Jain, Pinky (2014) The structure of prior knowledge.
PhD thesis, University of Nottingham.
Access from the University of Nottingham repository:
http://eprints.nottingham.ac.uk/14553/1/Pinky_Jain_PhD_-_2014_Education_-
_Structure_of_Prior_Knowledge.pdf
Copyright and reuse:
The Nottingham ePrints service makes this work by researchers of the University of
Nottingham available open access under the following conditions.
This article is made available under the University of Nottingham End User licence and may
be reused according to the conditions of the licence. For more details see:
http://eprints.nottingham.ac.uk/end_user_agreement.pdf
For more information, please contact [email protected]
THE STRUCTURE OF
PRIOR KNOWLEDGE
PINKY JAIN, BEd (Hons), MA
Thesis submitted to the University of Nottingham
for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy
JULY 2014
ii
ABSTRACT
The phenomenon of prior knowledge is deep rooted in the rhetoric of
education. There is much discourse within pedagogy about its value and
pivotal role in the formulation of new learning. However teachers are not
able to use prior knowledge effectively as they do not have a working
sense of it, but are using it intuitively and colloquially. While researchers
provide a multitude of definitions of prior knowledge, no one has
examined its elemental structure in a way that provides a model for
teachers to use and support learning. This deficit is surprising as prior
knowledge is a universally accepted pedagogical notion. The aim of this
thesis is to fill the deficit and establish a structure of prior knowledge.
The research was situated within Year 1 primary mathematics classrooms
following eight teachers across five schools over one academic year. Using
naturalistic research methodology, the data were gathered through audio
recordings of the interactions between teachers and children during
mathematics lessons. These recordings were analysed using grounded
theory and content analysis.
The research explored and produced a partial model of prior knowledge
emerging from the data which includes at least eight interconnected
elements – abstraction, acculturation, cognition, context, individual
motivation, metacognition, perception and social group. These can be
seen as elements which can shape children’s memory – the central
feature of the prior knowledge that they bring to each mathematical task.
Children may manifest different degrees of these elements, and possibly
iii
of others which did not appear in these data, in different proportions and
balances.
Such a prior knowledge model, even though it remains partial, gives a
deeper understanding to a common but widely misunderstood term. The
implications of knowing and understanding more and in more depth about
the structure of prior knowledge are potentially far-reaching for children,
schools, teachers and curriculum development.
Keywords: prior knowledge; prior learning; primary schools; primary
education; mathematics education; mathematics teachers; elementary
school mathematics; primary school teachers
iv
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
I would like to say many many thanks to all those who have supported me
in this journey. I hope that as time has passed, you all know how much
your support, truth and direction have helped me to meet my goal. There
are always people who pass through our lives and give us the nudge we
need to stay on track. Some for a fleeting moment and soon forgotten,
but their impact is felt forever. Thank you!
To all the schools, teachers and children who have let me be in their
classrooms to listen and learn how things should be done, your
commitment to helping improve how we support and enrich our children’s
lives is commendable.
I would like to thank Dr Peter Gates for his patience, warmth,
insightfulness, direction, faith and support for taking me on as his student
and sticking with me through what, at times for him, must have been a
never-ending journey. I have learnt a lot from you about how to support
students well and help them grow. Thank you does not really express how
much it means to me to have your support and faith, but thank you.
Many thanks to Jackie Stevenson for always being at the end of the phone
and email to point me in the right direction through the maze of the
academic paperwork process. You have been wonderful.
Thank you Mum for being my garden of calm. You have always been there
to support, love and say the right things to keep me going. If it were not
v
for you, I would have given up a long time ago. Without you to take all
the noise away from the house, I would not have been able to finish.
Lakshya, thank you for being my critical friend. I know how hard it is for
you to stop singing and letting Mummy do her work.
Maanvi, you have taught me so much about myself and how to be a better
parent. I want to thank you for being you and reminding me to keep
studying.
Karunika, you are my oasis of joy and have really taught me throughout
this process the power of asking questions and that we never stop
learning.
Nilesh, thank you for always being you and your amazing skill at being
able to help unknot my thoughts and read my handwriting, and for never
faltering in your belief in me.
vi
Ithaca
When you set out on your journey to Ithaca,
pray that the road is long,
full of adventure, full of knowledge.
The Lestrygonians and the Cyclops,
the angry Poseidon -- do not fear them:
You will never find such as these on your path,
if your thoughts remain lofty, if a fine
emotion touches your spirit and your body.
The Lestrygonians and the Cyclops,
the fierce Poseidon you will never encounter,
if you do not carry them within your soul,
if your soul does not set them up before you.
Pray that the road is long.
That the summer mornings are many, when,
with such pleasure, with such joy
you will enter ports seen for the first time;
stop at Phoenician markets,
and purchase fine merchandise,
mother-of-pearl and coral, amber and ebony,
and sensual perfumes of all kinds,
as many sensual perfumes as you can;
visit many Egyptian cities,
to learn and learn from scholars.
Always keep Ithaca in your mind.
To arrive there is your ultimate goal.
But do not hurry the voyage at all.
It is better to let it last for many years;
and to anchor at the island when you are old,
rich with all you have gained on the way,
not expecting that Ithaca will offer you riches.
Ithaca has given you the beautiful voyage.
Without her you would have never set out on the road.
She has nothing more to give you.
And if you find her poor, Ithaca has not deceived you.
Wise as you have become, with so much experience,
you must already have understood what Ithacas mean.
Constantine P. Cavafy (1911)
vii
viii
For Papa
I miss you reminding me
To never limit my challenges
But to challenge my limits
ix
x
TABLE OF CONTENTS
ABSTRACT ...................................................................................... iii
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ...................................................................... v
TABLE OF CONTENTS ....................................................................... xi
LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................. xv
LIST OF FIGURES ........................................................................... xvii
1 INTRODUCTION ....................................................................... 1
1.1 Motivation...................................................................... 1
1.2 Gaps in Understanding .................................................... 2
1.3 Data Gathering ............................................................... 4
1.4 Data Analysis ................................................................. 4
1.5 Research Findings ........................................................... 5
1.6 Chapter Outline .............................................................. 5
2 LITERATURE REVIEW ............................................................... 7
2.1 Introduction ................................................................... 7
2.2 Methodology for Literature Identification .......................... 10
2.2.1 Introduction ........................................................ 10
2.2.2 Determination of Vocabulary ................................. 13
2.3 Primary Education ......................................................... 16
2.3.1 Historical Backdrop .............................................. 16
2.3.2 Political Social and Cultural Backdrop ..................... 18
2.4 Primary Mathematics Classroom ..................................... 23
2.4.1 Mathematics Curriculum and Content ..................... 23
2.4.2 Impact of Mathematical Content on Teaching .......... 28
2.5 Prior Knowledge ........................................................... 33
2.5.1 Knowledge .......................................................... 35
2.5.2 Prior .................................................................. 50
2.5.3 Prior Knowledge Research ..................................... 50
2.5.4 Summary ........................................................... 67
2.6 Why Look at Prior Knowledge?........................................ 68
2.6.1 Effects of Prior Knowledge on Learning ................... 69
2.6.2 Effects of Prior Knowledge on Learning of Mathematics .
......................................................................... 72
2.7 Teachers’ Understanding of Prior Knowledge .................... 78
xi
2.8 Conclusion ................................................................... 86
3 IDENTIFYING RESEARCH METHODOLOGY ................................. 89
3.1 Introduction ................................................................. 89
3.2 Research Question ........................................................ 90
3.3 Nature of Research ....................................................... 91
3.3.1 What is Good Research? ....................................... 96
3.3.2 Objectivity and Subjectivity................................... 97
3.3.3 Positioning the Research ..................................... 102
3.4 Choosing a Research Methodology ................................ 103
3.4.1 Naturalistic Research .......................................... 103
3.4.2 Summary.......................................................... 107
3.5 Generalisation and Validity ........................................... 108
3.5.1 Internal Validity and Reliability ............................ 109
3.5.2 External Validity ................................................ 110
3.6 Conclusion ................................................................. 113
4 DATA COLLECTION METHOD ................................................. 115
4.1 Introduction ............................................................... 115
4.2 Design of the Data Collection ....................................... 116
4.2.1 The Schools ...................................................... 116
4.2.2 The Teachers .................................................... 119
4.2.3 Lesson Observation ............................................ 123
4.2.4 Recording and Transcribing ................................. 125
4.2.5 Other Data ........................................................ 127
4.3 The Data ................................................................... 128
4.4 Ethical Considerations ................................................. 129
5 ANALYSIS METHODOLOGY .................................................... 133
5.1 Introduction ............................................................... 133
5.2 Criteria for Selecting the Analysis Methodology ............... 136
5.3 Exploration and Evaluation of Possible Analysis Methodologies
................................................................................ 139
5.3.1 Hermeneutical Analysis....................................... 140
5.3.2 Domain Analysis ................................................ 142
5.3.3 Typological Analysis ........................................... 144
5.3.4 Analytic Induction .............................................. 146
5.3.5 Content Analysis ................................................ 148
5.3.6 Phenomenological Analysis.................................. 150
xii
5.3.7 Metaphor Analysis ............................................. 153
5.3.8 Grounded Theory ............................................... 155
5.4 Selection of Analysis Methodology ................................. 160
5.5 Worked Examples of the Analysis Process ...................... 161
5.5.1 Theoretical Sampling .......................................... 163
5.5.2 Analysis ............................................................ 165
5.5.3 Identifying Events .............................................. 166
5.5.4 Creating Concepts ............................................. 174
5.5.5 Developing the Model ......................................... 210
5.6 Ethical Considerations ................................................. 218
5.7 Summary................................................................... 220
6 PRIOR KNOWLEDGE MODEL .................................................. 223
6.1 Introduction ............................................................... 223
6.2 Memory ..................................................................... 224
6.2.1 Theoretical Perspective ....................................... 224
6.2.2 Definition .......................................................... 225
6.2.3 Empirical Evidence ............................................. 226
6.3 Context ..................................................................... 229
6.3.1 Theoretical Perspective ....................................... 229
6.3.2 Definition .......................................................... 238
6.3.3 Further Empirical Evidence.................................. 239
6.4 Acculturation .............................................................. 244
6.4.1 Theoretical Perspective ....................................... 244
6.4.2 Definition .......................................................... 251
6.4.3 Further Empirical Evidence.................................. 252
6.5 Metacognition ............................................................. 257
6.5.1 Theoretical Perspective ....................................... 257
6.5.2 Definition .......................................................... 262
6.5.3 Further Empirical Evidence.................................. 264
6.6 Other Emerging Categories .......................................... 270
6.6.1 Individual Motivation .......................................... 270
6.6.2 Perception ........................................................ 276
6.6.3 Cognition .......................................................... 283
6.6.4 Social Group ..................................................... 288
6.6.5 Abstraction ....................................................... 295
6.7 Summary................................................................... 301
xiii
7 RESEARCH IMPLICATIONS .................................................... 311
7.1 Introduction ............................................................... 311
7.2 Key Findings .............................................................. 311
7.3 Schools ..................................................................... 312
7.4 Teachers .................................................................... 316
7.5 Children ..................................................................... 320
7.6 Curriculum ................................................................. 322
7.7 Moving Forward .......................................................... 324
7.8 Summing Up .............................................................. 325
REFERENCES ................................................................................ 327
APPENDICES ................................................................................ 343
Appendix A – Data Collection Schools’ Ofsted Reports .............. 343
xiv
LIST OF TABLES
Table 2.1 Literature review concepts ................................................. 12
Table 4.1 Schools used for data collection ........................................ 117
Table 4.2 Discussion on ethical issues in data collection ..................... 129
Table 5.1 Key differences in grounded theory approaches (Onions, 2006,
p. 8-9) ........................................................................................ 157
Table 5.2 List of endpoint concepts ................................................. 209
Table 5.3 Mapping from concepts to categories ................................. 217
Table 7.1 Possible methods for understanding children’s prior knowledge
.................................................................................................. 318
Table 7.2 Possible methods for understanding children’s prior knowledge
.................................................................................................. 319
xv
xvi
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure 2.1 Plato’s Divided Line Model (Lavine, 1984, p. 32) .................. 44
Figure 5.1 Breakdown of life is a journey metaphor ........................... 155
Figure 5.2 Data collection and analysis process ................................. 163
Figure 5.3 Identifying relevant events ............................................. 167
Figure 5.4 Initial sorting of mathematical events ............................... 176
Figure 5.5 Different subsets of lengthier responses ........................... 180
Figure 5.6 Granular concepts for have done the task before ............... 190
Figure 5.7 Granular concepts for worked with others and other ideas for
tackling task................................................................................. 195
Figure 5.8 Granular concepts of some form of model or image............ 196
Figure 5.9 Granular concepts of different interpretations .................... 201
Figure 5.10 Granular concepts of child got answer wrong ................... 207
Figure 6.1 Forces influencing memory ............................................. 226
Figure 6.2 Composition of three children’s prior knowledge ................ 305
xvii
xviii
1 INTRODUCTION
1.1 Motivation
Prior knowledge plays a key role in children’s learning (Alexander, Pate,
Kulikowich, Farrell & Wright, 1989; Dochy, 1992; Alexander, Kulikowich &
Jetton, 1994). Whether it is examined from the constructivist, cognitive,
behavioural or any other perspective, it is widely accepted that prior
knowledge is the starting point for new learning (e.g. Vygotsky’s Zone of
Proximal Development). There are many studies which have concluded
that the variance observed in children’s test scores can be explained by a
child’s prior or pre-existing knowledge (Bloom, 1976; Tobias, 1994).
