential to
loving. The unmitigated fact is that love has to be unreal to be true or, rather, the unreal has to be
the heart’s only reality. Regardless of whether one agrees with this assertion, to interpret love as
idealization is for those attributes that distinguish the beloved from others. In a word, the self of
the beloved is inculcated in relation to need, with the qualities idealized to reinforce and justify
that love. Were this not true, love could not survive aging or illness, in which some attributes are
lost or altered. Conversely, we know that the attributes of a childhood sweetheart, frozen in time,
grow ever fonder in recollection, as do those of a loved one after death. This follows if true love
perception; a delusion is a false belief. One can falsely believe that one loves or is loved, and
misperceive the intentions of a lover. Lovers can be delusional, but is love itself an illusion or
From an evolutionary standpoint, jealousy is part of the instinctual repertoire that helps to
safeguard paternity for males and, for females, serves to ensure protection and assistance in child-
rearing (Buss, 1994). Regardless of its utility or adaptive value, the psychic structure of jealousy
is similar in men and women, with slight though not unimportant biases. These include the
tendency, in men, for greater discomfort over sexual infidelity, while for women an infidelity that
involves a transfer of affection tends to trump distress over casual sex. If men are less faithful than
38
women, it may reflect a difference in libido, opportunity, egocentricity, or risk-taking. The greater
ability of men to limit infidelity to a purely sexual encounter without emotional engagement allows
them to encapsulate the activity in states of objectified action that is relatively affect-free. Perhaps
for this reason, men tend to be more vindictive for sexual indiscretion than emotional reallocation,
and women more forgiving for sexual dalliance but not an affair of the heart (Brown 2012).
The drive to individuation is opposed to that for community, which is manifest in the security that
family affords, not only for the child but in the shared labor and mutual aid essential to marriage
or its equivalent. Community reaches its limit in abdication, individuation in alienation. Love
resolves these competing pressures, one to an extreme of independence, the other to possible loss
of self. We see these pressures at work at many social levels: autonomy of self and oneness with
the other; individuality and dependency; solitude and companionship; and the claims of the
individual versus those of the community that are central to so many legal and moral issues. We
should also examine anger, jealousy, and rejection in this light, that is, in the tension of self-giving
and self-protection. Possessiveness in love is closer to hunger than sexuality, even with a sexual
character, since the signature of true love is surrender, not ownership. A desperate need for a
beloved or a pathological jealousy may appear as great passion, but it derives from the transition
The prevalence of selfish need over unselfish giving, the uncertainty of love or its pretense, the
diversity of unions between and among the sexes, and the natural evolution of love over time, and
at different points in the life cycle, are all responsible for the cynicism that attends to discussions
of true love. There are also the covert motives, the self-interest, the counterfeit, the delusion, and
the covetousness that drive most human behaviors. To the extent love is a conscious decision, the
feeling is diminished, while to the extent it is a passion, it is mocked for lack of discernment. Love
39
as rational appraisal cannot be resolved with the union of genuine loving, though the natural
relaxation of passionate beginnings allows reason to creep into feeling as desire wanes and is
refined to subtler affect-ideas. We do not all find love, or if we find it, keep it, and we may have
to choose between a rapture that passes and an affection that is less passionate but enduring. There
is also the opposition of desire with a calculus of utility and the consumerism and corruption of
the deeper sensitivities, which make us believe that love is for the young, the innocent, or the
guileless. There is the fact that unfulfilled desire may persist, but once satisfied may not recur, so
that the sought-after fusion of lovers is the death of longing. If autonomy and self-sufficiency are
compensated. If a distraction, it is not worth the bother; if an obsession, it needs a cure. The analytic
mind asks if love is an illusory surrogate for an uncompromising objectivity in which a life without
illusion is a life without meaning. The impulse to self-protection leaves no room for the
vulnerabilities on which love depends. Selfishness, isolation, lack of generosity, and the
displacement of feeling to surrogate pursuits are some of its manifestations. The misanthrope and
sociopath are the extremes. The fundamental defect of character is a resistance to the assimilation
Love is the ultimate valuation of a thing, to which all desires are subordinate. There is nothing
more important than what is loved, which is to say that love, regardless of whether or not it is
implemented, realizes a value of immense signification. Apart from the self-preservative drives, a
genuine love for a person, for a thing or an occasion, from the perspective of the individual,
announces an object of the greatest possible value. Love and death, arising and perishing, creation
and destruction, are thematic in life and literature. A metaphoric death at a moment of great passion
*** ~~~~~~~***
40
CHAPTER -5
METHODS
41
CHAPTER -5
METHODS
The aim of the present study was to understand the relationship between metacognitive awareness,
emotional intelligence, aggression and passionate love among the female population, with the
following implications:
emotional intelligence.
of research looking at possible practice strategies to contain aggressive drive within healthy
42
Understanding the relationship between emotional intelligence and aggression to propose
emotional intelligence.
viability of research looking at possible practice strategies to contain aggressive drive within
healthy limits through strategies aimed at improving emotional intelligence among normal
43
Understanding the relationship between emotional intelligence and passionate love to
Understanding the relationship between passionate love and aggression to propose viability
Understanding the relationship between aggression and passionate love to propose viability
of research looking at possible practice strategies to optimize the balance between expression
Objectives
To find out the correlation that may exist between metacognitive awareness and emotional
To find out the correlation that may exist between metacognitive awareness and aggression
among women.
To find out the correlation that may exist between metacognitive awareness and passionate
To find out the correlation that may exist between emotional intelligence and aggression
among women.
To find out the correlation that may exist between emotional intelligence and passionate love
among women.
To find out the correlation that may exist between aggression and passionate love among
women
44
Hypotheses
among women.
There will be significant relationship between metacognitive awareness and passionate love
among women.
There will be significant relationship between emotional intelligence and aggression among
women.
There is significant relationship between emotional intelligence and passionate love among
women
Design
Within group design correlational study with purposive sampling employing cross sectional
Sample
Purposive sampling was used for the present study. With the assumption that the variables follow
normal distribution, the size of sample was set with lower limit of 30 to apply parametric tests for
analysis and the desirable limit was set at 107 to keep the error within 5% (error prob. = .05) and
95% confidence limit (power = .95) with conventional large effect size (0.5) (Two independent
Pearson’s r, two tail, allocation ratio N1/N2 = 1, critical z = 1.959964). Sample consisted of
women ready to participate in the study who matched the inclusion and exclusion criteria.
45
Study Locale
Participants of the study were from Sweekaar Academy of Rehabilitation Sciences, Secunderabad;
National Institute for the Empowerment of Persons with Intellectual Disabilities, Manovikas
Secunderabad; Krishna Institute of Medical Sciences & College of Physiotherapy, Minister Road,
Secunderabad; Mount Litera Zee School, Hayatnagar, Hyderabad and a few residential apartments
Inclusion Criteria
Exclusion Criteria
Presence of disability
Tools
46
Description of the Tools
Socio-demographic data sheet for this study includes information about the participant’s socio-
demographic variables like age, education, occupation/vocation, marital status, mother tongue
Metacognitive awareness inventory was developed by Gregory Schraw and Rayne Sperling
52 items classified into eight sub components subsumed under two broad categories knowledge
of cognition and regulation of cognition. Every true item is scored 1 and every false item is
knowledge (knowledge about self and strategies), procedural knowledge (knowledge about
how to use strategies) and conditional knowledge (knowledge about when and why to use 36
strategies) (Schraw & Dennison, 1994). Regulation of metacognition includes five areas:
one’s learning and strategy), debugging (strategies used to correct errors) and evaluation
(analysis of performance and strategy effectiveness after a learning episode) (Schraw &
Dennison, 1994).
The MAI was subjected to factor analysis. The first analysis produced a 6-factor solution with
coefficient alpha for five of the six factors below 0.80 (Schraw & Dennison, 1994). Next, the
authors of the test forced a two-factor solution with factor one corresponding to knowledge of
cognition and factor two to regulation of cognition. Six items loaded equally on factor one and
two (items 27, 34, 34, 40, 43, and 44), and two items failed to load on either factor (items 4
and 48). The coefficient alpha for items loading on each factor was as high as 0.91, indicating
47
high internal consistency, and the coefficient alpha for the whole instrument reached 0.95
(Schraw & Dennision, 1994). The two factors accounted for 65% of the sample variance. The
authors suggest that the results indicated that the MAI reliably measures knowledge of
cognition and regulation of cognition based on a two-factor solution (Schraw & Dennison,
developed by Schutte et al. (1998), using four sub-scales: emotion perception, utilizing
emotions, managing self- relevant emotions, and managing others’ emotions. It is structured
off of the EI model by Salovey and Mayer (1990). Every item is rated on a 5 point Likert scale
and item number 5, 28 and 33 are reverse scored. From the norms available, average score for
females is 131, that for males is 125, that for therapist is 135 and that for prisoners is 120.
Schutte and her colleges report a reliability rating of 0.90 for their emotional intelligence scale.