Walker (1987) and Weinert (1989) showed that intelligence cannot
compensate for low prior knowledge, however prior knowledge can
compensate for low intelligence. The British education system is based
upon the knowledge and understanding that the teaching function will be
a process of building new blocks of subject knowledge placed on prior
subject knowledge, as can be seen in the hierarchical structure of the
National Curriculum. With such importance and value placed upon prior
knowledge, it is essential that concentrated effort is given to
understanding prior knowledge.
As a primary classroom teacher, I have been interested in the ideas and
methods that children use to develop their mathematical skills. I am
baffled by and curious about the widespread cultural perception that not
being good at mathematics is acceptable. I also want to understand what
children bring to bear upon each classroom experience in mathematics
1
that leads to a huge variation in their ability to carry out mathematical
tasks. Therefore I have chosen the primary mathematics classroom as my
research context.
1.2 Gaps in Understanding
One of the most interesting and perplexing observations that I have made
is the great variation in children, who seemingly have similar lives, in their
ability to carry out mathematical tasks. In order to understand this
variation in children’s abilities to inform my teaching, I started by
exploring the concepts and ideas that authors such as Vygotsky, Dewey,
Piaget, Hughes, Evans, Clemson and Ginsburg had to offer. These
readings concluded that teaching and effective learning can only take
place when teachers have developed an understanding of what children
know and have learnt before. All learning theories rely on some form of
prior understanding or experience to be built upon in order for future
learning to take place. Therefore it is essential to understand individual
prior knowledge so that future learning can be tailored to individual needs.
As a classroom teacher, I am aware that there is a missing link between
the theoretical requirement to use prior knowledge for effective teaching
for learning, and the practical understanding of prior knowledge to
facilitate effective teaching for learning. That is to say, we realise the
theories state start from where the child is and build on this, but no
understanding is offered as to what is meant by where the child is or how
to gain this understanding for children.
2
There is limited literature looking at prior knowledge in any depth. There
are many reasons for this shortcoming. The major reason is due to the
lack of a clear agreed definition for prior knowledge. Furthermore, there is
even a lack in agreed labelling of prior knowledge. There are many terms
defined in various ways which lend themselves to being classified as prior
knowledge (prior learning, prior education, experience, prior concept,
experiential knowledge, experiential learning, background knowledge,
prior understanding). With such a variety of possible labels and the
confusion that ensues, it is vital to explicitly explore and gain an in-depth
understanding of prior knowledge. Before I do this, I must clarify the type
of knowledge that I am interested in. If it is subject-specific knowledge
(i.e. mathematical content knowledge), then the focus becomes
mathematical content and categorisation of this content to formulate a
detailed understanding of prior mathematical knowledge. However my
research is not concerned with prior subject knowledge, but the broader
concept of prior knowledge in the primary mathematics classroom.
Therefore I am making an upfront distinction between prior knowledge
and what is commonly understood as prior knowledge i.e. prior subject
knowledge, with my focus being the former. If one considers prior
knowledge to be all that an individual has as knowledge, then it goes
beyond subject-specific knowledge. Prior knowledge is affected by
experiences within and beyond the classroom. It is influenced by all areas
of life. However this aspect of prior knowledge has not been examined by
researchers.
Therefore the aim of this thesis is to provide an understanding of the
structure of prior knowledge of children in the context of the primary
3
mathematics classroom developed through a combination of theoretical
and empirical investigation.
1.3 Data Gathering
The underlying methodology used for structuring the research was
naturalistic research. The data collection comprised a year-long
observation of eight experienced Year One teachers across five primary
schools. Each teacher was observed regularly and their conversations in
the mathematics classroom were recorded through the use of a personal
remote microphone during the course of one academic year. The recorded
observations were transcribed. All data have been gathered and reported
as per the ethical guidelines for educational research from the British
Educational Research Association (2004). The specific ethical issues
relevant to my research are discussed in Sections 4.4 and 5.6.
1.4 Data Analysis
The structure of prior knowledge was developed through analysis of the
transcripts. I used grounded theory as the framework for the analysis and
content analysis to understand the meaning of each transcript so that the
resulting interpretations may be organised using the framework of
grounded theory. This analysis formed the last stage of the method used
to gain an understanding of prior knowledge. Each transcript was looked
at in detail to identify events (incidents within lessons in which children
are engaged in mathematics), concepts (groups of events which have
similar properties) and categories (groups of concepts which function in a
4
similar manner or may be shaped by a similar force), and used memoing
to identify patterns within the data.
1.5 Research Findings
The key contribution made by this thesis is a partial model for the pre-
existing or prior knowledge of children in the context of the primary
mathematics classroom through empirical understanding gained from
analysing the transcripts. The prior knowledge model I propose comprises
eight interconnected elements – abstraction, acculturation, cognition,
context, individual motivation, metacognition, perception and social group
– shaping children’s memory which is the central feature of the prior
knowledge that they bring to each mathematical task. These elements or
building blocks that make up prior knowledge are the same in each child,
with the fundamental difference being the proportion and balance of these
elements present in each child at any given time.
1.6 Chapter Outline
The structure of the thesis is conventional with the hope that this uses the
reader’s own prior knowledge to focus upon my research process. Though
this thesis is sequential, and in some ways hierarchical in its structure, it
should not restrict the reader to a linear process for reading it. The aim is
to present a three-dimensional thesis which can be viewed from any
angle, none being the beginning or the end. The justification for this tacit
form is the very nature of the content. Not knowing the reader, your
needs and your prior knowledge, it is hoped that giving the ability to view
5
this as a sphere, one can approach it from any point which suits individual
needs and extract from it any ideas which are useful.
Chapter 2 provides a review of the relevant literature which has influenced
my thinking and research. Key focus areas are the research context, prior
knowledge and teachers’ understanding of it.
Chapter 3 explores various research paradigms and methodologies to
identify a suitable methodological framework to conduct the research.
Chapter 4 presents the method used for the data collection. It includes
information about the schools and teachers used for data collection,
recording and transcribing lessons, and the overall data set and also
discusses relevant ethical issues.
Chapter 5 focuses on the exploration, evaluation and explanation
underpinning the selection of a methodology for analysing the qualitative
data gathered for this thesis. It includes the criteria for selecting an
analysis methodology, brief description of a number of relevant analysis
methodologies, a description of the selected analysis methodology along
with a worked example, and also discusses relevant ethical issues.
Chapter 6 explains the overall outcome from the research and analysis
carried out for this thesis. It presents the structure of the partial prior
knowledge model.
Chapter 7 examines the key findings, implications and value of this
research on schools, teachers, children and curriculum, and possible next
steps.
6
2 LITERATURE REVIEW
2.1 Introduction
This chapter provides a review of the relevant literature which has
influenced my thinking and research. Through this review, I aim to
establish a theoretical framework for prior knowledge which is firmly
based upon the foundation of previous work by other researchers. This
framework is relevant in aiding the understanding and placing of questions
being asked through this thesis into a well-established context. Another
by-product of analysing relevant literature will allow determining the
boundaries and contextual parameters within the areas in which this
research makes an original contribution. By the end of this chapter, I plan
to establish the current state of research in understanding of prior
knowledge, and identify any shortcomings within this understanding.
It is worth re-iterating my research objective, which is to provide an
understanding of the structure of prior knowledge of children in the
context of the primary mathematics classroom. The aim of the structure is
to assist teachers to develop their understanding of how to support
children’s learning.
In order to be transparent, it is vital to explicitly identify and explain the
research process which has evolved through the course of this study.
These are processes for the location, identification and analysis of
secondary sources of knowledge to give an anchor to my study. The
progression of this chapter shows the evolutionary path which I have gone
7
through to develop as a researcher, to establish the relevance of this
research for future application, and to further my existing knowledge in
the areas that I am considering. I have made extended efforts to knit
together not only the many different sources of information, but also the
vast plethora of ideas which have aided in understanding and clarifying
what it is exactly that I am trying to establish.
The biggest struggle for me in carrying out the literature review was not
the finding of relevant material, but the elimination of ideas which I
concluded as not having any value to my thinking or my research
objective. The process of thinking is crucial to the whole thesis as it gives
it a structure and sets out the limitations from the onset.
In Section 2.2, I describe the methodology that I used to identify, critique
and summarise the relevant literature reviewed in this chapter. I address
the problematic issues of varied terminology linked to the relevant
literature. Also stating the limits and parameters of the literature review
will establish a true picture of what is pre-existing knowledge in this area
and the limitations of this knowledge in informing this study.
In Sections 2.3 and 2.4, I consider the overall context for this research. In
order to understand any socio-educational research, I must establish a
picture of the reality within which this research is based, as it is not
possible or desirable to conduct this research in a vacuum. I look at the
historical, political, social and cultural backdrops of primary education and
the primary mathematics classroom as they were at the time of data
collection. I aim to provide research that will have future worth and is
based within a practical and real context.
8
Though I am not looking at the effectiveness of classroom practice as my
focus is on the prior knowledge of children, I acknowledge that this
practice has an influence on the nature and characteristics of the data that
I can gather. Due to the ever-changing nature of education and the
political climate, there have been evolutionary steps in the pedagogical
philosophy of the delivery of the curriculum which I have examined.
In Section 2.5, I examine prior knowledge by looking in turn at
knowledge, prior and prior knowledge. This section presents the many
difficulties which have come to light in relation to the different
terminologies and definitions found in the literature. The main themes
explored in the section are the complex issues linked to what I mean by
prior knowledge and how I define it. This section culminates in my own
working definition of prior knowledge. Academically I would like to
establish an unambiguous definition of prior knowledge which can be
applied to my empirical study. Through the thesis, I would like to develop
something that teachers can use in order to enhance children’s learning of
mathematics.
I specifically look into prior knowledge in relation to mathematics to
establish that I am contributing new knowledge. My main goal in this
research is to develop an initial model for understanding and identifying
the structure of prior knowledge in relation to primary school
mathematics, therefore without analysis of this area it is not possible to
establish an effective approach to the research.
Section 2.6 considers the value of looking at prior knowledge from the
perspectives of learning and learning mathematics.
9
In Section 2.7, I examine teachers’ understanding of prior knowledge in
general and in the children they are teaching. It is vital that in order to
build and establish any sense of value to the research, the role of the
teacher and their understanding of prior knowledge is established.
In the final section, through the synthesis of all the sections, I establish
the gaps which exist in understanding prior knowledge within the
literature. Crucially this section achieves the main outcome of defining and
establishing a shared understanding of the complexities of meeting my
research objective.
2.2 Methodology for Literature
Identification
2.2.1 Introduction
In order to cover all the possible lines of enquiry related to this research,
the process of locating the relevant literature went through many phases.
The initial temptation as a novice researcher was to look at the entire
plethora of available literature databases, to consider all the possible
related concepts, and to examine the multitudes of literature that was
located as a result. Through the process of initially searching all the major
databases, two things became apparent. Firstly, the vastness of the
possible literature which could be considered and the huge impracticality
of demands which came with wanting to look at all of it without any
omissions. Secondly, the interlinking of the literature and how this added
an extra layer of complexity, as it did not allow for straightforward
10
identification of the relevant literature. This mixing of concepts forced me
to consider ideas in ways in which I had not done before, and allowed me
to further consider and combine ideas which were not intuitive.
The starting point was my research objectives:
1. To gain an understanding of prior knowledge of children in the
context of the primary mathematics classroom.