The EI score, overall, is fairly reliable for adults and adolescents; however, the utilizing
emotions sub-scale has shown poor reliability (Ciarrochi, Chan & Bajgar, 2001).
which s/he can appraise and regulate emotions in self and others, and utilize emotions for
problem solving (Schutte et al., 1998). In the initial validation study, Schutte et al. used a set
of 62 items derived from the model of Salovey and Mayer (1990). Exploratory factor analysis
on data from 346 participants yielded a 4-factor model. The authors argued that by removing
29 items and reanalyzing the data, an adequate 1-factor solution was produced. Schutte et al.
reported adequate internal consistency reliability (r = .87 to .90) and acceptable test-retest
reliability (r= .78) for the one-dimensional scale. Furthermore, there was evidence that the EIS
was distinct from established measures of the Big Five. Subsequent validation studies rightly
48
questioned the one-dimensional structure of the EIS and sought to establish a multifactorial
solution. For instance, Petrides and Furnham (2000) sought to test the hypothesis, via
confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), that the EIS measured a general factor of EI. Results
indicated that the general factor model provided a poor fit to the data (e.g., comparative fit
index: CFI = .51, root mean square error of approximation: RMSEA = .11). Consequently,
Petrides and Furnham (2000) followed their CFA analysis with an unrestricted (exploratory)
appraisal of emotions, social skills, and utilization of emotions. Saklofske, Austin, and Minski
(2003) subjected the EIS to CFA and found moderate support for the 4-factor model. Gignac,
Palmer, Manocha, and Stough (2005) recognized the limitations associated with the use of
exploratory data-driven procedures to demonstrate factorial validity (see Thompson & Daniel,
1996) and sought to establish whether a theoretically derived solution would yield improved
of six dimensions of EI were extracted for further analysis. CFA results suggested that the
model with a first-order general factor, and four nested factors corresponding to appraisal of
emotions in the self, appraisal of emotions in others, emotional regulation of the self, and
emotional expression or emotional regulation of others factor. Gignac et al. (2005) argued that
further validation work on the scale was needed if the intention of the scale was to assess the
theoretical model proposed by Salovey and Mayer (1990). Lane et al. (2009) sought to expand
on the work of Gignac et al. (2005) by establishing a factor structure consistent with theoretical
49
proposals of Salovey and Mayer (1990). A principle reason for investigating the EIS offered
by Lane et al. was to determine the validity of the scale for use with an athletic population.
Lane et al. scrutinized the 33- items of the EIS for content validity and categorized them into
one of six dimensions based on EI theory (appraisal of own emotions, appraisal of others’
of emotion, and optimism). CFA yielded a poor fit to the data for a single-factor solution
typically used in the literature and an acceptable fit to the data for the theoretically derived 6-
factor solution. In an attempt to improve model fit, Lane and colleagues re-specified the model:
Using CFA results in conjunction with content validity results they removed 13 items that
contained no emotional content. Lane et al. (2009) argued that by definition items that assess
EI should contain reference to feelings in general and references to specific emotions. As the
above procedure removed all but 1 item from the optimism scale, this factor was discarded
from further analyses. Subsequent CFA on the remaining 19 items resulted in acceptable fit
indices for the single-factor solution and good fit indices for the 5-factor solution The
multifactorial solution offered by Lane et al. (2009) holds promise as it closely resembles
The Indirect and Direct Aggression Questionnaire (I-DAQ) is a test that has been developed
to assess both indirect and direct aggression (verbal and physical) in age groups ranging from
adolescents to the elderly by Ruiz-Pamies, M et al.(2014). Each item is rated on a 5 point Likert
scale and item number 1, 5, 8, 10, 12, 14, 16, 17, 19, 22, 24 and 26 are reverse scored. It tool
assess three traits: Physical Aggression (PA), Verbal Aggression (VA) and Indirect Aggression
(IA) and is gender sensitive. Taking into account that the limitations of the existing
questionnaires are not related to the content of their items, the questionnaire was developed
50
from items drawn from different aggression questionnaires (Anguiano-Carrasco & Vigil-Colet,
2011; Buss & Durkee, 1957; Buss & Perry, 1992; Connelly, Newton, & Aarons, 2005; Infante
& Wigley, 1986; Kaukiainen et al., 2001; Lawrence, 2006; Toldos, 2005). 55 items rated by
15 expert judges with experience in personality test development were assigned into
dimensions of PA, VA, & IA. Eliminating 12 ambiguous items, 43 items were used for the
pilot study and the items with loadings lower than λ = .30 on the content factor or with complex
loadings (greater than λ = .30 on more than one content factor) were removed. Fit of the three-
factor model was tested using the semi-restricted model. The goodness of fit indexes were NFI
= .97; CFI = .98; AGFI = .97; RMRS = 0.04 and RSMEA = .078 (90% C.I. = .074 –.083),
which indicated a good fit to the proposed model taking into account the cut-off values
proposed by Hu and Bentler (1999). Appropriate congruence coefficients between the expected
and the obtained solutions were observe (threshold value of .85) (Hopwood & Donnellan,
2010). The correlations among factors ranged from .32 to .44. All the scales showed adequate
factor reliabilities. I-DAQ is also sensitive to the sex differences usually found in aggression
Passionate love scale was developed by Hatfield, E & Sprecher, S (1986). It is a 30 item
love. Each item is rated on a 9 point Likert scale. The PLS scale was specifically designed to
assess the cognitive, emotional, and behavioral components of passionate love. The cognitive
components consist of: Intrusive thinking; preoccupation with the partner; Idealization of the
other or of the relationship; Desire to know the other and be known by him/her. Emotional
components consist of: Attraction to the partner, especially sexual attraction; Positive feelings
when things go well; Negative feelings when things go awry; Longing for reciprocity—
51
passionate lovers not only love, but they want to be loved in return; Desire for complete and
permanent union; and Physiological (sexual) arousal. Finally, behavioral components consist
of Actions aimed at determining the other’s feelings; Studying the other person; Service to the
other; and Maintaining physical closeness. Hatfield and Sprecher (1986) reported a coefficient
alpha of .91 for the 15- item version and .94 for the 30-item version. Others have also reported
high levels of reliability for the scale (e.g., Sprecher & Regan, 1998). The PLS appears to be
primarily one-dimensional, with one primary factor emerging from a principal components
significant correlation between respondents’ scores on the PLS and their scores on the 1964
Crowne and Marlowe Social Desirability Scale (Hatfield & Sprecher, 1986). There is some
evidence for the construct validity of the PLS. For example, it has been found to be associated
positively with conceptually similar scales and measures (Aron & Henkemeyer, 1995; Hatfield
& Sprecher, 1986; Hendrick & Hendrick, 1989; Sprecher & Regan, 1998).
Researchers have used the PLS in exploring many different topics, including cross-cultural
differences in passionate love (Hatfield & Rapson, 2005; Hatfield, Rapson, & Martel, 2007;
Landis & O’Shea, 2000), prototype approaches to love (Fehr, 2005), neural bases of passionate
love (Aron et al., 2005; Bartels & Zeki, 2004), changes in passionate love over the family life
cycle (Tucker & Aron, 1993), correlates of sexual desire (Beck, Bozman, & Qualtrough, 1991),
the effects of an emotionally focused couples therapy (James, 2007), degree of bonding with
an abusive partner (Graham et al., 1995), and the effects of having married couples engage in
novel activities (Aron, Norman, Aron, McKenna, & Heyman, 2000). The PLS is copyright by
Hatfield and Sprecher (1986). Permission is given to all clinicians and researchers who wish
52
Procedure
After formulating the research problem, necessary permission and ethical approval was obtained
from the department. Test instruments were selected after a thorough internet search, with studies
establishing the reliability & validity and free right to use it for non-commercial use. Prior
permission was obtained from Institute of Mental Health and Institute of Speech and Hearing,
Sweekaar Academy of Rehabilitation Sciences to collect data from its members. To obtain as
representative a sample as possible, participants were chosen as per hailing from as much different
states in India as possible and from foreign students doing internship in the institute. To further
reduce bias in the sample, more than twice as much data collected from the institute was collected
from different parts of Hyderabad giving priority to participants hailing from different places of
origin/ with different mother tongue. Participants were briefed regarding the study as well as about
the nature of psychological test they have to undergo. They were assured of confidentiality of the
test results. They were also informed of their right to know the test results. Prior consent was
obtained from the participants after ensuring their free will to participate as well as no coercion or
Statistical Analysis
The data was entered into the profile sheet and was entered into the statistical software (version
21). Parametric and non-parametric tests were employed where ever appropriate. Descriptive
statistics like mean, standard deviation, variance, frequency and percentage analysis was carried
out on the socio-demographic data for deeper understanding of the sample. One-Sample
Kolmogorov – Smirnov Test was done to find out the distribution of the variables. After
ascertaining normality of distribution, Pearson’s correlation was used to determine the degree of
relationship. Regression analysis was carried out to determine the variance of predictor on criterion
53
Test and Independent Sample Kruskal-Wallis Test was further carried out to analyze the data on
*** ~~~~~~~***
54
CHAPTER - 6
RESULTS
55
CHAPTER - 6
RESULTS
The current study involved a single group (within group design) of women in the age range 18 to
40 years. Initial analysis involved the distribution of population characteristics. The mean age of
The participants in the study were from ten states in India and one woman was an internee from
56
Table 2. Distribution of Participants across Locale.
Frequency Percent
Andhra Pradesh, India 3 3.1
Assam, India 1 1.0
Chhattisgarh, India 1 1.0
Karnataka, India 5 5.2
Kerala, India 12 12.4
Maharashtra, India 1 1.0
Punjab, India 1 1.0
Rajasthan, India 2 2.1
Skane, Sweden 1 1.0
Tamil Nadu, India 1 1.0
Telengana, India 69 71.1
Total 97 100.0
Among the participants 24.7 % were late adolescents and 75.3 % were early adults. 49.5 % of them
were students, 14.4% were un-employed and 36.1% were employed. 32% of the participants were
either doing or have completed their research degree. 35.1% of the participants were married while
57
All the variables followed random sequence and normal distribution, viz. Metacognitive
Awareness (Mean = 42.20, SD = 6.04, Runs test sig .203, Kolmogorov-Sirinov test sig .454);
Emotional Intelligence (Mean = 126.72, SD = 8.26, Runs test sig .123, Kolmogorov-Sirinov test
sig .861); Aggression (Mean = 73.53, SD = 7.43, Runs test sig .284, Kolmogorov-Sirinov test sig
.879); Passionate Love (Mean = 205.73, SD = 38.51, Runs test sig .077, Kolmogorov-Sirinov test
sig .063).
58
Chart 2. Distribution of Metacognitive Awareness
59
Chart 4. Distribution of Aggression
60
The central tendencies of all the dependent variables across categorizing variables of age,
employment status, educational qualification and marital status were found to be as follows.