2. To use the understanding to develop a structure of prior knowledge
within the primary mathematics classroom which can be used by
teachers to enhance children’s mathematical learning.
To address the above, I took each objective and fragmented it into
possible root notions (Table 2.1). Literature was located under the
following parameters:
1. Only materials written in English or translated into English were
considered.
2. The databases were searched only up to 1986. Through reading,
material prior to 1986 which was identified was also considered.
3. Five of the major research literature databases were used – ERIC,
BEI, IBSS, Zetoc and PsycINFO.
4. The searches were carried out with some keywords which were
identified as a product of the background reading. I have
considered the identification of pertinent vocabulary in detail in the
following section.
Some of the above parameters were as a result of my own shortcomings.
For example, I have only considered literature written in or translated into
11
English. Also I have made the choice to only go as far back as 1986. The
logic behind this choice was the idea that through looking at a substantial
body of past work, any other seminal work prior to that should be evident
in the literature identified up to 1986.
The overwhelming fear at this stage was having gaps in my literature
review. However I felt, as a result of my initial background reading, that
there was a ball of string effect in place. That is to say, through the
location of some of the initial relevant literature and following their
references, I was able to locate further literature of relevance.
Furthermore, previously identified relevant references would often keep
reappearing. Thus, like a ball of string, there were many points at which
the same literature appeared and crossed over. This ensured that key
themes and seminal articles were located.
This drive to review all the possible areas of literature which may have
even the vaguest influence on the concepts being investigated led to the
identification of an enormous quantity of literature. This stage of the
searching process seemed endless, and through a process of dual
classification – the information that the material had to offer and the type
of subject matter it was covering – I was able to hone in on the literature
which would form the cornerstone of my research.
Table 2.1 Literature review concepts
Knowledge
Prior knowledge – gaps in understanding
Primary school education – overview of political, historical, cultural and
social contexts
12
Nature of the primary classroom – what does teacher and pupil interaction
show in mathematics
Teachers’ understanding of prior knowledge in general and in mathematics
Mathematics education – overview of political, historical, cultural and
social contexts
Mathematics and prior knowledge
Children’s learning and the effects of prior knowledge on
performance/ability and understanding
Having detailed the practical process and the obstacles faced in the path
to identification and location of key literature, it is hoped that the
limitations and extent of the scope of the study have been made
transparent. Openness to the background from which the ideas presented
in this thesis have been obtained allows the reader to gain a firm
understanding about the possibilities that the results have to offer and
increases the applicability of the outcomes established.
2.2.2 Determination of Vocabulary
The most problematic issue that I faced in this thesis was one of definition
and identification of relevant vocabulary to explicitly explain the notions I
wished to explore. From the onset, there were many methods which I
used to try and verbalise what it was that I wished to consider and the
notion of defining that which was not understood has been a great
challenge. Here I feel it is of value to look at the different stages of
thinking which have passed and how these have fed into the process of
forming a personal lexicon which has formed the conceptual framework
for this research.
13
Firstly, I wanted to understand the processes which take place in the
classroom within each individual child which enable them to have an
understanding of the mathematics they are involved in. Secondly, I
wanted to reflect on the causes of the varying abilities of children which
facilitate reactions and interactions while involved in mathematics.
Therefore what are these processes or structures which determine the
level of engagement and the success that each child has with the
mathematics taking place in the classroom? Furthermore, the extended
nature of this interaction manifests itself in the ability or aptitude which
the individual shows towards mathematics. So how to define this abstract
and almost random process?
I also wanted to understand what children bring to bear upon each
classroom experience in mathematics. What is it about the processes
which take place in each child that result in such huge variation in the
outcome/understanding of the mathematics they are involved in? The
initial reading of the literature which looks at children’s understanding in
mathematics (because the understanding or outcome that they show in
carrying out mathematical tasks is a direct result of their processing and
thinking) puts heavy emphasis upon the notion that “understanding
seems to take two major forms: perceiving accurately and making
connections among various areas of knowledge, including our intuition”
(Ginsburg, 1989, p. 183).
Furthermore this concept of understanding focuses on the construction of
“schemata to link what we know already with our new learning” (Clemson
& Clemson, 1994, p. 18).
14
I wanted to explore this notion of what we already know being the
lynchpin to any new understanding, as this was the constant and recurring
theme in all the literature that I considered in order to understand why
children are so different in their ability, interaction and success in
mathematics. Literature alluded to knowledge, understanding, exposure,
and experience prior to the learning experience that the children are
engaged in as the factors that shape the children and create differences in
each individual. Furthermore, in contrast to many theorists e.g. Piaget,
this process of learning is not simply a biological progress (Blanck, 1996).
Hence these factors, among others, need to be considered in order to
understand and conceptualise the solutions for this thesis. Therefore the
various terms which came from these initial readings were prior
experiential learning, prior knowledge and prior learning.
To summarise, in order to understand the reasons for the differences in
individual ability within mathematics, the literature read as a background
to this thesis highlighted prior knowledge as a key factor which children
bring to the classroom, prior knowledge which has not been influenced by
their current situation but by their past experiences. Therefore the
proposed lexicon of terms stated above formed the search path for the
identification of the literature which will help to understand this small key
concept and how it functions and enables children to carry out
mathematical tasks in the classroom.
15
2.3 Primary Education
This section maps changes which have historically occurred in English
primary education. Reviewing the legislative path of education allows me
to consider the historical perspective as well as the political, social and
cultural views influencing changes in the culture of schools. It is through
understanding the engrained history that has evolved that we can
consider how change can be implemented and ideas that are likely to
succeed.
2.3.1 Historical Backdrop
The social and moral pressure to invest in children and their education has
constantly been the subject of debate within society. This has resulted in
numerous initiatives, reports and laws. A landmark publication to ignite
the reformation of the current system was the Plowden Report (Plowden,
1967). This was the first comprehensive review of primary education since
the Hadow Report of 1931 (Hadow, 1931). The Plowden Report
emphasised the need to see children as individuals and also relied heavily
on Piagetian theories which were the dominant influences of the time.
The report’s recurring themes are individual
learning, flexibility in the curriculum, the
centrality of play in children’s learning, the use
of the environment, learning by discovery and
the importance of the evaluation of children’s
progress – teachers should ‘not assume that
only what is measurable is valuable.’
(Gillard, 2004)
16
This is in grave contrast to the introduction of the National Curriculum
(NC) through the 1988 Education Reform Act (1988 c. 40). The focus on
individual and free learning was now changed to the fixed “syllabus and
content that every child should study until he or she leaves school” (Moon,
2001, p. 1).
In some senses, this has led to removal of the child (the individual) from
the way we teach to be replaced by children (the masses). The NC
removed any sense of children being different or having different ways of
learning. The Rumbold Report (Rumbold, 1990) completes the cycle of
primary education and the philosophical changes it has been through since
the 1960s. The report recommends a rethink of how a system can develop
and support the children in a new society. There is emphasis once again
on quality and provisions being made for children as young as three and
the ability for parents to have choice of many settings for the care of their
children.
Since then, there have been many other reports and reviews into the way
in which primary education is delivered or should be delivered in England,
with the latest being the Cambridge Primary Review (Alexander, 2010)
and the Rose Review (Rose, 2009). Educators continue to struggle with
the balance between education of the masses and the individual. This brief
overview illustrates the pendulum swinging between education for all and
education for the individual.
Principles within education are influenced by changes in societal attitudes
and demands of the individual through time. Knowing this allows us to see
the change in the nature of schools and the views we have of children and
17
their development within the system. Within the current educational
landscape, there is great ambiguity and debate on the nature and shape
of primary school education which has manifested itself in the position we
find ourselves in at present, with lack of agreement on a new curriculum
and suspension of all existing guidance.
2.3.2 Political Social and Cultural Backdrop
Having considered the path taken through history in primary education
which has led to a set of underlying philosophies, the main aim of this
section is to consider the influences which brought about these changes.
The introduction of the National Curriculum (NC) was very closely linked
to the political situation of the 1980s. Moon (2001) argues that the reason
for the creation of the NC was that it allowed for a reduction of differences
between schools, and a reduction in the inequality of provision.
The government believes that all people should
follow a broad, balanced and suitably
differentiated programme until age 16; that
such a programme should contain a strong
element which relates to the technological
aspects of working life.
(Department of Education and Science, 1985, para 46)
The NC aims to raise standards, improve communication, allows for
provision to be made for progress and continuity, and necessitates
measurement and tracking of individual attainment. At the time of
introducing the NC, the social structure was demanding all of these and
viewed these to be lacking in the education system. The 1980s growth of
incomes, the growth of two-parent working families, and to some extent,
18
a greater movement of individuals geographically, all led to the need for a
national measurable structure for schools. Also the economic and political
situation of the time focused on employment and the need for young
people to be employable.
By the 1980s the effects of the global economy
were being realised. The ‘Asian tiger’ economies
of Japan, South Korea and Taiwan were
producing better industrial goods more cheaply
and were sucking away customers from Britain.
Their education systems appeared to benefit
from teaching basic skills through traditional
methods. Controlling the curriculum to make it
more suited to industrial production was seen as
one of the means of enabling Britain to
compete.
(Ward, 2004, p. 83)
The overall cultural, economic and social environment put pressure on
educational change to be a major part of the political agenda. The
enterprising materialistic 1980s demanded the same from schools. It can
be argued that the Thatcher administration was responsible for some of
the most radical changes in education in the UK to date. There were many
political reasons behind such a radical change in the way the system was
structured and functioning.
On the one hand, the government had taken
central control of the curriculum and national
testing, but had de-centralised spending and
management. In fact, the so-called devolution of
funding was designed to reduce the power of
the LEAs (Local Education Authorities). It was
the Conservative government’s political
intention to limit the power of left-wing Labour-
controlled local authorities, particularly the
Inner London Education Authority (ILEA).
(Ward, 2004, p. 84)
19
The need to gain control on a local level to ensure political longevity was
another crucial reason behind this great change. There seemed to be little
pedagogical motivation for introducing the NC, but one that did have an
impact on the pedagogical dialogue of schools.
In the late 1990s, the Labour government did little to change the nature
and ethos of education as they enjoyed this centralised structure and
control over education. Therefore we were left in the UK to date with a
system which, in many senses, was rigid and set in an inflexible structure.
Furthermore, it was a structure which required great amount of
maintenance and also demanded a lot of tasks which were not related to
the job of teaching and learning.
The primary schools in England suffered from a severe case of split
allegiances. Firstly, there was the rigour of the NC and the state control
through the NC. With this came the whole mechanism of assessment
levelling, teaching only written content, administrative duties which must
be completed, and ranking and reporting to the consumer or stakeholder.
All of these were in line with the capitalist market methodology for the
functioning of schools. Secondly, there was the need of the individual child
and the teachers’ understanding of what their children needed. Thirdly,
teachers needed to push children to measure up to the required standard
for the results to be achieved for the school.
There was a pedagogical mismatch between the way in which the NC was
implemented and the needs of children. This mismatch has been
identified, and since September 2008 there have been some moves to
allow teachers greater freedom in the ways in which they implement the
20
curriculum, e.g. removal of the Qualifications and Curriculum Agency
subject guidance from schools. Furthermore, the move to an open-ended
curriculum structure has given greater opportunities for schools and
teachers to teach in the way their children learn best. However,
paradoxically there still looms over teachers this need to validate their
choices in terms of fixed and constant measurements. This is most
evident in the Foundation Stage. For example, though teachers have been
given free rein to choose methods for teaching which they feel best allow
their children to learn (through play and unstructured activities), there is
still the need for them to measure this learning and produce comparative
data. So in some senses, the changes in place have given freedom with
one hand, but still tie the teacher to the NC and all its trappings on the
other hand. Overall there continues to be a huge mismatch between
political intentions and pedagogical needs which has an impact on the
nature of the classroom and the relationship between knowledge and the
individual.
It needs to be noted that, as was the case historically, some of the
motivation for educating children has changed little. Furthermore, the
expectation of the system from outside observers is that it will aid in the
production of effective members of the working society. The question of
knowledge being possessed for its own sake has not been addressed by
the system.
Therefore to conclude, the system in place at present is
an anticipatory mirror, a perfect introduction to
industrial society. The most criticised features of
education today – the regimentation, lack of
21
individualisation, the rigid systems of seating,
grouping, grading and marking, the
authoritarian role of the teacher – are precisely
those that made mass public education so
effective an instrument of adaptation for its
place and time.
(Toffler, 1970, p. 355)
Though this observation may seem a little out of date and an overly dark
view of what education is in England, it depicts the atmosphere in primary
schools which prevails, to some extent, even today. As we approach a
time of change, we are poignantly reminded to pay heed to our prior
experiences.
Wheels have been pointlessly reinvented.
Initiatives have been introduced at such a pace
that they have been superseded before being
properly evaluated. The lessons of past attempts
to reform have not been learnt. The lessons of
past research and development have been
treated as irrelevant not because they are
genuinely inapplicable but merely because they
are more than a few months old, or maybe
because they challenge the preferred political
agenda. Yet knowledge, understanding and
progress, in policy as in the classroom, grow by
cumulation – by understanding, respecting,
learning from and building upon past experience
– not by relentless quest for novelty.
(Alexander, 2010, p. 38)
However overall the dominant force of central control means that what
goes on in the classroom needs to be accountable, and therefore can be
quite mechanical, with very little scope for overall variation from school to
school and class to class. The school culture is one of rigid structure and
depends on the state to dictate the ways in which teachers behave. This is
slowly changing with new demands to personalise and individualise
22
learning. My data were collected at the ebb of that changing tide. This
thesis is perfectly placed to be part of this change and rethink about how
we teach and manage children’s knowledge.
2.4 Primary Mathematics Classroom
This section examines the nature of the primary mathematics classroom. I
will only focus on Key Stage 1 (pupils aged five to seven) and not look any
further as it is not within the parameters of my research.
To support this examination, I will consider the following areas:
the mathematics curriculum and content, specifically the National
Curriculum, the National Numeracy Strategy and the Primary
Framework for Mathematics;
the impact of the mathematical content i.e. the National Numeracy
Strategy and the Primary Framework for Mathematics on teachers’
pedagogical choices.
2.4.1 Mathematics Curriculum and Content
There are many external influences on the mathematics classroom today.
As considered in previous sections, the centralisation of education has had
the largest impact upon the way classrooms are shaped. Major influences
upon mathematics in the classroom were the issuing of the Cockcroft
Report in 1982 Mathematics Counts (Cockcroft, 1982), the primary report
issued by the Numeracy Task Force in 1998 Numeracy Matters (Reynolds,
1998b), and more recently the Williams Review in 2008 Independent
Review of Mathematics Teaching in Early Years Settings and Primary
23
Schools (Williams, 2008) and the House of Commons Public Accounts
Committee report in 2009 Mathematics Performance in Primary Schools:
Getting the Best Results (House of Commons Public Accounts Committee,
2009).
The Cockcroft Report considered not only the nature, content and level of
mathematics being taught, but also the changes that were taking place in
society and how these affected the mathematics needed. The main aim of
the report was to give recommendations to enable building of better
mathematics teaching. However a key point to note is that the report
looked at teaching and learning of mathematics with the lens of further
employability of individuals, and not the learning of mathematics for its
own sake. The report made a slight shift in its focus from what was in
place in the classroom at the time – mathematics that was more
theoretical (declarative knowledge) in principle to a more practically
applicable mathematics (procedural knowledge). The report made several
recommendations which resulted in major changes that are still prevalent
in classrooms today.
As noted in the previous section, one of the main goals of the government
of the 1980s was to restrict the power of urban LEAs which led to the
overall centralisation of the curriculum. This resulted in the establishment
of the National Curriculum (NC) in 1988. However, due to the lack of
clarity in the NC of what was required by teachers to teach, there was
demand for a further detailed curriculum document for mathematics and
English. As a result, the National Numeracy Strategy (NNS) started as a
24
project in 1996 culminating in implementation in all primary schools from
September 1999 (Department for Education and Employment, 1999).
The Numeracy Strategy provides a highly
structured model for the teaching of elementary
mathematics … The strategy offers schools a
very detailed year-by-year curriculum, and
incorporates a requirement that each primary
school class should devote 45-60 minutes each
morning to mathematics.
(Gardiner, 2000, p. 6/6)
These (the NC and the NNS) have been the most dominant documents to
influence the primary mathematics classroom to date.
National Curriculum for England and Wales in
1989 was undoubtedly the most significant
statutory intervention in primary mathematics
for over a hundred years. Nevertheless, the
arrival of the National Numeracy Strategy into
English primary schools in 1999 will almost
certainly have had a greater impact.
(Askew, Millett, Brown, Rhodes & Bibby, 2001, p. 5-6)
The nature of teaching, planning, use of mathematics and language, and
also the content covered and content omitted was determined through the
implementation of the NC and the NNS. These documents have shaped
the nature of primary mathematics teachers as well as the training of new
teachers. Gardiner offers an interesting logic for the enormity of the
impact upon the system of one reform that has been supported.
England has no tradition of pedagogy and
didactics. There is therefore no accepted formal
way of analysing the challenges which confront
the mathematics teacher, or of communicating
intended modifications to existing or intending
teachers. The only vehicles are therefore
pragmatic ones: from textbooks, syllabuses and
25
examinations, to personal example and
encouragement to “reflect on one’s experience”
(though without a theoretical framework).
(Gardiner, 2000, p. 7/7)
Before the advent of the NC, it can be argued that there was no single
national pedagogical philosophy upon which teachers could base their
teaching decisions. It was this lack of pedagogical framework which led in
part to the rigidity with which the NC was adopted. Hence the NC, to some
extent, filled a gap in our pedagogical framework and further, in our social
views and discourse of education. The ripple effect of this fundamental
change was felt in every aspect of school and knowledge dissemination.
The NNS has and continues to shape, due to the absence of any other
guidance during the current process of rethinking, both the attitudes of
teachers and the expectations of parents. One of the key visible changes
in the primary classroom has been the introduction and emphasis on
mental maths and the view that mathematics is only valuable if there is
an application of it in real life.
The National Curriculum (and associated
assessment) encourages teachers: To see
school mathematics as being motivated and
justified by its uses (“We believe it should be a
fundamental principle that no topic should be
included unless it can be developed sufficiently
for it to be applied in a way which pupils can
understand” (Cockcroft, 1982, p. 133). “Pupils
should be given opportunities to use and apply
mathematics in practical tasks [and] in real-life
problems” (Department for Education, 1995, p.
11)).
(Gardiner, 2000, p. 7/7)
26
The balance of values between mathematics for practical applications and
mathematics for its own sake is currently under question with the current
coalition government considering reduction in the curriculum constraints,
and through changes in Ofsted’s (Office for Standards in Education,
Children’s Services and Skills) emphasis towards assessing learning. The
nature of the teacher-pupil interaction changed considerably as a result of
the philosophical change in approaches to mathematics brought upon by
the NNS. It is argued by many proponents of the NNS that there was
greater clarity in what teachers were to teach and, to a great extent, the
methods they were to apply. There was a balance to be achieved when we
consider the limitations which existed in the NNS when the learning of
such a diverse subject as mathematics was too prescriptive. Some of the
studies carried out to look at the impact of the NNS noted
the de-professionalisation of teachers. The
pressures which have been exerted on schools
in recent years to try to change the culture have
undermined the sense of professional autonomy
which is an essential ingredient in all good
teaching: teachers feel that their every move is
being monitored, often using inappropriate
criteria.
(Gardiner, 2000, p. 15/15)
As a result of such criticism, in 2006 the Primary Framework for
Mathematics (PFM) superseded the NNS (Department for Education and
Skills, 2006). Overall these are some of the factors influencing the culture
of the current primary classroom. Though there are wider issues, these
factors have a role to play in how teachers plan and implement the
teaching of mathematics. There is no unanimous agreement on the value
or validity of the changes in place through the NC. However, I must
27
accept that they inform the nature of the classroom and are a backdrop to
my study. It is not the scope of my research to question this backdrop,
merely to work with full knowledge of its strengths and flaws.
2.4.2 Impact of Mathematical Content on
Teaching
In this section, I want to look at the mathematical content from the
perspective of the impact it has had on the teachers and their pedagogical
choices. Looking at these factors is important as the backdrop of my
thesis is the National Numeracy Strategy (NNS) and the Primary
Framework for Mathematics (PFM) (collectively called strategies in this
section) as they play out in the classroom. All of my data are collected in
the culture of classrooms using both the NNS and the PFM. Teachers have
an assortment of complex and diverse approaches to the way in which
these two very similar documents are used. Despite the PFM being the
latest guidance, the teachers involved in my data collection preferred the
NNS as it offered detailed guidance for planning and delivery of
mathematics lessons while covering the same learning objectives as the
PFM. Therefore understanding the very nature and purpose of the NNS
remains vital as the classrooms in which I collected my data were still
depending upon the NNS as a major influence in supporting teacher
planning. Furthermore, taking time to examine how these documents
have shaped teacher behaviour is crucial, as it will allow me to look at the
data collected within context.
The structure of both the NNS and the PFM content is in school year
groups. The outline of what children should be able to do is organised in
28
strands. However the PFM makes no attempt to indicate how teachers
should teach each learning objective. Hence teachers tend to rely on the
NNS and the vast departmental online resources. Furthermore there is
little indication given as to the methods for measuring the level of
children’s ability once they can achieve the objectives. There are overall
assessment criteria, but they are not as clear as the content criteria. The
planning structure emphasises continuous assessment through the use of
Assessing Pupils’ Progress (APP).
Critics of both documents argue that the extent to which mathematics was
considered is limited and the emphasis is more on numeracy. There is
very limited application of terms such as mathematics or mathematical.
The language applied throughout the strategies is to aim, it would seem,
to move away from traditional notions of mathematics and promote a new
rebranded form of considering numbers. The strategies view numeracy as
an aspect of mathematics.
Numeracy relates to the broader area of
mathematics. Numeracy is described below as a
proficiency in various skills. The National
Curriculum for mathematics at each level is in
part focused directly upon such skills and in part
upon laying the foundation for higher levels of
mathematical study which, in turn, provide
further skills valuable in adult life.
(Reynolds, 1998a, p. 11)
The content for both strategies is developed under this definition of
numeracy:
Numeracy at Key Stages 1 and 2 is a proficiency
that involves a confidence and competence with
numbers and measures. It requires an
29
understanding of the number system, a
repertoire of computational skills and an
inclination and ability to solve number problems
in a variety of contexts.
(Reynolds, 1998a, p. 11)
This definition of numeracy and overall philosophy of the strategies are,
and continue to be, the rooting of values, practices and culture of the
primary mathematics classroom.
One of the biggest weaknesses of the strategies is the inability of the
curriculum content to guide teachers in terms of assessment. This lack of
clarity in assessing children has, to some extent, reduced the ways in
which the strategies have been applied with teachers initially rigidly
applying the requirements and measuring children in terms of levels
based on the NC documentation. However through the use of APP and
Assessment for Learning (AfL) models, teachers are beginning to look at
other methods for assessing children, though this is an area which needs
further development.
Mathematical concepts are varied in their complexity and how children
understand the concepts can also be varied. This has created an
ambiguous precedent within the teaching of numeracy. For example,
when we look at a Year One teaching program and consider one of the
objectives from the Calculating strand, it states, “Relate addition to
counting on; recognise that addition can be done in any order; use
practical and formal written methods” (Department for Education and
Skills, 2006, p. 72).
30
This objective can be demonstrated by children in many different ways.
For example, a child could simply show that they are able to count on as a
way of achieving addition, or can also demonstrate that they know that
adding can be done in any order by using the larger number as a starting
point, or also that tens can be added first and then units next, and so on.
The difficulty is that the teacher is not given flexibility to make any
different assessments. That is to say, all that can be noted is whether the
child has met the objective or not. There is no scope to consider how the
child met the objective as only the achievement of the objective is
recorded. This target-led assessment means that knowledge is treated in
a very linear manner and learners move along this linear continuum, and
hence pedagogical choices are made to fit this mould. The result of this is
that the progress of each child’s knowledge can be placed on a limited
trajectory with one target after another to be met and children moved on
accordingly. Among others, this has been one of the key criticisms of the
strategies.
The results of the 2007 assessment of Trends in International
Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS) showed that England did not fare
well taking seventh place after countries such as Hong Kong, Japan,
Singapore and Taiwan (Sturman et al., 2008). This, among other reasons,
has prompted a rethink within the current government to consider other
methodologies for further raising standards of mathematics. However as
this has not been made public knowledge, one can only guess what shape
these new strategies may take. This current rethinking and possible
investment is a result of the realisation that the strategies have not had
31
the long term effects it was hoped that they would in raising standards in
mathematics.