61
The distribution of emotional intelligence, aggression and passionate love remained the same
across age category (Mann- Whitney U Test Sig. 0.831, .739 & .404 respectively); but
metacognitive awareness did not follow same distribution across the age category (Table 6).
However the distribution of all the variables remained the same across the categories of educational
qualification (Table 7). The distribution of metacognitive awareness and passionate love did not
remain the same across married and unmarried women (Mann- Whitney U Test Sig. 0.000 & .017
respectively). But the distribution remained the same among married and unmarried women for
emotional intelligence and aggression (Mann- Whitney U Test Sig. 0.751 & .750 respectively)
(Table 8). The distribution of metacognitive awareness and passionate love did not remain the
same across categories of occupational status i.e. student, unemployed and employed women
(Kruskal- Wallis Test Sig. 0.001 & .038 respectively). Also, the distribution of emotional
intelligence and aggression remained the same across the categories of occupational status as
student, unemployed and employed women (Kruskal- Wallis Test Sig. 0.575 & .549 respectively)
(Table 9).
62
Table 6. Test of Normality and Distribution of Variables across Age Category
63
Table 8. Test of Normality and Distribution of Variables across Marital Status
64
Pearson’s correlation analysis was carried out to find the relationship between the variables.
Metacognitive awareness was found to be correlated with emotional intelligence at .01 significance
level (r= .374, sig (2 tailed) = .000) (Table 10). No other variables were found to be related to
each other.
The amount of variance in emotional intelligence (R square = .140, beta = .374, Sig = .000)
approached significant level at 95 % confidence level (Table 11). The regression equation is:
65
Table 10. Results of Correlation Analysis between Metacognitive Awareness, Emotional
Intelligence, Aggression and Passionate Love.
N 90 83 81 68
N 83 86 77 63
N 81 77 88 66
N 68 63 66 73
66
Table 11. Model Summary of Linear Regression of Metacognitive Awareness as predictor
of Emotional Intelligence.
R R Square Adjusted Std. Error Change Statistics
R Square of the R Square F Change df df Sig. F Change
Estimate Change 1 2
.374a .140 .129 7.790 .140 13.195 1 81 .000
ANOVAa
Sum of df Mean F Sig.
Squares Square
Regression 800.836 1 800.836 13.195 .000b
Residual 4916.031 81 60.692
Total 5716.867 82
a. Dependent Variable: Emotional Intelligence
b. Predictors: (Constant), Metacognitive Awareness
Correlation Coefficients
Unstandardized Standardized t Sig.
Coefficients Coefficients
B Std. Error Beta
(Constant) 104.688 6.106 17.145 .000
Metacognitive .517 .142 .374 3.633 .000
Awareness
Residuals Statistics a
N = 83 Minimum Maximum Mean SD
Predicted Value 120.72 131.58 126.65 3.125
Std. Predicted Value -1.898 1.577 .000 1.000
Standard Error of .858 1.843 1.177 .280
Predicted Value
Adjusted Predicted Value 120.52 131.87 126.66 3.125
Residual -17.028 16.558 .000 7.743
Std. Residual -2.186 2.125 .000 .994
Stud. Residual -2.215 2.139 -.001 1.005
Deleted Residual -17.487 16.773 -.013 7.911
Stud. Deleted Residual -2.271 2.189 .000 1.015
67
Chart 6. Distribution of Regression Standardized Residual of Emotional Intelligence as
predicted by Metacognition.
68
Chart 8. Scatter Plot of Regression Standardized Predicted value Vs Observed value of
Emotional Intelligence.
69
CHAPTER -7
DISCUSSION
70
CHAPTER -7
DISCUSSION
The aim of the present study was to understand the relationship between metacognition, emotional
In the present study a total of 130 participants gave consent to be part of the study. Leaving aside
carelessly filled and partially filled questionnaires, data was available from 108 participants.
Further filtering of data was carried out by eliminating data from participants whose response was
among extreme values for more than two variables. This brought down the number of actual
sample to 97 with 90 valid data for the variable metacognitive awareness, 86 for emotional
intelligence, 88 for indirect-direct aggression and 73 for passionate love. Hence the achieved
power for correlation between metacognitive awareness and emotional intelligence (effect size =
.5, sample size = 83) was 0.88 (critical z 1.96), that between metacognitive awareness and
aggression (effect size = .5, sample size = 81) was 0.88 (critical z = 1.96), that between
metacognitive awareness and passionate love (effect size = .5, sample size = 68) was 0.81, that
between emotional intelligence and aggression (effect size .5, sample size = 77) was 0.86, that
between emotional intelligence and passionate love (effect size = .5, sample size = 63) was 0.78
(critical z = 1.96) & that between aggression and passionate love (effect size = .5, sample size =
66) was 0.80 (critical z = 1.96). The mean age of the participants was 25.5 years with 73
participants in their early adulthood and 24 participants in their late adolescence. The sample had
an all India representation with participants from ten states in India as well as one foreign national
who was an internee taken from Sweekaar Academy of Rehabilitation Science. 34 of the
participants were married and 63 of the rest of the participants were single. 48 of the participants
71
were students, 35 of them were employed and 14 of them were unemployed (house wives). Among
all the participants 66 of them were either doing or had completed their research degree.
The distribution of the variables followed normal distribution (refer table 4). However the
distribution did not remain the same for all the set of variables across categorizing variables of
age, employment status and marital status. This result need to be taken with caution since the
primary aim of the sampling was not collecting data across these categories and hence the number
of data available in each category differed considerably. Also they were skewed with respect to
native place, educational category in a few subjects, and profession related to interaction with
clients in a curative setting (i.e. human caring involved viz, clinical psychology, rehabilitation,
speech and audiology, physiotherapy and nursing). Except a few, most of them had research degree
in psychology or rehabilitation. Hence any attempt at generalization of the result obtained over the
categories would be immature. However looking at the result with impartiality and with an aim to
find further hypotheses for future research would only be modest. In that view following discussion
is presented, before the main aim of the study are put to scrutiny.
The distribution of metacognitive awareness did not remain the same across age category of early
adulthood and late adolescence (refer table 6). Also the mean of metacognitive awareness was
found to be high among Early Adults (EA) in comparison with Late Adolescents (LA), (EA - Mean
= 43.61, SD = 5.824; LA - Mean = 37.57, SD = 4.214). It may be rationally inferred that the
metacognitive process continue to develop over the age as one gets more accustomed to knowledge
about their own thoughts. This has further implication in the intervention aspects in clinical
psychology that training in metacognitive skills be carried out even after one has entered into her
early adulthood and its evidence based practice may be established. Also, metacognitive therapy
may be more suitable and less time consuming for the early adults in comparison with late
adolescents among women because of their higher metacognitive awareness. The fact that
72
distribution of metacognitive awareness remained same but the mean was found to be higher
among research scholars (NR- Mean = 41.76, SD = 5.756; R- Mean = 43.18, SD = 6.634) together
with change observed over the age (early adulthood) also indicate that the intellectual involvement
over a period of time might improve metacognitive awareness among women. But the result could
also be due to the fact that those with higher metacognitive intelligence were successful in pursuing
education up to research degree, hence further research is needed to ascertain the fact. Also, most
of the research degree holders were from the field of clinical psychology or rehabilitation which
might have helped them improve their metacognitive skills as part of their studies and professional
requirements. The distribution of metacognitive awareness did not remain the same across the
category of employment (refer table 9). This, together with the fact that mean value of
metacognitive awareness is lower for Students (S) compared to Employed (E) or Unemployed
(UE) groups (E- Mean = 44.47, SD = 5.507; UE- Mean = 44.57, SD = 5.585; S- Mean = 39.80,
SD = 5.716), is showing that the effort to know about one’s own thought happen at a lesser extent
among the women students. This could be due to the casual factor that students are usually busy
with their studies rather than knowing about their cognitive processes. This suggests that
metacognitive skills of knowing one’s own thought and controlling them develop only on
conscious effort put for the same, rather than as gradual developmental process over the age. This
fact need to be read together with the finding of the study that early adults have better
metacognition in comparison with late adolescents. Again, the distribution of metacognition did
not remain the same across married and single women (refer Table 8). Also the mean of
metacognitive awareness was higher among married (M) women compared to single (S) women
(M- Mean = 45.13, SD = 4.831; S- Mean = 40.59, SD = 6.070). Inference from this result requires
further research as to whether the factors other than those already mentioned above viz, higher age
and current non engagement in studies (distraction away from being aware of one’s thoughts
73
among women students) contribute to the observed higher metacognition among married women.
One possibility could be the presence and interaction with the partner makes married women more
aware of their own thoughts and cognitive processes. Here again partner temperaments and
relationship pattern need to be considered for meaningful insight into the process. However, the
mere fact that metacognition is higher among married women makes them suitable candidates for
Distribution of emotional intelligence did not change across the category of age. Also, the mean
value of emotional intelligence remained nearly the same among early adults (EA) as well as
among late adolescents (LA) (EA- Mean = 126.87, SD = 8.021; LA- Mean = 126.21, SD = 9.265).
This is unexpected finding that age and associated experiences does not influence improvement in
emotional intelligence among women. This could be due to sampling bias discussed above. But
distribution of emotional intelligence did not change across any of the categories of education,
employment status and marital status among women. Also, the mean value remained more or less
the same (refer table 5, 6, 7, 8 & 9). Hence the chances are that emotional intelligence could be an
entity which is more or less static or remains within a range for an individual or at least for a
women. But for establishing that, appropriate sampling method may be chosen along with suitable
research design. This is despite the fact that correlation exists between metacognitive awareness
& emotional intelligence and metacognition changes across categories of age, employment status
and marital status. However, this finding is in agreement with the claim Bar-On (2000) put up, that
relatively small differences in emotional intelligence exist across the life-span. Even though we
have to consider what Roberts, Zeidner, et al. (2001) reported, that the types of investigation
carried out to examine age differences has been both meager and contradictory. Cultural aspects
in emotional intelligence also need to be addressed for a better understanding, considering the fact
that 74.2 % of the sample for the present study was from single mother tongue background and
74
many other participants were also residents of the same cultural setting for quite some time. Non-
significant change of emotional intelligence of women across the categories of age, employment
status, educational qualification and marital status should draw attention of psychotherapists in
that, affective therapy may not be a suitable intervention strategy among women.