Since 2000, results at both Key Stages 1 (age
seven) and Key Stage 2 (age 11) have levelled
off. In 2008, 79% of pupils attained the
expected standard or above in mathematics at
Key Stage 2 in national tests. While this was the
highest ever recorded result, and 2% higher
than the previous year, it fell well short of the
target of 85% that the Department set to
achieve by 2006. 21% of pupils started
secondary school without a secure foundation in
mathematics. In 2008, 30,000 (5% of 11-year
olds) left primary school with mathematical
skills that were, at best, at the level of those
expected of a seven year old.
(House of Commons Public Accounts Committee, 2009, p. 7)
For teachers, the above findings imply that though the strategies were put
into place to raise standards in mathematics, there are still shortfalls
which they need to address (House of Commons Public Accounts
Committee, 2009). This could be due to the lack of pedagogical choice
offered through the constraints of these strategies and what needs to
occur is allowing teachers to make choices for addressing the needs of the
children they are teaching. Teachers feel under pressure to continuously
improve in all areas. The NC has entitlement as its core root. That is to
say:
Entitlement to equality of access to an
appropriate curriculum
Entitlement to equality of teaching experienced
Entitlement to equality of learning outcome
(Askew et al., 2001, p. 6)
32
Askew et al. (2001) argue that it is unreasonable to expect this notion of
entitlement to be the same for all children, and causes “tension between
teaching to meet the needs of the individual and teaching to meet the
needs of the collective” (p. 7).
It is this tension that is influencing the role of the teacher, the pedagogical
choices that they make and the atmosphere of the classroom with mixed
external messages, leaving teachers to make judgments and
interpretations as individuals and as schools (Askew et al., 2001).
To conclude this section, the nature of mathematical content and the
overall implementation of the strategies have caused some difficulties with
teachers and offer limited ideas for individualised teaching. But overall,
the fundamental issue in the classroom is the way in which teachers have
been encouraged to consider equity and entitlement. This has led to
teachers viewing children as groups rather than as individuals. The
demand for improvement of outcomes and need for equity has put
pressure upon teachers to provide the same educational provision for all
children. This interrelated conflict is between the knowledge that
individualised planning is the most supportive approach to extend
children’s learning and the dilemma of equity needs to be supported by
the understanding of prior knowledge.
2.5 Prior Knowledge
There are so many points from which I could start to consider the
following questions. How do I define prior knowledge? What do these
33
words mean? Are there different meanings linked to different contexts?
Are there different interpretations of these words in the literature?
In this section, I will address these and other questions in order to gain a
picture of the current views in the literature of prior knowledge. I will
establish a definition, according to the literature available, of prior
knowledge which can then be the springboard for development and the
basis of this thesis. At first I consider the literature available in all the
contexts, and not just primary school mathematics, as this will enable me
to derive a precise lexical definition of the term prior knowledge. The
ultimate goal at the end of this thesis is to propose a structure of prior
knowledge based on the outcome of this study which may be applied in
practice.
The section is organised in the following way:
considering the complex ideas linked to terminology and semantics
of the words prior knowledge, i.e. prior and knowledge, and the
many different definitions found in the literature for prior
knowledge;
looking at how prior knowledge can be defined using the literature
already available as a point of reference;
concluding with an unambiguous working definition of prior
knowledge based on literature which will be developed throughout
this thesis. Also a definition that can be understood by others and
form a common vocabulary between the researcher and the reader.
34
2.5.1 Knowledge
Defining knowledge is pivotal to this thesis. In this section I will look at
knowledge from the following perspectives:
philosophical and theoretical (epistemological);
cultural and societal;
educational (school knowledge) views on children;
mathematical;
individual.
It is of value to look at each perspective individually and distil the key
points which are applicable to my research objective. The definition
formulated here will be the first step in defining prior knowledge.
2.5.1.1 Philosophical and Theoretical Perspective
Knowledge is an ambiguous term which means different things depending
upon the context. Rand (1979) gives a sound starting point to the
philosophical debate when she writes “Knowledge is … a mental grasp of a
fact(s) of reality, reached either by perceptual observation or by a process
of reason based on perceptual observation” (p. 45).
Therefore knowledge, it can be argued, is an abstract concept which
allows us to contextualise and, to some extent, verbalise what we see, do,
observe, and interact with as humans. Furthermore, knowledge is
increased by what we gain from those interactions. Rand’s definition is in
no way an attempt to simplify the wider and more detailed definitions
offered by epistemological theoreticians, but more of a way to focus upon
35
the issues. Considering Rand’s statement in detail allows me to focus upon
what knowledge looks like, how it is acquired, and provides a starting
point for the development of a working definition. Knowledge is the by-
product of synthesising individuals’ observations and interactions. David
Hume (an empiricist) offers an overarching theory of knowledge which
forms one end of the spectrum by which Rand’s above statement is
supported. Sense perceptions which are broken into two notions of
impressions and ideas are the ways in which we expand our
understanding and knowledge (Hume 2010). Therefore to follow Hume’s
argument – we cannot know anything which we have not had prior
impression of in sensory experience. That is to say, our minds are void of
knowledge and only interactions with our sense allows for our knowledge
to grow.
However, Immanuel Kant’s theory of knowledge opposes this view of the
human mind being void of any knowledge prior to interaction with the
world and forms the other end of the knowledge spectrum (Kant 2010).
Kant proposes that the mind has twelve pure concepts (or categories)
which enable us to organise our vast numbers of sense observations.
These concepts are unity, plurality, totality, affirmation, negation,
limitation, substance-accidents, cause-effect, causal reciprocity,
possibility, actuality and necessity. One of the key arguments made by
Kant is that “the mind is not passive, as Hume and other empiricists also
claim” (Lavine, 1984, p. 194).
The mind is more active in the process of acquiring and sorting
knowledge. Knowledge gained is given some structure and meaning
36
through the twelve concepts and the process of sorting the incoming
knowledge. Furthermore Kant states that the concepts remain the same
and universally form the structure of any mind (Lavine, 1984, p. 194).
Thus the concepts make sensory observation a more interactive process
rather than passive as thought by the empiricist school of thought. Kant’s
twelve categories presuppose all experiences and remain unchanged
through any experience. Lastly Kant proposes that these twelve concepts
are a necessary tool for the mind as they enable the processing of
experiences to take place without which there would be no knowledge
which could be of further value.
There are further perspectives which believe that knowledge has its
source in rational truth and knowledge is derived from the use of senses
and critical analyses of those thoughts. Not just mere organisation into
categories or filtered through the categories as Kant stated, but going one
stage further, and using what has been before to assess and develop new
knowledge. This type of knowledge requires a deeper sense of
consciousness of all things and is built up over a series of interactions.
Therefore knowledge is always changing and fluid.
Whichever definition is prescribed, be it Hume’s which states knowledge is
limited to the moment and does not interact with the mind to a great
extent, or Kant’s views which consider the brain as a filter of knowledge
which is flexible, it is most striking that these perspectives of knowledge
are not simply facts and figures which must be learnt. Hume’s and Kant’s
definitions give scope to bring in all aspects of life as knowledge.
37
2.5.1.2 Cultural and Societal Perspective
The culture in which we exist and attain knowledge has a huge impact
upon the nature of that knowledge. It is this relationship between the
external (the culture) and internal (the knowledge that we are able to
acquire) factors which shape our thinking. In this section, I want to focus
not on what types of cultural knowledge there are, but more so on what
the culture we are operating in has to offer in the way of understanding
knowledge. Furthermore, I want to examine what knowledge is valued
within our culture. It is essential that I consider this with society and
culture being linked and influencing one another.
Culture should be regarded as the set of
distinctive spiritual, material, intellectual and
emotional features of society or a social group,
and that it encompasses, in addition to art and
literature, lifestyles, ways of living together,
value systems, traditions and beliefs.
(UNESCO, 2002, p. 9)
This is a concise but vital description of culture as it allows me to examine
the vastness of knowledge. In order to understand knowledge in this
culture, there needs to be a brief examination of English culture, the place
it gives to knowledge, and how it uses and expands knowledge. English
culture is wide and varied and has been and continues to be influenced by
many other cultures. Though a part of Europe, England is vastly different
in its nature and responses to the development and value it places on
knowledge. Schools are the pivotal way in which knowledge is
disseminated to the nation, and thus affect national identity. Merttens and
Head (2000), in Coulby’s book’s preface, ask the key question “To what
38
extent is what we know we ‘know’ bound in with what we believe we are,
in terms of our ethnic, national, religious and cultural identity?” (p. ix).
As teachers (one of the distributors of knowledge), if what we are and
who we are as individuals is linked strongly to what we know, then it must
be crucial to examine what is it that we define as key knowledge within
our culture. Within England, the structure of knowledge and the selection
of what is considered to be true knowledge is visible in the National
Curriculum (NC) and other institutional structures such as colleges (and
their courses) and universities (and their degree programmes).
All curricular systems are a selection from the
vastness of human knowledge. What humanity
knows and what it thinks it knows has been
amassed, revised and refined across many
centuries.
(Coulby, 2000, p. 12)
Therefore a reflection of accepted knowledge has been selected within our
system. The NC for all state primary schools focuses on the following
areas of knowledge – mathematics, science, English, art and design,
geography, history, physical education, information and communication
technology, religious education, music, design and technology, and
personal social health and citizenship education.
England has been influenced in the selection of this path towards
curriculum-based knowledge by societal attitudes towards the systems in
place for the acquisition of knowledge. According to Apple (1979),
institutions such as schools and colleges are there for cultural
reproduction. He goes on to further argue that “The dominant fact of our
39
current social order is the central role that capital, wealth, and economic
power play in it” (p. 69).
Furthermore, knowledge is “cultural capital” (Apple, 1979, p. 2). By
defining the knowledge that everyone is expected to have, schools confer
special status on that knowledge which is important to dominate interests.
This is the key point in defining cultural knowledge. Though there are vast
areas in human knowledge and understanding within this culture, there
are only some small aspects that are valued and therefore expanded.
However the wider question of knowledge dissemination through the Web
is one which needs to be considered as it has almost eliminated the
societal selection of knowledge for dissemination, as it allows the freedom
to any individual to share and gain any knowledge. Though this area is
fascinating, it requires more investigation than the scope of this thesis.
A good barometer for the areas of knowledge valued within the UK are the
statistics for applications into different university courses, as these
indicate what is popular within the culture and will go some way to
defining what we within the UK value as knowledge. In 2011, there were
122,787 applicants for a law course in contrast to the 13 applicants for
Classical Greek studies (UCAS, 2011). Also there were 97,055 applicants
for psychology and only 13 for Portuguese studies. From these figures and
the shape of the current curriculum, it seems that, as a society, we place
higher value on studying areas that have a practical application and lead
to a well-defined career path. Society is choosing to acquire knowledge in
areas in which the knowledge that it gains has practical and financial
value. The number of applicants to non-applied courses is far lower (e.g.
40
physics – 24,046, zoology – 9,991). We seem not to value knowledge for
knowledge’s sake, but more for the output that can be achieved. This is
supported by the UCAS (The Universities and Colleges Admissions
Service) figures for 2011.
This notion of knowledge is in contradiction to the previous section
(Section 2.5.1.1) which takes the theoretical view of knowledge being all
aspects of human life and all its variations. The cultural response is to
limit the knowledge which is explored and to place value on only a very
limited area of human subject knowledge and not the wider view of
knowledge.
In conclusion, English society and culture does put a high value on some
aspects of knowledge, and this importance is perpetuated through
institutions and other such structures. Therefore when considering
knowledge from this perspective, we need to bear in mind the value
placed on each area as these are what the culture regards as knowledge,
which in the case of England is reflected in the content of the various
curricula in place. So for this thesis, I will consider subject knowledge in
terms of the NC as this is what affects the age range of my research
group. This is in no way denying the vastness of knowledge and what I
have left out, it is simply narrowing the parameters for the search in
understanding prior knowledge.
2.5.1.3 Educational Perspective
The previous section (Section 2.5.1.2) illustrates that what has been
chosen for teaching within school is a reflection of societal and cultural
41
views of knowledge. Therefore I need to examine the curriculum and how
knowledge is structured within the formal educational setting of a school.
The National Curriculum (NC) influences what knowledge is important and
at what stage of a child’s development should this knowledge be taught
and assessed. Within our society, we deal with knowledge in a very
fragmented and hierarchical manner, and this is no different within the
structures of the NC. The NC views knowledge as being only subject-
specific knowledge and not the wider view of knowledge. Hence any
references to knowledge in the context of the NC refer only to subject
knowledge. Furthermore, there is guidance on the progress children
should be making and the levels they should have reached within the
process of knowledge acquisition.