The distribution of indirect-direct aggression also did not change across the categories of age,
education, employment status or marital status and the mean value remained more or less the same
across the categories (refer table 5, 6, 7, 8 & 9). This again maybe pointing to the fact that
aggression remains the same in a woman’s life from adolescence till end of early adulthood despite
other intervening factors like employment status, educational qualification or marital status. Again,
to establish such a finding sampling need to be done with a view to avoid cultural aspects in
expression of aggression, wide range of educational and employment category, equal and larger
number of participants in all category including marital status, partner traits and their aggression,
socio economic status etc. But the finding is similar to that reported by follow-up studies viz.
aggression in females is found to be less stable than in males (Huesmann et al., 1984; Huesmann
& Eron, 1986; Olweus, 1984; Pulkkinen, 1987), hence remains the same.
The distribution of passionate love remained the same among early adults and late adolescents
(refer table 6) but the mean value was found to be higher for early adults (EA- Mean = 208.17, SD
= 37.406; LA- Mean = 199.67, SD = 41.430). This may be assumed to be due to the fact that early
adults enter into relationship more in comparison with late adolescents in the cultural setting from
which the sample was drawn. The distribution of passionate love remained the same across the
category of education but higher central tendencies were observed among non-research (NR)
degree holders in comparison with research scholars (R) (refer table 5 & 7) (NR- Mean = 209.45,
the fact that the research scholars in the present sample was predominantly from the field of clinical
75
psychology and rehabilitation, it may be inferred that understanding the psychological process and
emotional expressions behind relationship and among people may have been a negatively
motivating factor for the women to enter into passionate love. Also majority of the research degree
holders participated in the study were unmarried, hence non commitment in the relationship
(whether existed or not) might be a crucial factor for women to go into passionate love relationship.
Moreover, as discussed above, the sampling need to be modified as the huge difference in number
is evident in the present sample (NR – 60, R – 13). The distribution of passionate love did not
remain the same across employment status (refer table 9). The mean of passionate love score was
found to be lower for the female students in comparison with employed and unemployed women
40.330). This again could be due to the fact that students were not married or not into committed
relationship in the cultural background from which the sample was taken. Also, the distribution of
passionate love did not remain the same across marital status (refer table 8) and the mean score of
passionate love was higher for married women (M- Mean = 219.04, SD = 33.533; S- Mean =
197.44, SD = 39.418). Hence from all the above results it may be inferred that passionate love
improves among women as they enter into committed relationship. Again establishing this fact
with generalizability requires further research with appropriate sampling. This is particularly
important for making any inference regarding passionate love, as most number of extreme value
elimination happened for the variable passionate love (N = 73, Missing values 24) in the present
study.
Coming to the main aim of the present study, viz to understand the relationship among
metacognition, emotional intelligence, aggression and passionate love, Pearson’s correlation was
carried out.
76
Significant correlation was found between metacognitive awareness and emotional intelligence
(r= .374, Sig. at .01 level). This is in agreement with the already published literature. Cross and
Paris (1988) note that metacognition includes affective and motivational states. Similarly,
Martinez (2006) argues that metacognition entails the management of affective states, and that
metacognitive strategies can improve persistence and motivation in the face of challenging tasks.
Paris and Winograd (1990) concur, arguing that affect is an inevitable element of metacognition,
because as students monitor and appraise their own cognition, they will become more aware of
strengths and weaknesses. Eisenberg (2010) reviews the research on young children’s emotion-
related self-regulation, which is the set of “processes used to manage and change if, when, and
how one experiences emotions and emotion-related motivation and physiological states and how
emotions are expressed behaviorally” (p. 681). This emotion-related self-regulation refers to
monitoring and regulating the impact of emotions and motivational states on one’s performance
and parallels the regulation of cognition involved in the executive functioning dimension of
metacognition. Mayer and Salovey (1997) defined emotional intelligence as the ability to perceive,
appraise, and express emotions accurately; the ability to access and generate feelings to facilitate
cognitive activities; the ability to understand emotion-relevant concepts and use emotion-relevant
language; and the ability to manage one’s own emotions and the emotions of others to promote
growth, well-being, and functional social relations. Implications of the findings are that research
may be carried out to design intervention strategies to address affective disorders through
enhancing or changing metacognition. Also, research might be carried out to design intervention
and/or emotional intelligence. Even with this modest finding it is rational to practice such
therapeutic interventions in the said situations in clinical settings with appropriate evaluation of
the client.
77
Other finding of the present study is that there is no significant relationship between metacognitive
awareness and aggression (refer table 10). This could also be rationally understood that
metacognitive awareness requires knowing about one’s own thoughts which itself serves as a
monitoring function (metacognition by definition being able to know one’s cognitive processes
and able to control it). Aggression is mostly instinctual and acted upon with suppression of
monitoring the thoughts and cognition. Even on the thought out acts of aggression or suppression
of aggression, the simultaneous monitoring of cognition and thoughts seems to be suspended for
the moment. And for those having impulsivity and which is manifested in their aggressive
behavior, it is a rare chance that monitoring of cognitive processes and control of the same is
No correlation was found between metacognition and passionate love (refer table 10). This may
also be understood in the same rationality that when one is in passionate love, that person
especially a woman will tend not to focus on or monitor her own thoughts and cognitions.
No correlation was found between emotional intelligence and aggression (refer table 10).
one’s emotions; be able to process emotional information accurately and efficiently; and have the
insight to skill fully use one’s emotions to solve problems, make plans, and achieve in one’s life
(Salovey & Mayer, 1989–1990). Hence having emotional intelligence goes against the aggressive
instinct. But then the other argument will be why it is not negatively correlated. The answer lies in
the same definition. When one is being able to process emotional information accurately and
efficiently; and have the insight to skill fully use one’s emotions to solve problems, make plans,
and achieve in one’s life, the aggression element need not always be left out. According to Schwarz
& Bless, (1991) a careful, analytical approach that avoids risk taking may often be most
appropriate in times of threat or stress. Negative emotions occur when something has gone wrong
78
or threatens to go wrong. They occur when there is a problem, when goals are at risk, or when
resources seem inadequate. Negative emotions have the potential for functional utility because
there are times when the effects of negative emotions are useful. Recognizing this potential for
benefit is the key to understanding why it is neither odd nor self-contradictory to claim that
negative emotions can be useful or even desirable. For example, anger involves a readiness to be
confrontational and antagonistic, and to the extent that confrontation and antagonism are adaptive
under conditions that induce anger, this readiness will facilitate adaptive behavior.
No correlation was found between emotional intelligence and passionate love (refer table 10).
According to Brown J.W., (2012) love is the ultimate valuation of a thing, to which all desires are
subordinate. There is nothing more important than what is loved, which is to say that love,
from the self-preservative drives, a genuine love for a person, for a thing or an occasion, from the
perspective of the individual, announces an object of the greatest possible value. From this stand
point we can understand that when a women is in passionate love, the person tends to sub serve
everything the emotional intelligence will naturally would have modulated, in the pursuit of the
ultimate valuation.
No correlation was found between aggression and passionate love (refer table 10). According to
Brown J. W (2012) aggression is the core of what is derived to the self-assertiveness of want,
including a variety of egocentric feelings—greed, arrogance, hate, pride, contumely—in which the
satisfaction of the will at the expense of the other is centered primarily in the acquisition of goods
or the protection of goods possessed. There is immediate discharge into pre-objects as drive-energy
fills the act completely and unselfconsciously. This drive is in essence going against the ultimate
valuation expressed in love. Hence a non-correlation between the variables is a rational finding.
79
Linear regression analysis was done to find the predictive ability of metacognitive awareness on
emotional intelligence. The amount of variance in emotional intelligence (R square = .140, beta =
.374, Sig = .000) was significant level at 95 % confidence level (Table 11). The R square value is
14% which suggest that other factors also contributing to the dependent variable.
For the present study, sample was collected from women aged 18 to 40 who can speak English.
This is a poor reflection of the women population the study was trying to explore. Including a
larger age range with women in their late adulthood might give a different result. Stratified
sampling or quota sampling representing percentage of aged women in each category may give
better results with generalizability. Also, majority of the data collected was from those either
physiotherapy. Nearly 75% of the sample obtained was from one linguistic group. All these factors
have compromised the representative nature of sample and the statistic may well me differing from
the parameters. Also, the number of sample varied drastically based on the categorizing variables
even though primary aim of the sampling for the present study was not to make comparison based
on scores across categories. Hence the inference made through non parametric test on categorizing
variables may not be generalizable given the fact that it was skewed in number, educational
qualification, marital status and employment status from profession involving human caring. The
planned sample size needed for generalizability with 5% error bound on estimate was 107.