The curriculum is developed further up the school hierarchy with deeper
content in the same areas. Irrespective of whether there is agreement
within the educational institution on these areas of knowledge or even the
structure with which it is implemented, it is a mandatory requirement of
the state.
In England and Wales, a centralised National
Curriculum has been rigorously enforced,
specifying in minute detail what is to be covered
in primary and secondary schools.
(Coulby, 2000, p. 17)
With the NC being key to the educational perspective, it would seem of
little benefit to move away from it when considering the knowledge which
is being assessed within children, irrespective of whether one agrees with
the NC. Also though the debate of what other knowledge would be better
42
covered in school is an interesting one, it serves no purpose for this
thesis. However in the next section (Section 2.5.1.4) I am going to
consider what is being covered within the current framework in
mathematics as this has an impact upon prior knowledge in the
classroom.
The final question remains – if the NC is a small aspect of all human
knowledge, what else is there that is not considered? Furthermore, what
implication does this have on our understanding of the individual and the
knowledge that is present in these settings? Plato offers a possible
method for looking at what knowledge is and how different types of
knowledge are linked to each other (Lavine, 1984). In his divided line
model (Figure 2.1), there is a distinction between the visible or sensory
knowledge (object) and the invisible or theoretical knowledge (thought).
Though there is no implication of linear progression between each area of
understanding, there are a number of similarities between Plato’s (Lavine,
1984) views about the structure of the world and Piaget’s (Piaget, 1954)
view of understanding and knowledge. For example, Plato believes that
awareness of images is the lowest form of knowledge (Lavine, 1984);
Piaget (1954), in his theory of cognitive development, indicates that the
sensory motor stage, which is the first stage of an individual’s
development, constitutes sensing images and the physical environment as
the start of knowledge development. Both Plato’s (Lavine, 1984) and
Piaget’s (Piaget, 1954) views support the ability to abstract and analyse
thought as being a higher level of knowledge.
43
Thought Object
Reason Higher
Dialectic Forms
Knowledge Intelligible World
Understanding
Forms of Science
(Science,
and Mathematics
Mathematics)
Belief
Things, Objects
(Perceptions)
Opinion Visible World
Shadows,
Conjecture
Images,
(Imagining)
Reflections
Figure 2.1 Plato’s Divided Line Model (Lavine, 1984, p. 32)
This is paralleled in the way in which knowledge is ordered and structured
within the NC. Overall the dominant theory within culture and society
today is that knowledge in its nature has stages and is hierarchical. Also
through the systems in place for acquiring knowledge, there is the
overarching thought that the access to this knowledge can only take place
in a predetermined order which, according to Piaget (1954), is linked with
age and has no bearing on ability or experience. Therefore the change
from the basic knowledge e.g. that 1+1 is indeed 2, to understanding the
reasons why 1+1 is 2 has many stages which are influenced by many
factors and must be passed through and, it seems, cannot be omitted.
To conclude, the educational view of knowledge is that knowledge is
ordered in interconnected stages. Furthermore, as reflected in the NC,
each facet of knowledge is linked and dependent on a previous aspect of
knowledge, and this is built up over time in the structure which is set up
44
by the curriculum. Knowledge has a defined path which can be measured
and valued. There is a start and end to knowledge within education.
2.5.1.4 Mathematical Perspective
In this section, I have taken care not to use the title mathematical
curriculum as I do not want to constrain the debate to the mathematics
which exists within the current narrowly-defined curriculum. Furthermore,
the debate needs to be much broader in order to allow the development of
a structure for prior knowledge. There are many ways to approach the
analysis of mathematical knowledge. The parameters are quite broad.
However, in order to make the points relevant, I wish to consider
mathematical knowledge within young children and look at what this
means in terms of this thesis. The key question I want to address in this
section is: what is meant by mathematics? What are the key ideas within
mathematics which form the basis of mathematical knowledge? Clemson
and Clemson (1994) propose the following areas: counting and ordering,
reasoning and proof, the triangle, zero and place value, statistics,
standard measure, and calculators and computers as being the key areas
of mathematical knowledge that must be considered.
The Cockcroft Report looks at similar areas of knowledge which should be
taught in schools. It proposes that measurement, shape and space,
graphical work, logical thinking, number and computation (e.g. place
value) are all key areas of mathematical knowledge which should be
prominent in the primary classroom (Cockcroft, 1982). These are reflected
in the current curriculum.
45
However, there is more to mathematical knowledge in these areas than
what is being taught in schools. Asimov (1991), in the foreword to A
History of Mathematics, writes, “Mathematics is a unique aspect of human
thought” (p. vii). Therefore, what is mathematical knowledge?
Virtually every philosopher who has discussed
mathematics has claimed that our knowledge of
mathematical truths is different in kind from our
knowledge of the propositions of the natural
sciences. This almost unanimous judgment
reflects two obvious features of mathematics.
For the ordinary person, as for the philosopher,
mathematics is a shining example of human
knowledge, a subject which can be used as a
standard against which claims to knowledge in
other areas can be measured.
(Kitcher, 1984, p. 3)
The common understanding that mathematical knowledge is a priori –
“mathematical apriorism” (Kitcher, 1984, p. 3) – is linked to the theories
which are debated in Section 2.5.1.1. However, the epistemological view
is debated by many and the influence upon mathematics in the past two
decades has changed the nature of the current view of mathematical
knowledge.
A growing number of scholars question the
universality, absoluteness and perfectibility of
mathematics and mathematical knowledge.
(Ernest, 1999, p. 67-68)
Although mathematical apriorism has been –
and continues to be – an extremely popular
doctrine, it has not gone completely
unquestioned.
(Kitcher, 1984, p. 4)
46
The debate, it seems, lies between the historical philosophical concept of
mathematical knowledge and the more recent context-influenced
mathematical knowledge “mathematical apriorism — mathematical
empiricism” (Kitcher, 1984, p. 4). Is it possible to have mathematical
knowledge which exists without expression and evaluation of this
knowledge? Kitcher (1984) rejects the view that mathematical knowledge
is tacit, and questions the traditional view that time does not change the
nature of mathematical knowledge which is consistent through time. It
would seem therefore that there is only one perspective – that
mathematical knowledge is an evolving process which is influenced by
historical events and also by the current context. This is in contrast to
most philosophers of mathematics.
They have supposed that, independently of the
historical process through which mathematics
has been elaborated, the individual
mathematician of the present day can
reconstruct the body of knowledge bequeathed
to us by our predecessors, achieving systematic
knowledge which does not reflect the patterns of
inference instantiated in the painful historical
process.
(Kitcher, 1984, p. 5)
Mathematical knowledge is changing due to the influence of “social
context and professional communities of mathematicians” (Ernest, 1999,
p. 68).
Their social organisation and structure is central
to the mechanisms of mathematical knowledge
generation and justification, and they are the
repositories and sites of application and
transmission of tacit and implicit knowledge.
(Ernest, 1999, p. 68)
47
So it can be concluded that mathematical knowledge is not simply a set of
truths, but a combination of truths and the interaction that humans have
with these truths which create new understanding.
The argument for including tacit ‘know how’ as
well as propositional knowledge as part of
mathematical knowledge is that it takes human
understanding, activity and experience to make
or justify mathematics.
(Ernest, 1999, p. 69)
Therefore mathematical knowledge is, in terms of this, not only the
philosophical perspective of tacit knowledge, but also the ability to apply
techniques to give solutions.
2.5.1.5 Individual Perspective
This section serves two aims – one, of summarising or concluding all the
previous sections; and the other, of looking at what all these theories and
deliberations mean in terms of individual knowledge.
So far I have considered knowledge from various key perspectives, all of
which have reached the core conclusion that knowledge is not merely
present and stable, but evolving in its nature. Furthermore, the evolution
is influenced by many factors such as:
individual experiences through one’s senses of the environment;
individual ability to make connections and filter the experience
received through the senses;
communal influences upon knowledge construction and
interpretation through the cultural and societal context;
48
the expectations of cultural communities such as schools and the
demands and values that they place upon knowledge;
the natural process of historical change. Time has an impact on the
way knowledge is interpreted, used and applied, therefore changing
philosophical views of knowledge.
What of the individual? Knowledge is not stable, and therefore an
individual’s perception of knowledge is also not stable. This section has
argued that knowledge and understanding of it has little to do with ability,
but more so to do with the nature of that knowledge and the relevance of
that knowledge in context. Furthermore, it has been argued that it is
through experience that we gain in knowledge and make sense of the
world. One could say that knowledge is a process of development for
individuals and society. It is a way by which we mark how far we have
come from our starting point. We must, however, not attempt to
categorise a path which needs to be taken and leave each individual to
create their own knowledge path to “mental grasp of the fact(s) of reality”
(Rand, 1979, p. 45). With this understanding, I am in a position to
conclude and move the discussion onto the main aspect of this thesis –
prior knowledge. The foundation has been set, and a clear and distinct
definition of knowledge established. Therefore in the next section, I will
look at the concept of prior and how this then leads to the establishment
of an understanding of prior knowledge.
49
2.5.2 Prior
Having considered in detail areas and issues concerning knowledge and its
definition in Section 2.5.1, I am now at the stage where I can focus on
defining prior and then how prior and knowledge meld together and
develop the ideas for prior knowledge. Common definitions of the word
prior allude to events in the past. Therefore any definition I consider must
allow for this passing of time and the effects it has on the knowledge
being gained. Furthermore something that is prior has already taken place
and is part of the individual’s reality as a consequence of their experience
of it. Though it is not yet clear if this is conscious or subconscious, it is an
act which is already completed or an experience which an individual has
already passed through.
2.5.3 Prior Knowledge Research
Having defined prior (Section 2.5.2) and knowledge (Section 2.5.1), the
ideas in those sections lead me to begin defining prior knowledge. Prior
knowledge is a vast term with many interpretations. The literature search
I carried out for this thesis revealed some interesting outcomes. When
searching through the various academic literature databases, I used the
terms prior knowledge or prior experiential learning or prior learning as a
way of getting a broad base (as established in Section 2.2.2). Looking at
the type of publications that they appear in, 4173 sources were located
which consider these terms as part of their research. Very few of these
sources give any definition or structure to what they mean by prior
50
knowledge in terms of their research. The literature fell into ten broad
categories of studies that use prior knowledge as their central tenet:
1. Accrediting applicants / students in higher education for their prior
learning / knowledge, be this formal or informal learning, and
various methods to be able to carry out this process formally
(around 5% of the literature located).
2. Subject-specific research and what causes variation in individuals’
ability to understand the subject matter with prior subject
knowledge being one of the many variables considered (around
10% of the literature located).
3. Expert versus novice debate, factors which influence the process of
becoming an expert, and differences between experts and novices
based on many factors including prior knowledge (around 5% of the
literature located).
4. Effects of prior knowledge on learning and performance, with some
of them linking what students know about one area to what is being
taught (around 33% of the literature located).
5. How prior knowledge is used in learning and the learning
experiences it forms (around 20% of the literature located).
6. Pre-service teachers and the effect prior knowledge has on their
choice of methods to teach (around 7% of the literature located).
7. Prior knowledge as a general factor in learning (around 1% of the
literature located).
8. Prior knowledge as a specific part of the learning process (around
14% of the literature located).
51
9. Evaluation of prior knowledge as an entity on its own (around 4%
of the literature located).
10. Defining prior knowledge (around 1% of the literature located).
This very simple survey illustrates that though there is wide acceptance of
the key role that prior knowledge plays in learning and knowledge
acquisition, there is little understanding or agreed vocabulary which
defines prior knowledge in any given context. This raises many questions
about the understanding we have of prior knowledge.
Using some of these areas of research as a starting point, I am going to
tease out what they offer my understanding of prior knowledge. Though
these areas are not directly related to the area of my research in terms of
their context, they will aid in informing the understanding of my work, and
hopefully they will start to shed some light on many alternative views to
the understanding and the questions I am asking.
In the remainder of this section, I examine in detail only the first three
categories of studies from the list above as they had allowed me to
consider how prior knowledge is understood. The next five categories are
related to the effect of prior knowledge on learning and therefore do not
consider what is prior knowledge. The last two categories have ideas to
offer which run throughout my literature review, and therefore are
integrated into the examination of the first three categories.
2.5.3.1 Accreditation of Prior Learning
An area of research which has prior knowledge as a key concept for their
studies considers the complex issues which need resolving when trying to
52
accredit students for their prior knowledge or prior learning in higher
education. There are many researchers who have looked at the process of
accrediting students in higher education for the knowledge, skills and
understanding they bring to their studies. It is interesting to consider how
the assessments may take place as it will offer me a process by which I
can view what their criteria are and understand what may constitute prior
knowledge. Approximately 200 sources were identified, and many
common themes emerge from them. Having analysed many of the
sources, the similarities are striking. Therefore it is of little value to review
all of the literature. It is much more valuable to assess a small key sample
from the overall pool and pull out from them some fundamental principles
which can then guide our understanding and develop thought.
Researchers such as Dochy, Segers & Buehl (1999); Taber (2001); Ajello
& Belardi (2002); O'Donnell, Dansereau & Hall (2002); Starr-Glass (2002)
and Spencer (2005) are all concerned with developing tools to assess the
knowledge gained by individuals outside the formal contexts of an
educational setting in a variety of subject areas by asking key questions
such as “how to make learning, which takes place outside the context of
formal education and training institutions, more ‘visible’” (Ajello & Belardi,
2002).
Interestingly all the sources in this area talk about prior learning and not
prior knowledge, hence giving me my first semantic stumbling block. Prior
learning is the skill and knowledge acquired from previous experience –
formal or informal. There is much ambiguity in the research about what is
meant by prior learning. However learning implies change, and knowledge
as established in Section 2.5.1 is the process by which experiences and
53
understanding of the world are ordered. Therefore the relationship
between knowledge and learning is crucial to the understanding of prior
learning. Learning is the process by which we gain knowledge, with old
learning being prior knowledge. According to Harris (2000), Kolb defines
learning as “Experience + reflection = learning”.
Therefore the gaining of knowledge is based on experience and reflection.
This simple notion helps widen the definition of prior knowledge. Surely
for an individual to have any knowledge, there must be some prior
learning and if learning in relation to Kolb’s definition is situated in
experience which has taken place in context, then all learning will
eventually be prior knowledge. Therefore this, to some extent, gives me
an understanding about the nature of gaining prior knowledge. The
learning process is an active one, and it is this that aids in the use of prior
knowledge and also helps develop our prior knowledge. Sotto (1994)
explains this link between prior learning and prior knowledge; when he
discusses perception and learning, he asks “But how is it possible for a
drawing to be recognizable as two so very different things?” (p. 68).
It is this interplay between perception, reflection and inference that allows
the development of prior knowledge, that is to say, learning which
changes into knowledge (the structure upon which new learning will be
based). The above groups of researchers have established that prior
learning and factors which affect it are key to prior knowledge. Therefore
prior learning and prior knowledge can be considered to be synonymous.
Another theme which has occurred throughout this group of researchers is
the notion of what knowledge is worth. The ability to measure learning in
54
terms of use value of certain knowledge is complex and arbitrary (Briton,
Gereluk & Spencer, 1998). Briton et al. (1998) argue that it is the
exchange value of knowledge that is important. That is to say, when
considering prior knowledge as a factor in the ability to achieve success in
any given area, one of the key features of prior knowledge has to be the
ability to transfer understanding from one situation or context (in which
the gaining of knowledge/learning has taken place) and apply it to
another. Though I am not concerned with giving formal values for prior
knowledge in my study, it is vital to know that only transferable
knowledge is valuable in the context of impacting upon a child’s ability to
perform, and for any knowledge to grow and develop, it must be able to
evolve through transfer.
Evans (2002) argues that knowledge, and more so knowledge individuals
bring to new situations (prior knowledge), in its widest sense has both
tacit and explicit elements. Tacit knowledge, in terms of prior knowledge,
is knowledge which is classified as intuitive knowledge, but in its infancy,
it was knowledge which was explicit and susceptible to change with
variation in context.
It is this notion of tacit prior learning and the assessment of it that covers
complex issues for the purpose of our understanding. It is not so vital to
understand how to weigh or quantify prior knowledge, but how to
recognise its existence within children in the context of doing mathematics
and the shape it has taken.
The final theme that Spencer (2005); Evans (2002); Harris (2000) and
many others have identified is the key area for understanding prior
55
knowledge acquisition as reflected experiences in context. The
constructivist school of learning considers experience as a key feature of
prior knowledge.
Learning does not originate “in the head” nor is
it a product of individual meaning-making. The
learner acts within the environment rather than
on it.
(Harris, 2000)
Therefore I argue that prior knowledge is knowledge which has been
acquired through interaction with many different settings and is
contextually situated. To summarise, the following have been the key
concepts so far:
The relationship between learning and knowledge is intertwined in such a
way that it is difficult to differentiate what is learning and when this has
turned into knowledge. Furthermore debating the difference between prior
learning and prior knowledge is semantic as both have the same
characteristics. If an individual has learnt something, it is a fair
assumption that they have knowledge of the said something.
The value of any particular type of knowledge has also become
established. It has been established that all prior knowledge has equal
value. However usefulness in or transferability to any given context is key
to prior knowledge. Therefore when concerned with prior knowledge in
mathematics, it is crucial that all prior knowledge is considered as useful
as long as it is transferable and useful to children in carrying out
mathematical tasks.
56
The final outcome is the essential factor of context. All knowledge is
gained in a context and is shaped by the context in which it was first
acquired. Therefore when considering prior knowledge, what will be visible
is the whole knowledge base of an individual in a given context, and what
will determine its impact on individual’s learning is the ability for each
facet of knowledge, no matter where it was gained, to be transferred into
new situations and aid in further knowledge acquisition. That is to say, an
individual will bring to bear all their prior knowledge in any given situation
and only the facets that are useful will be used to understand and gain
new knowledge.
2.5.3.2 Subject-specific Research
Another area of research into prior knowledge is subject-specific research
i.e. research which considers what causes variations in student’s ability to
understand particular subject matter, with many studies focusing on prior
knowledge as one of the factors contributing to this variation. Hazel,
Prosser & Trigwell (2002) consider methods by which meaningful learning
can occur. Furthermore they consider work by Ausubel and Novak and
give me the starting point for considering this group of research.
57
In addition to what students know and learn,
how they learn has proved crucial in
contributing to our understanding of the
pathway to high quality learning outcomes. ...
Prior knowledge had both a direct and an
indirect impact on post knowledge and at this
level there were differences across contexts.
The limitations of this research were that it
included learning strategies but not learning
intentions which are considered to be a part of a
learning approach, and that propositional but
not experiential knowledge was tapped.
(Hazel et al., 2002, p. 738-739)
The question which needs to be considered is – why does prior knowledge
have such a powerful influence upon students’ understanding of any
subject? What does prior knowledge provide to the process of developing
new knowledge? These are some of the questions addressed by this area
of research. I examine how this area of research approaches the concept
of prior knowledge. What do they mean by prior knowledge? Before
considering these key questions, I took a step back and looked at two
main researchers in this area – Ausubel and Novak – as they seemed to
offer the seeds from which much thinking in this area has developed. It
must be noted that much of the debate is subject-specific to science and
the learning of abstract concepts within science. Ausubel and Novak
express views which oppose the dominant Piagetian perspective on
learning.
The past decade has witnessed a controversy
between the Ausubelian and Piagetian science
educators regarding the relative importance of
prior knowledge and formal reasoning ability in
students' understanding of abstract concepts
and hence for their achievement of these
concepts. Joseph Novak, one of the strongest
advocates of Ausubel's postulates, claims that
children who lack formal thought may acquire
58
some abstract concepts so long as they possess
the relevant background knowledge.
(Zeitoun, 1989, p. 227)
The central tenet of Ausubel's theory is that knowledge is organised
hierarchically. New knowledge is linked, anchored, attached to existing
knowledge and is meaningful. Ausubel’s views do not agree with rote or
repetitive learning, or even discovery learning (Ausubel, Novak &
Hanesian, 1978). The relationship between prior knowledge and learning
in Ausubel’s view is that “prior knowledge influences the process whereby
this learning occurs” (West & Fensham, 1974, p. 62).
And by this learning, it implies further learning. As with many other
researchers, Ausubel’s view of prior knowledge is that it plays an essential
role in any meaningful learning. The other factor to take from his research
is this view of meaningful learning. Ausubel defines different types of
learning.
Ausubel distinguishes between ‘rote’ and
‘meaningful’ learning and postulates that
meaningful learning occurs when the learner’s
appropriate existing knowledge interacts with
the new learning. Rote learning of the new
knowledge occurs when no such interaction
takes place. The distinction is not simply a
dichotomy. Rote learning is the lower end of the
meaningful learning continuum. Depending on
the nature of the learner’s existing knowledge
and how it interacts with the new knowledge so
there will be varying degrees of meaningful
learning. Ausubel calls those aspects of existing
knowledge that can provide these interactions of
meaningful learning, ‘subsumers’.
(West & Fensham, 1974, p. 63)
59
This notion of subsumers is key to the development of learning, and is an
element of an individual’s cognitive structure. The difference between a
competent student and a poor student in terms of Ausubel’s theory is the
degree and depth of prior knowledge.
Meaningfulness is best judged by the number of
associations possible in a given piece of
information – the richer the associations, the
quicker the learning and the slower the
forgetting.
(Brightman, 1982, p. 217)
Therefore it would seem that the thoughts offered by Ausubel are crucial
to the development of this idea of prior knowledge being a pre-requisite
for the development of new learning.
A subsumer is any concept, principle or
generalising idea that the learner already
knows.
(West & Fensham, 1974, p. 63)
Therefore Ausubel offers to the definition of prior knowledge that it is any
knowledge “that can provide association or anchorage for various
components of new knowledge” (West & Fensham, 1974, p. 63).
This in many ways links back to the Piagetian idea of construction of
knowledge. Both Ausubel and Piaget view prior knowledge and its use as
an active process in learning. For Ausubel, prior knowledge not only has a
role to play in learning but can be changed in its behaviour, and the view
that prior knowledge acts as subsumer is key to new learning.
Novak further explores this notion of the role of prior knowledge as
subsumers in the efficiency of learning. He takes ideas presented by
60
Ausubel and, still in the area of science education, investigates further this
idea of how prior knowledge is constructed and influences the process of
new learning. Novak developed the ideas of concept mapping which linked
directly to Ausubel’s ideas of all knowledge needing a hook upon which to
hang new concepts. When looking at Novak’s work, this idea of
meaningful learning occurs in his thinking as well and links to the need to
have prior knowledge which is relevant to what is being learnt.
Concept maps are graphical tools for organising
and representing relationships between concepts
indicated by a connecting line linking two
concepts.
(Novak & Canas, 2007, p. 29)
The notion of concept maps links with the Ausubel theory of cognitive
structures. It is this framework of complex links which enables individuals
to learn further. Both Ausubel and Novak have looked at how new
knowledge is developed and have added the value of prior knowledge in
this process, but only Novak has, to some extent, proposed a plan of prior
knowledge and how this is structured in any individual. However he still
falls short of a definition of prior knowledge. Though he touches on how
this prior knowledge is formed through the theories of child development,
he has not looked at the factors which influence the structure of this map
which in essence is a map of prior knowledge. In his paper with Canas
(2007), there is some inspection of the psychological basis of concept
maps which gives me some idea of how they are formulated and some
clue about the factors which cause the variations in individuals. These are:
the concepts acquired between birth and three; the discovery learning
61
process; and the use of language after three to create new concepts and
understanding.
The chain reaction between these factors is clear e.g. if a child has not
gained a breadth of experience in the discovery learning process stage,
then it will limit the child’s ability to use language to ask questions to aid
discovery of new more complex concepts. There again Ausubel’s notion of
meaningful learning is key as it is this present learning which will open the
paths for future learning, and this requires three conditions:
1. The material to be learned must be
conceptually clear and presented with language
and examples relatable to the learner’s prior
knowledge
2. The learner must possess relevant prior
knowledge
3. The learner must choose to learn
meaningfully
(Novak & Canas, 2007, p. 30)
So how does this inform my definition of prior knowledge? It presents me
with the start of a structure for prior knowledge based on these studies
and the key characteristics of prior knowledge. Ausubel and Novak,
through their studies in science education, have started to offer a
structure for prior knowledge and the causes for variation in this structure
within individuals. This helps me to understand that prior knowledge will
not be formulated or look the same across individuals. One thing I must
contend with in my research is for the vast variety in the structure of prior
knowledge. Also that present knowledge is future prior knowledge and this
can be influenced in the ways it is acquired. This leaves education with the
62
potential for great achievements and changes in future development of
understanding.