However after eliminating the extreme values and erroneously filled up questionnaires, total
sample size came down to 97 with valid data for metacognition being 90, that for emotional
intelligence being 86, that for aggression being 88 and that for passionate love being 73. This
eventually reduced the available number of data for correlation between metacognitive awareness
and emotional intelligence to 83, that between metacognition and aggression to 81, that between
80
metacognition and passionate love to 68, that between emotional intelligence and aggression to
77, that between emotional intelligence and passionate love to 63 and that between aggression and
passionate love to 66. Hence the power achieved for correlation between metacognitive awareness
and emotional intelligence came down to .88, that between metacognition and aggression to .88,
that between metacognition and passionate love to .81, that between emotional intelligence and
aggression to .86, that between emotional intelligence and passionate love to .78 and that between
aggression and passionate love to .8 at the effect size of 0.5. Sperling et al (2004) utilizing the MAI
to determine college student metacognitive awareness, found a significant correlation between the
knowledge of cognition factor and the regulation of cognition factor. Even though previous
available research established correlation with metacognition and metacognitive awareness and
hence metacognitive awareness was selected as the representative variable for metacognition, the
actual correlation between metacognition and other variables of emotional intelligence, aggression
and passionate love might be different from the correlation between metacognitive awareness and
the said variables. The inference drawn regarding intervention strategies based on statistics would
have been more meaningful if a tool for measuring metacognition (involving control of cognition
and executive functions) was used rather than that measuring a component of it namely
metacognitive awareness. Similar logic holds for using a scale for passionate love in lieu of
expressive love or ability to love or attitude towards love for representing the broader concept of
love. Then the correlation effect would have had further meaningful insight into the intervention
strategies. Understanding of semantics of the test items play a crucial role especially when it is a
rating scale. Since the questionnaires used for the assessment were in English and the participant’s
mother tongue was not English, this might have affected the scoring to a certain extent. Application
of statistical tools without removing outliers and extreme scores revealed that the distribution of
scores for the variable passionate love did not follow normal distribution. Also elimination of most
81
number of extreme values (24) happened for the variable of passionate love and for passionate
love most number of people left the questionnaire unfilled, partially filled or erroneously filled as
per the given direction. Cultural factor might have also played a role in giving information
regarding passionate love. Hence applying parametric tests for finding the correlation with a
possibility of the distribution in actual population not following normal distribution, can give
erroneous or non-robust results. Also, it to be noted that with passionate love scores not following
normal distribution (ie, with extreme scores and outliers included), application of non-parametric
statistics revealed correlation between metacognitive awareness and passionate love (spearman’s
rho = .231, significant at .05 level). Hence larger sampling across cultures need to be taken before
inference can be drawn about the relationship between the variables of metacognitive awareness
Metacognitive Awareness Inventory (MAI) was the instrument used to measure participant’s
because it is often implicit and, therefore, not directly observable (Flavell, 1979; Schraw, 2000;
Tobias & Everson, 2009). Attempts to quantify metacognition and assess it quickly and efficiently
led to the development of the MAI (Schraw & Dennison, 1994). One limitation of the MAI is that
the participants must be aware of their cognition to respond appropriately to the 52 items. Reliance
on the participants to assess their own thinking can lead to inaccuracies in responses. Also, the
MAI factor analysis conducted by Schraw and Dennison (1994) revealed that the items factored
on the two categories of metacognition, knowledge and regulation of cognition, but did not factor
questionable. In previous studies, the scores for knowledge of cognition and regulation of
cognition were positively correlated in the moderate to high range (Schraw & Dennison, 1994;
82
Sperling, Howard, Staley, & DuBois, 2004). To what extent metacognition of the participants were
Conclusion
The study convincingly and conclusively established the fact that there exist a significant
relationship between metacognitive awareness and emotional intelligence among women. The
other findings of the study are; there is no significant relationship between metacognitive
awareness and aggression, between metacognitive awareness and passionate love, between
emotional intelligence and aggression, between emotional intelligence and passionate love &
between aggression and passionate love. Present study revealed that scores of metacognitive
awareness, emotional intelligence, indirect-direct aggression and passionate love follows normal
distribution with mean and standard distribution as follows. Metacognitive awareness (Mean =
42.20, SD = 6.04), Emotional Intelligence (Mean = 126.72. SD = 8.26), Aggression (Mean = 73.53.
SD = 7.43) and Passionate love (Mean = 205.73, SD = 38.51). The study also revealed that
Future Research
The present study was to find the relationship between metacognition, emotional
intelligence, aggression and passionate love among women. The study may be extended
interventions strategies.
The research may be repeated to find the difference across gender so as to find out how the
relationship among the variables varies and to find out what specific factors could be
contributing to the same. This will give leverage in choosing therapeutic strategies across
gender.
83
The present study gives direction for future research studies to find out how intervening
variables like age, education, social status, employment status, marital status, cultural
background etc affect the relationship between the variables of metacognition, emotional
The study need to be carried out to find out how personality traits affects the relationship
However R square value was only 14% suggesting influence of other variables. Also,
present study rejects any correlation between emotional intelligence and aggression as well
as passionate love. Hence further research is needed to find out what other factors along
intelligence in women. This research can be repeated for men as well as including both the
genders.
Also, research may be carried out to establish suitability of therapy viz metacognitive,
affective or supportive psychotherapy for affective disorders among women based on their
the same during clinical interview can be carried out resulting in time saving as well as in
choice of suitable intervention strategy. This may be repeated across genders and including
both genders.
awareness and emotional intelligence and the predictive ability of metacognitive awareness
84
Present study established significant relationship between metacognitive awareness and
emotional intelligence among women. Further research may be carried out to find the
debugging strategies and evaluation. This will give meaningful insight into the
Research may be carried out to establish the efficacy of affective therapy among women,
as the extended analysis of the available data from the present study indicated that
distribution of emotional intelligence remains the same among women across the
categories of age, educational qualification, employment status and marital status with
For the present study exclusion criteria included current or previous history of mental
illness, disability, substance abuse and sever medical complications. Research may be
aggression and love across all the ICD diagnostic categories. This will give insight into the
fact that how different categories of mental illness affect these variables as well as
relationship among them. This will also give meaningful insight into which therapies can
be best chosen for different diagnostic categories. The research may be done among men,
therapies across age categories of adolescence, early adulthood, middle adulthood, late
85
CHAPTER -8
SUMMARY
86
CHAPTER -8
SUMMARY
Study in Retrospect
The study is entitled “Metacognition, Emotional Intelligence, Aggression and Passionate Love
among Women”. The aim of the study is to understand the relationship between metacognition,
emotional intelligence, aggression and passionate love among female population and to establish
statistically valid relationship among those variables. The hypotheses of the study are
2. There will be significant relationship between metacognitive awareness and aggression among
women.
3. There will be significant relationship between metacognitive awareness and passionate love
among women.
4. There will be significant relationship between emotional intelligence and aggression among
women.
5. There will be significant relationship between emotional intelligence and passionate love
among women.
6. There will be significant relationship between aggression and passionate love among women.
Purposive sampling was used for the study. After eliminating extreme responses and erroneous
responses, the study consisted of 97 participants with informed consent. Metacognitive awareness
inventory developed by Gregory Schraw and Rayne Sperling Dennison (1994), Emotional
intelligence scale developed by Schutte et al. (1998), Indirect and direct aggression questionnaire
Hatfield, E & Sprecher, S (1986) are used for the present study. The statistical techniques used in
87
the study include mean, standard deviation, median, percentage analysis, Kolmogorv-Smirinov
test, Mann-Whitney U test, Kruskal-Wallis test, Pearson’s correlation and linear regression
analysis.
among women.
among women.
women.
among women.
7. There is no significant relationship between aggression and passionate love among women.
Tenability of Hypotheses
Tenability of the hypothesis may be tested on the basis of results from the data collected.
among women.
88
Findings of the study fully substantiate this hypothesis. Pearson’s correlation coefficient
obtained for the correlation between metacognitive awareness and emotional intelligence is
There is significant relationship between metacognitive awareness and aggression among women.
Findings of the study reject this hypothesis. Pearson’s correlation coefficient obtained for the
significance (r = .113).
There is significant relationship between metacognitive awareness and passionate love among
women.
Findings of the study reject this hypothesis. Pearson’s correlation coefficient obtained for the
correlation between metacognitive awareness and passionate love is non-significant at .01 level of
significance (r = .154).
There is significant relationship between emotional intelligence and aggression among women.
Findings of the study reject this hypothesis. Pearson’s correlation coefficient obtained for the
significance (r = -.014).
There is significant relationship between emotional intelligence and passionate love among
women.
89
Findings of the study reject this hypothesis. Pearson’s correlation coefficient obtained for the
correlation between emotional intelligence and passionate love is non-significant at .01 level of
significance (r = .037).
There is significant relationship between aggression and passionate love among women. Findings
of the study reject this hypothesis. Pearson’s correlation coefficient obtained for the correlation
between aggression and passionate love is non-significant at .01 level of significance (r = .076).
skills will have a positive effect on ability to evaluate one’s emotions accurately and plan
effectively to achieve in life, as the person is able to monitor her cognitive process. Hence
2. Also, improving skills to monitor one’s emotions is bound to have enhancing effect on the
3. Findings of the study reject any significant relationship between metacognition and
aggression among women. Hence to address disorders involving element of aggression viz
impulse control disorders, personality disorders, relationship and systemic problems, effect
research may be carried out to establish this fact and through comparative study behavioral
90
or other appropriate intervention strategies might be suggested as evidence based practice.
Also, research might be carried out to establish the fact in general population.
4. And research can be done to establish what other factors will be able to contain aggressive
drive within healthy limits among normal population as a preventive strategy against
mental ill health, considering the fact that one’s ability to know and monitor one’s own
cognitive process not necessarily be effective in reducing aggressive drive among women.
5. Findings of the study reject any significant relationship between metacognition and
passionate love among women. Hence to address relationship problems and systemic
problems where lack of love among the members is the predisposing factor or the root
Further research may be carried out to establish this fact and appropriate intervention
strategies that will equip the person to develop love might be devised.
6. Research can be done to establish what other factors influence the expression of love
considering the fact that one’s ability to know and monitor one’s own cognitive process
not necessarily result in expression of passionate love among women. And whether pursuit
of love help people achieve self-actualization, given the fact that knowing about one’s own
cognitive process and controlling them doesn’t influence that person’s love, with the
assumption that knowing and controlling one’s own cognitive process if not necessarily
7. Findings of the study reject any significant relationship between emotional intelligence and
aggression among women. Hence to address disorders involving element of aggression viz
impulse control disorders, personality disorders, relationship and systemic problems, effect
might be questionable. Further research may be carried out to establish this fact and through
91
comparative study, behavioral or other appropriate intervention strategies might be
suggested as evidence based practice. Also, research might be carried out to establish this
8. Research may be done to establish what other factor will be able to contain aggressive drive
within healthy limits among normal population as a preventive strategy against mental ill
health, considering the fact that one’s ability to know and monitor one’s own emotions
9. Findings of the study reject any significant relationship between emotional intelligence and
passionate love among women. Hence to address relationship and systemic problems
where lack of love among the members is the predisposing factor or root cause, effect of
questionable. Also, it follows form the results that expression of love or ability to be in
passionate love need not be focused during the intervention strategy to address affective
disorders. Further research may be carried out to establish this fact and appropriate
intervention strategies that will equip the person to develop love might be devised, given
the fact that being able to monitor and control one’s emotions need not necessarily
10. Research can be done to establish what other factors influence the expression of love
considering the fact that one’s ability to know and monitor one’s own emotions need not
necessarily result in expression of passionate love among women. And whether pursuit of
love help people achieve self-actualization given the fact that knowing about one’s own
emotions and controlling them doesn’t influence that person’s love, with the assumption
that knowing and controlling one’s own emotions if not necessarily help one achieve self-
actualization but at least enhance the process. Other assumption behind the suggestion
92
being that the ability to be in passionate love motivates the people to undertake actions
beyond daily activities and even towards self-actualization which might need to be
11. Findings of the study reject any significant relationship between aggression and passionate
love among women. Hence to address relationship and systemic problems where lack of
love among the members is the predisposing factor or the root cause, taming aggressive
drive need not be taken as the prime focus to solve such problems. Also, it follows form
the results that expression of love or ability to be in passionate love need not be focused
during the intervention strategy to address disorders involving element of aggression viz
Further research may be carried out to establish this fact and appropriate intervention
strategies that will equip the person to develop love might be devised, given the fact that
aggressive drive need not necessarily influence that person’s ability to express love or be
passionately in love.
12. Research can be done to establish what other factor influence the expression of love
considering the fact that aggressive drive need not necessarily hinder the expression of
passionate love among women. Further research may be carried out looking at possible
emotional intelligence among women. Hence any tool employed to assess metacognitive
awareness during the clinical interview may discriminate sufficiently as whether to use
93
disorders among women. This might bring down the time spent on therapeutic process and
might aid to move towards brief therapy modes in the said intervention strategies.
14. From the findings of the present study, it is modest to suggest research aimed at integrating
*** ~~~~~~~***
94
REFERENCES
95
References
Anguiano-Carrasco C., & Vigil-Colet A. (2011). Assessing indirect aggression in aggressors and
targets: Spanish adaptation of the Indirect Aggression Scales. Psicothema, 23, 146–152.
Archer J, Coyne SM. 2005. An integrated review of indirect, relational, and social aggression.
Personality and Social Psychology Review 9: 212–230
Aron, A., Fisher, H., Mashek, D. J., Strong, G., Li, H., & Brown, L. L. (2005). Reward, motivation,
and emotion systems associated with early-stage intense romantic love. Journal of
Neurophysiology, 94, 327-337.
Aron, A., & Henkemeyer, L. (1995). Marital satisfaction and passionate love. Journal of Social
and Personal Relationships, 12, 139-146.
Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bar-On, R. (1997). The Emotional Intelligence Inventory (EQ-i): Technical manual. Toronto:
Multi-Health Systems.
Bar-On, R. (2000). Emotional and social intelligence: Insights from the Emotional Quotient
Inventory. In R. Bar-On & J. D. A. Parker (eds.), The handbook of emotional intelligence (pp.
363–388). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Bartels, A. & Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. Neuroimage,
21, 1155-1166.
Bechara, A., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Poor judgment in spite of high intellect:
Neurological evidence for emotional intelligence. In R. Bar-On & J. D. A. Parker (eds.), Handbook
of Emotional Intelligence. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
96
Beck, J. G., Bozman, A. W., & Qualtrough, T. (1991). The experience of sexual desire:
Psychological correlates in a college sample. The Journal of Sex Research, 28, 443-456.
Biederman, J., Newcorn, J., & Sprich, S. (1991). Comorbidity of attention deficit hyperactivity
disorder with conduct, depressive, anxiety, and other disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry,
148, 564–577.
Boldizar, J. P., Perry, D. G., & Perry, L. C. (1989). Outcome values and aggression. Child
Development, 60, 571 – 579
Brown, J. W. (2005). Process and the Authentic Life. Heusenstamm: Ontos Verlag.
Brown, J.W. (2012). Love and other emotions. On the process of feeling. London: Karnac Books
Ltd
Brown, A. L., Campione, J. C., & Barclay, C. R. (1979). Training and self-checking routines for
estimating test readiness: Generalization from list learning to prose recall. Child Development, 50,
501-512.
Buss, A. (1963). Physical aggression in relation to different frustrations. Journal of Abnormal and
Social Psychology, 67, 1-7.
Buss A. H., & Durkee A. (1957). An inventory for assessing different kinds of hostility. Journal
of Consulting Psychology, 21, 343–349. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0046900.
Buss A. H., & Perry M. (1992).The Aggression Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 63, 452–459. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.63.3.452.
Carson, A. J., Ringbauer, B., MacKenzie, L., Warlow, C., & Sharpe, M. (2000). Neurological
disease, emotional disorder, and disability—they are related: A study of 300 consecutive new
referrals to a neurology outpatient department. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry,
68, 202–206.
Ciarrochi, J., Chan, A., & Bajgar, J. (2001). Measuring emotional intelligence in adolescents.
Personality & Individual Differences, 31, 1105–1119.
Ciarrochi, J., Chan, A., & Bajgar, J. (2001). Measuring emotional intelligence in adolescents.
Personality & Individual Differences, 31, 1105–1119.
97
Ciarrochi, J., Chan, A., & Caputi, P. (2000). A critical evaluation of the emotional intelligence
construct. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 539–561.
Cohen, R. (1955). Social norms, arbitrariness of frustration and status of the agent of frustration in
the frustration-aggression hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 222-226.
Connelly C. D., Newton R. R., & Aarons G. A. (2005). A psychometric examination of English
and Spanish versions of the revised Conflict Tactics Scales. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 20,
1560–1579. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0886260505280341.
Costa, P. T., Jr. & McCrae, R. R. (1985). Hypochondriasis, neuroticism, and aging: When are
somatic complaints unfounded? American Psychologist, 40, 19–28.
Cox, B. J., Borger, S. C., Asmundson, G. J. G., & Taylor, S. (2000). Dimensions of
hypochondriasis and the five-factor model of personality. Personality & Individual Differences,
29, 99–108.
Damasio, A. R. (1994). The brain binds entities and events by multiregional activation from
convergence zones. In H. Gutfreund, G. Toulouse, et al. (eds.), Biology and computation: A
physicist’s choice (pp. 749–758). Singapore: World Scientific Publishing Co.
Deary, I. J., Wilson, J. A., & Kelly, S. W. (1995). Globus pharyngis, personality, and psychological
distress in the general population. Psychosomatics, 36, 570–577.
Doherty, O., & Matthews, G. (1988). Personality characteristics of opiate addicts. Personality and
Individual Differences, 9, 171–172.
Dollard, J., Doob, L. W., Miller, N. E., Mowrer, 0. H., & Sears, R. R. (1939). Frustration and
aggression. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Endler, N., Denisoff, E., & Rutherford, A. (1998). Anxiety and depression: Evidence for the
differentiation of commonly co-occurring constructs. Journal of Psychopathology & Behavioral
Assessment, 20, 149–171.
Endler, N., & Parker, J. (1990). Multidimensional assessment of coping: A critical review. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 844–854.
98
Eron, L. D. (1982). Parent-child interaction, television violence and aggression of children.
American Psychologist, 42, 435-442.
Eron, L. D. (1987). The development of aggressive behavior from the perspective of a developing
behaviorism. American Psychologist, 42, 435-442.
Farmer, A., & McGuffin, P. (1999). Comparing ICD-10 and DSM-IV. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 175, 587–588.
Fehr, B. (2005). Prototype-based assessment of laypeople’s views of love. Personal Relationships,
1, 309-331.
Feingold, A. (1994). Gender differences in personality: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,
116, 429–456.
Freud, S. (1957). Instincts and their vicissitudes. Standard Edition (Vol. 14, pp. 109-140).
London: Hogarth Press. (Original work published 1915).
Freeman, J. (1993). Parents and families in nurturing giftedness and talent. In K. A. Heller, F. J.
Mönks, & A. H. Passow (Eds.), International handbook of research and development of giftedness
and talent (pp. 669-684). Oxford: Pergamon.
Frodi, A., Macaulay, J., & Thome, P. R. (1977). Are women always less aggressive than men? A
review of the experimental literature. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 634-660.
Frost, W. D., & Averill, T. R. (1982). Difference between men and women in the everyday
experience of anger. In: J. R. Averill (Ed.), Anger and aggression: An essay one motion. (pp. 281-
316). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Gignac, G.E., Palmer, B.R., Manocha, R. and Stough, C. (2005) An examination of the factor
structure of the Schutte self-report emotional intelligence (SSREI) scale via confirmatory factor
analysis. Personality and Individual Differences 39, 1029-1042.
99
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional Intelligence: Why it can matter more than IQ. New York: Bantam
Books.
Goldner, E. M., Srikameswaran, S., Schroeder, M. L., Livesley, W. J., & Birmingham, C. L.
(1999). Dimensional assessment of personality pathology in patients with eating disorders.
Psychiatry Research, 85, 151–159.
Graham, D. L., Rawlings, E. I., Ihms, K., Latimer, D., Foliano, J., Thompson, A., Suttman, K.,
Farrington, M., & Hacker, R. (1995). A scale for identifying “Stockholm syndrome” reactions in
young dating women: factor structure, reliability, and validity. Violence and Victims, 10, 3-22.
Green, J., Gilchrist, A., Burton, D., & Cox, A. (2000). Social and psychiatric functioning in
adolescents with Asperger syndrome compared with conduct disorder. Journal of Autism &
Developmental Disorders, 30, 279–293.
Hatfield, E., & Rapson, R. L. (2005). Love and Sex: Cross-Cultural Perspectives. Lanham, MD:
University Press of America
Hatfield, E, Rapson, R. L., & Martel, L. D. (2007). Passionate love and sexual desire. In Shinobu
Kitayama & Dov Cohen. (Eds.) Handbook of cultural psychology. New York: Guilford Press, pp.
760-779
Hatfield, E., & Sprecher, S. (1986). Measuring passionate love in intimate relations. Journal of
Adolescence, 9, 383-410.
Helms-Lorenz, M., van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Poortinga, Y. H. (2003). Cross-cultural differences
in cognitive performance and Spearman's hypothesis: G or c? Intelligence, 31, 9-29.
Hendrick, C., & Hendrick, S. S. (1989). Research on love: Does it measure up? Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 784-794.
Hopwood C. J., & Donnellan M. B. (2010). How should the internal structure of personality
inventories be evaluated? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 332–346.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1088868310361240.
Hu L., & Bentler P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indices in covariance structure analysis:
Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.
100
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118.
Huesmann, L. R. (1977). Formal models of social behavior. Paper presented at the meeting of the
Society for Experimental Social Psychology, Austin, Texas.
Huesmann, L. R. (1982). Television violence and aggressive behavior. In D. Pearl, L. Bouthilet &
P. Lazar (Eds. ), Television and behavior: Ten years of scientific programs and implications for
the SO's. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Huesmann, L. R., & Eron, L. D. (1984). Cognitive processes and the persistence of aggressive
behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 10, 243-251.
Huesmann, L. R., Eron, L. D., Lefkowitz, M. M., & Walder, L. 0. (1984). The stability of
aggression over time and generations. Developmental Psychology, 20, 1120-1134.
Infante D. A., & Wigley C. J. (1986). Verbal aggressiveness: An interpersonal model and measure.
Communication Monographs, 53, 61–69. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03637758609376126.
Jacobs, J. E., & Paris, S. G. (1987). Children’s metacognition about reading: Issues in definition,
measurement, and instruction. Educational Psychologist, 22(3&4), 255-278.
Jensen, A. R. (1985). The nature of the black-white difference on various psychometric tests:
Spearman’s hypothesis. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 193-263.
Kaukiainen A., Salmivalli C., Björkqvist K., Österman K., Lahtinen A., Kostamo A., &
Lagerspeltz K. (2001). Overt and covert aggression in work settings in relation to the subjective
well-being of employees. Aggressive Behavior, 27, 360–371.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.1021.abs.
Kennedy, S. H., Dickens, S. E., Eisfeld, B. S., & Bagby, R. M. (1999). Sexual dysfunction before
antidepressant therapy in major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 56, 201–208.
101
Ketelaar, T., & Clore, G. L. (1997). Emotion and reason: The proximate effects and ultimate
functions of emotions. In G. Matthews (ed.), Cognitive science perspectives on personality and
emotion (pp. 355–396). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Kim, J. A., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S. E., Streiner, D. L., & Wilson, F. J. (2000). The prevalence of
anxiety and mood problems among children with autism and Asperger syndrome. Autism, 4, 117–
132.
Kirmayer, L. J., Robbins, J. M., & Paris, J. (1994). Somatoform disorders: Personality and the
social matrix of somatic distress. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 125–136.
Kroenke, K., Jackson, J. L., & Chamberlin, J. (1997). Depressive anxiety disorders in patients
presenting with physical complaints: Clinical predictors and outcome. American Journal of
Medicine, 103, 339–347.
Lambert, W. E. (1985). What happens to children when they act aggressively in six cultures?
In: F. R. Brush & J. B. Overmeier (Eds.), Affect, conditioning, and cognition: Essays on the
determinants of behavior (pp. 325-339). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Landis, D. & O’Shea (2000). Cross-cultural aspects of passionate love; An individual difference
analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31, 754- 779.
Lane, A.M., Meyer, B.B., Devonport, T.J., Kevin, A.D., Thelwell, R., Gill, G.S., Diehl, C.D.P.,
Wilson, M. and Weston, N. (2009) Validity of the emotional intelligence scale for use in sport.
Journal of Sports Science and Medicine 8, 289-295.
Leowald, H. (2000). The Essential Leowald. College Park, MD: University of Maryland
Publishing.
Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford
University Press.
Mackintosh, N. J. (1998). IQ and human intelligence. New York: Oxford University Press.
102
Malpass, J. R., O’Neil, J. H. F., & Hocevar, D. (1999). Self-regulation, goal orientation, self-
efficacy, worry, and high-stakes math achievement for mathematically gifted high school students.
Roeper Review, 21(4), 281-289.
Martin, E. D., & Sher, K. J. (1994). Family history of alcoholism, alcohol use disorders, and the
five-factor model of personality. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 55, 81–90.
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2000). Competing models of emotional intelligence.
In R. J. Sternberg (ed.), Handbook of human intelligence (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., Caruso, D. R., & Sitarenios, G. (2003). Measuring emotional intelligence
with the MSCEIT V2.0. Emotion, 3(1), 97–105
Mevarech, Z. R., & Kramarski, B. (1997). IMPROVE: A multidimensional method for teaching
mathematics in heterogeneous classrooms. American Educational Research Journal, 34(2), pp.
365-395.
Mascie-Taylor, C. G., & Gibson, J. B. (1978). Social mobility and IQ components. Journal of
Biosocial Science, 10, 263–276.
Miller, N. E. (1941). The frustration-aggression hypothesis. Psychological Review, 48, 337- 342.
Parke, R. D., & Slaby, R. G. (1983). The development of aggression. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.),
Handbook of child psychology (4th ed., pp. 547-642). New York: Wiley.
Mineka, S., Watson, D., & Clark, A. (1998). Comorbidity of anxiety and unipolar mood disorders.
Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 377–412.
Minnaert, A., & Janssen, P. J. (1999). The additive effect of regulatory activities on top of
intelligence in relation to academic performance in higher education. Learning and Instruction, 9,
77-91.
Mitchell, S.A. (1993) ‘Aggression and the endangered self ’, Psychoanalytic Quarterly 62(3): 351-
82.
Morales-Vives F., & Vigil-Colet A. (2010). Are there sex differences in physical aggression in the
elderly? Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 659–662.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.034.
Moyer, K. E. (1977). The psychobiology of aggression. New York: Harper & Row.
103
Nelson, T.O. and Narens, L. (1990). Meta-memory: a theoretical framework and new findings.
In Bower, G.H., Ed., The psychology of learning and motivation (pp.125–173). New York, NY:
Academic Press.
Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. (1996). The communicative theory of emotions: Empirical tests,
mental models, and implications for social interaction. In L. L.Martin & A. Tesser (eds.), Striving
and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation (pp. 363–393). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Perleth, C. (1992). Strategy use and metamemory in gifted and average primary school children.
In K. A. Heller, & E. A. Hany (Eds), Competence and responsibility, the third European
conference of the European council for high ability (pp. 46-52). Seattle: Hogrefe & Huber.
Perry, D. G., Perry, L. C., & Rasmussen, P. (1986). Cognitive social learning mediators of
aggression. Child Development, 57, 700-711.
Petrides, K.V. and Furnham, A. (2000) On the dimensional structure of emotional intelligence.
Personality and Individual Differences 29, 313-320.
Pfeiffer, S. I. (2001). Emotional intelligence: A popular but elusive construct. Report Review, 23,
138-142.
Rauste-von Wright, M. (1989). Physical and verbal aggression in peer groups among Finnish
adolescent boys and girls. International journal of Behavioral Development, 12, 473- 484.
Riskind, J. H., Gessner, T. D., & Wolzon, R. (1999). Cognitive vulnerability to alcohol dependence
in an inpatient sample: Implications of the looming maladaptive style. Unpublished Manuscript.
Roberts, R. D., Zeidner, M., & Matthews, G. (2001). Does emotional intelligence meet traditional
standards for an intelligence? Some new data and conclusions. Emotions, 1, 196–231.
104
Rolls, E. T. (1999). The brain and emotion. New York: Oxford University Press. Rowe, H. A. H.
(1991). Observing thinking and learning processes. In G. Evans (Ed.) Learning teaching cognitive
skills (pp. 9-26). Melbourne: A.C.E.R.
Ruiz-Pamies, M., Lorenzo-Seva, U., Morales-Vives, F., Cosi, S., & Vigil-Colet, A. (2014). I-
DAQ: A new test to assess direct and indirect aggression free of response bias. The Spanish
Journal of Psychology, 17, 1-8.
Saklofske, D.H., Austin, E.J. and Minski, P.S. (2003) Factor structure and validity of a trait
emotional intelligence measure. Personality and Individual Differences 34, 707–721.
Salovey, P., Bedell, B. T., Detweiler, J. B., & Mayer, J. D. (1999). Coping intelligently: Emotional
intelligence and the coping process. In C. R. Snyder (ed.), Coping: The psychology of what works
(pp. 141–164). New York: Oxford University Press.
Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional Intelligence. Imagination, Cognition and
Personality, 9, 185-211.
Salovey, P., Stroud, L. R., Woolery, A., & Epel, E. S. (under review). Perceived emotional
intelligence, stress reactivity, and health: Further explorations using the trait meta-mood scale.
Psychology and Health. In Press
Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. (1996). The domain of supervisory processes and the temporal
organization of behaviour. In A. C. Roberts & T. W. Robbins (eds.), The prefrontal cortex:
Executive and cognitive functions (pp. 22–35). New York: Oxford University Press.
Shaw, G. K., Waller, S., Latham, C. J., Dunn, G., & Thomson, A. D. (1997). Alcoholism: A long-
term follow-up study of participants in an alcohol treatment programme. Alcohol & Alcoholism,
32, 527–535.
Shraw, G., & Graham, T. (1997). Helping Gifted students Develop Metacognitive Awareness.
Roeper Review, 20(1), 4-8.
105
Sperling, R. A., Howard, B. C., Staley, R. & DuBois, N. (2004). Metacognition and self- regulated
learning constructs. Educational Research and Evaluation, 10(2), 117-139.
doi:10.1076/edre.10.2.117.27905
Sprecher, S., & Regan, P. C. (1998). Passionate and companionate love in courting and young
married couples. Sociological Inquiry, 68, 163-185
Steer, R. A., Clark, D. A., Beck, A. T., & Ranieri, W. F. (1995). Common and specific dimensions
of self-reported anxiety and depression: A replication. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 542–
545.
Sternberg, R. J. (1994). Intelligence. In R. J Sternberg (Ed.), Thinking and problem solving (pp.
263-288). San Diego: Academic Press, Inc.
Terman, L. M., & Merrill, M. A. (1937). Measuring intelligence: A guide to the administration of
the new revised Stanford-Binet tests of intelligence. New York: Houghton Mifflin.
Thompson, B. and Daniel, L.G. (1996) Factor analytic evidence for the construct validity of scores:
A historical overview and some guidelines. Educational and Psychological Measurement 56, 197-
208.
Tobias, S., & Everson, H. T. (2009). Metacognition and children’s writing. In D. J. Hacker, J.
Dunlosky, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Metacognition in educational theory and practice (pp. 107-
126). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Toldos M. P. (2005). Sex and age differences in self-estimated physical, verbal and indirect
aggression in Spanish adolescents. Aggressive Behavior, 31, 13–23.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ab.20034.
Tucker, P., & Aron, A. (1993). Passionate love and marital satisfaction at key transition points in
the family life cycle. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 12, 135-147.
Van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Poortinga, Y. H. (1997). Towards an integrated analysis of bias in cross-
cultural assessment. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 13(1), 29-37.
Van Hout-Wolters, B. (2000). Assessing active self-directed learning. In R. Simons, J. van der
Linden, & T. Duffy (Eds.), New learning (pp. 83–101). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Veenman, M.V.J., Elshout, J.J. and Meijer, J. (1997). The generality vs. domain-specificity of
106
metacognitive skills in novice learning across domains. Learning Instruction, 7, 187–209.
Veenman, M. V. J., Kerseboom, L., & Imthorn, C. (2000). Test anxiety and metacognitive
skillfulness: Availability versus production deficiencies. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 13,
391-412.
Veenman, M. V. J., Kok, R., & Blöte, A. W. (2005). The relation between intellectual and
metacognitive skills in early adolescence. Instructional Science, 33, 193-211.
Veenman, M. V. J., & Spaans, M. A. (2005). Relation between intellectual and metacognitive
skills: Age and task differences. Learning and Individual Differences, 15,159-176.
Veenman, M. V. J., Wilhelm, P., & Beishuiszen, J. J. (2004). The relation between intellectual and
metacognitive skills from a developmental perspective. Learning and Instruction, 14, 89-109.
Veenman, M. V. J., Wilhelm, P., & Beishuizen, J. J. (2004). The relation between intellectual
and metacognitive skills from a developmental perspective. Learning and Instruction, 14, 89-109.
Verschaffel, L., De Corte, E., Lasure, S., Van Vaerenberg, G. Bogaerts, H., Ratinckx, E.
(1999). Learning to solve mathematical application problems: A design experiment with fifth
graders. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 1(3), 195-229.
Waller, J. H. (1971). Differential reproduction: Its relation to IQ test score, education, and
occupation. Social Biology, 18, 122–136.
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1997). Measurement and mis-measurement of mood: Recurrent and
emergent issues. Journal of Personality Assessment, 68, 267–296.
Wells, A., & Matthews, G. (1994). Attention and emotion: A clinical perspective. Hove, England:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
White, R. T. (1986). Origins of PEEL. In J. R. Baird and I. J. Mitchell (Eds.), Improving the quality
of teaching and learning: An Australian case study – the PEEL Project (pp. 1- 7). Melbourne:
Monash University.
107
White, J. D. (1999). Personality, temperament, and ADHD: A review of the literature. Personality
& Individual Differences, 27, 589–598.
Widiger, T. A., & Clark, L. A. (2000). Toward DSM-V and the classification of psychopathology.
Psychological Bulletin, 126, 946–963.
Widiger, T. A., & Trull, T. J. (1992). Personality and psychopathology: An application of the five-
factor model. Journal of Personality, 60, 363–393.
Yore, L. D., & Craig, M. T. (1992). Middle school students’ metacognitive knowledge about
science reading and science text: Objective assessment, validation and results. East Lansing, MI:
National Center for Research on Teacher Learning. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No.
ED356134)
***~~~~~~~***
108
APPENDICES
109
Sweekaar Academy of Rehabilitation Sciences
Department of Psychology, Secunderabad.
Consent Form
You may not be expected to get any direct benefit from being a part of this study. But the results
of the research may provide benefits to the society in the form of advancing of psychological
intervention especially for women.
We assure you that the collected details from you for the study will be kept confidential. Your
participation/non-participation in this study will not affect your relationship with the researcher or
the institution where you are associated.
________________________________________
_______________________________________
Date:____________________
110
Socio-Demographic Data Sheet
Age:
Education:
Occupation/ Vocation:
Place of Origin:
Mother tongue:
No of family members:
Any Disability:
111
Metacognitive Awareness Inventory (MAI)
11. I ask myself if I have considered all options when solving a problem.
19. I ask myself if there was an easier way to do things after I finish a task.
112
22. I ask myself questions about the material before I begin.
23. I think of several ways to solve a problem and choose the best one.
36. I ask myself how well I accomplish my goals once I’m finished.
38. I ask myself if I have considered all options after I solve a problem.
43. I ask myself if what I’m reading is related to what I already know.
113
47. I try to break studying down into smaller steps.
49. I ask myself questions about how well I am doing while I am learning something new.
50. I ask myself if I learned as much as I could have once I finish a task.
51. I stop and go back over new information that is not clear.
114
Emotional Intelligence Scale
Instructions: Indicate the extent to which each item applies to you using the following scale:
1 = strongly disagree
2 = disagree
3 = neither disagree nor agree
4 = agree
5 = strongly agree
_____ 1. I know when to speak about my personal problems to others.
_____ 2. When I am faced with obstacles, I remember times I faced similar obstacles and overcame
them.
_____ 6. Some of the major events of my life have led me to re-evaluate what is important and not
important.
_____ 8. Emotions are some of the things that make my life worth living.
_____ 12. When I experience a positive emotion, I know how to make it last.
115
_____ 16. I present myself in a way that makes a good impression on others.
_____ 17. When I am in a positive mood, solving problems is easy for me.
_____ 18. By looking at their facial expressions, I recognize the emotions people are experiencing.
_____ 20. When I am in a positive mood, I am able to come up with new ideas.
_____ 23. I motivate myself by imagining a good outcome to tasks I take on.
_____ 24. I compliment others when they have done something well.
_____ 26. When another person tells me about an important event in his or her life, I almost feel
_____ 27. When I feel a change in emotions, I tend to come up with new ideas.
_____ 28. When I am faced with a challenge, I give up because I believe I will fail.
_____ 29. I know what other people are feeling just by looking at them.
_____ 30. I help other people feel better when they are down.
_____ 31. I use good moods to help myself keep trying in the face of obstacles.
_____ 32. I can tell how people are feeling by listening to the tone of their voice.
_____ 33. It is difficult for me to understand why people feel the way they do.
Source: Schutte, N.S. (1998). Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence,
Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 167-177.
116
Scoring for the Emotional Intelligence Scale:
Reverse score (1=5, 2=4, 3=3, 4=2, 5=1) items 5, 28, 33.
Average scores:
117
Sex: Women □ Man □ Age: ______
The phrases listed below refer to different ways of acting and thinking. Read each one and mark
with an X the alternative, of the five listed, that best fits your way of being. The response for each
statement alternatives are:
1. Completely disagree 2. Disagree 3. Neither agree nor disagree 4. Agree 5. Completely agree
1. No matter how much people get at me, I avoid getting into fights.
4. When somebody annoys me I do something to try and make he/she looks stupid.
10. I find it difficult for me to take advantage of the feelings of others in order to obligue them to
do something.
11. When I’m angry with someone I know I leave them out of activities on purpose.
14. If someone I know messes with me, I try not to turn others against them.
118
16. Even if there is someone in a group who I do not like, I try not to exclude them.
17. When someone annoys me or pushes me, I would rather leave than fight.
18. When I’m angry with a friend I get other people to stop talking to them.
19. I prefer to get out of the way and stay out of trouble whenever someone is hassling to me.
20. There are people who pushed me so far that we came to blows.
21. I have taken anything (even a pin or button) that belonged to someone else.
23. When I’m angry with somebody I say unpleasant things about them even though they may not
be true.
24. I don’t often exclude people I don’t like from the conversation.
26. Even if I was angry with someone I would never make false accusations about them.
Reverse score (1=5, 2=4, 3=3, 4=2, 5=1) items 1, 5, 8, 10, 12, 14, 16, 17, 19, 22, 24 & 26.
119
PASSIONATE LOVE SCALE
1. Since I’ve been involved with ____________, my emotions have been on a roller coaster.
8. I’d get jealous if I thought ____________ were falling in love with someone else.
120
not at all true 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 definitely true
16. ____________ is the person who can make me feel the happiest.
20. If I were separated from ____________for a long time, I would feel intensely lonely.
21. I sometimes find it difficult to concentrate on work because thoughts of ____________ occupy
my mind.
121
23. Knowing that ____________ cares about me makes me feel complete.
24. I eagerly look for signs indicating ____________’s desire for me.
25. If ____________ were going through a difficult time, I would put away my own concerns to
help him/her out.
30. I get extremely depressed when things don’t go right in my relationship with ____________.
Note: Items 2, 5, 6, 7, 8, 10, 11, 14, 15, 17, 19, 22, 24, 29, 30 make up the shortened version.
122
Scoring for Passionate Love Scale:
Scoring is either kept continuous or broken into the following classifications (for the 15 item
shortened version):
*** ~~~~~~~***
123