2.5.3.3 Novice vs. Expert
There are many researchers who have looked at the area of novices
versus experts (Chi, Glaser & Farr, 1988; Schmidt et al., 1989; Schneider,
Körkel & Weinert, 1989; Shrager & Mayer, 1989; Haenggi & Perfetti,
1992; Kaplan & Murphy, 2000). This links to my initial reason and
curiosity for considering this research – understanding individual
differences. They have all used various contexts to assess what makes an
expert and a novice, and examined why an expert is more competent than
a novice. This understanding and the ideas it explores has input to offer to
my research. What does the development process of an expert do to prior
knowledge and how is it influenced by prior knowledge? The deviation
from the norm that experts display raises the question of how and what
are the factors which have influenced their development into experts.
More importantly, what is the role of prior knowledge? Chi et al. (1988), in
their review of experts and their characteristics, give me a historical
perspective about the development in understanding of experts and
novices.
They point to research carried out in the area of artificial intelligence and
how this has enhanced understanding of what constitutes as an expert,
and the factors that contribute to the creation of an expert. The outcome
of all this research is a move away from power-based strategy which
performs vast searches in order to achieve tasks efficiently to knowledge-
63
based systems which are concentrated on the knowledge that underlies
human expertise (Chi et al., 1988).
Knowledge-based systems are developed in domain-specific areas based
on emulation of the knowledge which the expert possesses. The need to
fill the gaps in knowledge in order to build systems has led to a vast
amount of research into what develops expertise and the nature of
experts. Chi et al. (1988) give six characteristics of an expert:
experts excel mainly in their own domains;
experts perceive large meaningful patterns in their domain (have a
greater number of connections in their knowledge);
experts are fast, they are faster than novices at performing the
skills of their domain, and they quickly solve problems with little
error;
experts have superior short-term and long-term memory;
experts see and represent a problem in their domain at deeper
(more principled) level than novices; novices tend to represent a
problem at a superficial level;
experts spend a great deal of time analysing a problem
qualitatively.
It is the domain knowledge that allows the expert to be so. From the
perspective of prior knowledge, the process of developing expertise allows
for crossover of knowledge and interlinking of information. To summarise
therefore, an expert has not only spent a vast amount of time building
knowledge in a particular area, s/he also has the ability to apply that
knowledge in a wide variety of ways. Furthermore, an expert is able to
64
access the knowledge in many unrelated ways in order to solve different
problems in a given area. It is this crossover that Chi et al.’s (1988)
research emphasises in knowledge that creates an expert. That is to say,
the greater the interaction of experience in a given area, the greater the
chances of being an expert. Anderson (1995) offers three stages for the
acquisition of skills – cognitive, associative and autonomous stages.
Chi et al. (1988) and Anderson (1995) offer key elements in
understanding the role and effect of prior knowledge in experts and what
makes an expert. An expert has ordered knowledge in many different
ways to allow for not only quick retrieval, but also to make many links
between different relationships. Anderson (1995) goes on to consider the
effects of practice on experts’ movement from one stage to the next.
Practice allows the formulation of connections and aids the learner to
move from the cognitive to the associative stage. This is the stage where
prior knowledge has its main effect and change.
The connections among the various elements
required for successful performance are
strengthened.
(Anderson, 1995, p. 274)
This is the case in mathematics. The ability to connect with great speed
differs with areas of understanding. Bugelski (1962) states that time
spent studying content, sometimes referred to as total time on task by
classroom researchers, is a good predictor of learning.
The question is why – what is it about repetition and practice that
develops a novice into an expert? The key difference, it would seem,
65
between the novice and the expert is the wide variety of experiences that
an individual has been exposed to in a related area. It is this constant and
varied interaction that individuals have between what they have
experienced and what they have learnt. Effective practice is the key to
expertise development. Practice influences prior knowledge in many ways.
Hayes (1985) found that no one reaches genius levels of performance
without at least ten years of practice.
When looking at the transition from one area of skill to the next, the key
role that practice plays in shaping prior knowledge is highlighted. Practice
or rehearsal is the key to moving from one stage to the next.
To summarise therefore, the major differences between a novice’s and an
expert’s prior knowledge are:
novice’s structure of knowledge is not ordered for quick retrieval;
expert’s structure of knowledge is not only ordered for quick
retrieval, but also has many overlaps and interconnections;
the practice carried out by an expert allows this structuring of
knowledge and fine tuning of how to approach a problem based on
experience and to find the shortest worked-out route to a solution;
experts are more flexible in their use of knowledge and can find
usefulness in many contexts as they are using their knowledge in a
given area in many ways;
the ability to abstract is greater in an expert due to practice.
Overall, an expert’s prior knowledge is shaped differently in relation to a
given domain.
66
2.5.4 Summary
This leaves me with the difficult task of conceptualising what I mean by
prior knowledge, especially as this has not been defined in its entirety in
any literature. From the literature that I have just reviewed, I can
establish a fragmented framework for prior knowledge. Knowledge, and to
some extent prior knowledge, is the whole of a person’s actual knowledge
that is:
available before a certain learning task (Hume, 2010);
transferable (Briton, Gereluk & Spencer, 1998);
structured in schemata (Clemson & Clemson, 1994);
declarative and procedural (Anderson, 1995);
partly explicit and partly tacit i.e. internalised and intuitive (Ernest,
1999; Evans, 2002);
dynamic in nature i.e. it is not a quantity but an ever-changing
pattern of connections made through different experiences (Kant,
2010);
stored in their knowledge base (Ajello & Belardi, 2002);
contextually situated (Harris, 2000);
subject-knowledge forming a subset of prior knowledge (Zeitoun,
1989).
The above understanding is synthesised from the reviewed literature and
highlights the gap within this body of literature, that of a clear and
definitive definition of prior knowledge. If I take the key points discussed
67
so far in order to assist with developing my own definition, I come up with
the following:
Prior knowledge is the experiential framework
which has brought an individual to the level of
knowledge at which they are at present.
Exploring this definition, it is what has gone by and where the individual is
at in their knowledge and understanding as a consequence of their life
journey to date. It is, in effect, a roadmap for each individual which shows
the cause and effect relationship which an individual has with their
knowledge.
2.6 Why Look at Prior Knowledge?
At this juncture in the research, I feel that it is important to question the
value of prior knowledge. Why consider prior knowledge as an area that
requires any investigation? Does it have anything to offer to education? In
order to do this, an examination of the effects that prior knowledge has on
learning and learning mathematics will offer me a valuable insight into
why it is an area worth researching. Therefore this section will consider
the following questions:
What is the effect of prior knowledge on children’s learning?
What is the effect of prior knowledge on children’s learning of
mathematics?
The information gathered from reviewing literature to address the
questions above should allow me to give justification to the value of
considering prior knowledge in the process of educating children.
68
2.6.1 Effects of Prior Knowledge on Learning
Within education, prior knowledge is recognised as a key element in the
process of new learning.
The most important single factor influencing
learning is what the learner already knows.
(Ausubel et al., 1978, p. iv)
Bartlett’s (1932) proposal of schema theory recognised the contribution of
prior knowledge in the construction of new learning. Dochy et al.’s (1999,
p. 145) review highlights many researchers who have considered the
value of prior knowledge and its effects on learning (Alexander et al.,
1994; Bjorklund, 1985; Chi & Ceci, 1987; Chi et al., 1988; Dochy, 1992;
Glaser, 1984; Glaser, Lesgold & Lajoie, 1987; Pressley & McCormick,
1995; Schneider & Pressley, 1989). Indeed much of the research I found
concluded that prior knowledge has an effect on learning and
performance. This illustrates the importance that the education process
puts upon prior knowledge.
A well-organised and coherent knowledge base
initiates inference, conceptualization and the
acquisition of principled understanding.
(Glaser & De Corte, 1992, p. 1)
Many researchers look at the impact of prior knowledge on performance.
Dochy summarises Lodewijks’ work as:
69
This involves a tripartite assumption i.e.
prior knowledge is a very important variable in
educational psychology;
the degree (content and degree of organization)
of prior knowledge of a student must be familiar
or measurable for the achievement of optimal
learning;
a learning situation is optimal to the degree to
which it is in accord to the level of prior
knowledge.
(Dochy, 1992, p. 23)
While researchers and teachers are unanimous in agreeing that prior
knowledge has an effect on learning, what type of effect does prior
knowledge have on this process? In the remainder of this section, I
examine whether the effects are positive or negative. Though there is
data available on the different effects of prior knowledge on the learning
process, for my thesis it is of greater value to consider whether these
effects are positive or negative. I am not going to consider how does prior
knowledge affect learning and performance as this adds little to
establishing my goal in this thesis of understanding the structure of prior
knowledge.
Several studies demonstrate that prior knowledge is potentially an
important variable contributing to the explanation of post-test variance
(Bloom, 1976; Dochy, 1992; Tobias, 1994). Bloom (1976) offers
quantitative data which claims correlation of 0.50 to 0.90 between pre-
test and post-test results. Dochy (1992) found that up to 42% of test
variance can be attributed to prior knowledge. There are several different
figures available on the variations of performance due to prior knowledge
70
as a variable in the testing process e.g. Tobias (1994) 30-60%. When
considering these results, Dochy (1992) points out that one must consider
other factors such as the environment within which the data are collected.
However even with a reduction in percentage of variation due to other
factors, he argues
The results of these investigations reveal that
prior knowledge generally explains a
considerable amount of the variance in
performance.
(Dochy et al., 1999, p. 155)
Resnick (1981) reviews various papers in her research in order to
understand how to instruct better, looking at areas of reading,
mathematics, science and problem solving. She notes that variation within
reading from many researchers such as Voss “have shown that individuals
with high prior knowledge of a topic remember more propositions from a
text on that topic” (p. 669).
The quantitative data concur with some of the anecdotal data that prior
knowledge explains the variability in learning and performance outcome.
Other studies consider variables such as motivation, quantity and quality
of instruction (Parkerson, Lomax, Schiller & Walberg, 1984). Along with
other factors such as peer groups which influence achievement, Parkerson
et al. (1984) found that prior knowledge still influences achievement by
0.72 which is the greatest impact on achievement from all the factors.
Therefore, based on the above review, it is a fair conclusion that prior
knowledge has an overall positive effect on learning and performance.
71
On the other hand, Dochy et al. (1999) have located eleven studies which
have found a negative effect or no effect of prior knowledge. However
they conclude that due to methodology, these are fundamentally flawed in
their results and therefore should have little value placed upon them. Also
the simple fact that there are so few studies even reporting negative
effects on learning due to prior knowledge limits the weight we put on
this. The key question which was also addressed in this paper, which does
need to be noted, is that if flawed methodology can produce negative
results, can the same be the case for positive results?
Overall, we conclude that only four studies used
weak assessment methods. ... There is a strong
relationship between prior knowledge and
performance.
(Dochy et al., 1999, p. 168)
2.6.2 Effects of Prior Knowledge on Learning of
Mathematics
In order to understand the effects of prior knowledge on learning of
mathematics, I am going to consider the body of research which focuses
on studying the impact of prior knowledge in the mathematics classroom
setting. This research is organised into different areas, many of which are
subject-specific. For example, Thompson (1995) looks at pre-number
activities and the early number curriculum, and Marshall (1993) considers
understanding of rational numbers through a schema-based approach.
However, though these are interesting, they give me little understanding
of the direct effect of prior knowledge on children’s mathematics and also
add little to the scope of my thesis. I am interested in looking at what
72
children bring to the learning experience and how this affects their ability
to perform mathematical tasks, not just prior subject knowledge, but the
wider idea of prior knowledge. Considering research that focuses on
children’s informal mathematics in the early years will allow me to explore
the prior knowledge which children have before they come to the
classroom that affects children’s learning of mathematics.
There is a great deal of research carried out on the informal mathematics
that children bring to the classroom (Atkinson, 1992; Ausubel et al.,
1978; Baldwin & Stecher, 1925; Baroody, 1987; Dickson, Brown &
Gibson, 1984; Donaldson, 1989; Gelman, 1980; Groen & Resnick, 1977;
Haylock & Cockburn, 1989; Hughes, 1986; Lave, Murtaugh & de la Rocha,
1984; Resnick & Ford, 1981; Skemp, 1987; Starkey & Gelman, 1982;
Tizard & Hughes, 1984). It is worth considering a few of the themes that
this area of research has considered. The research falls into the following
eight characteristics.
i. The developmental theories which consider what young children
know about mathematical concepts.
ii. Research which looks at the process/facts surrounding bridging the
gap between school formal and home informal.
iii. Research which explores the learning which takes place in the
informal setting and its effects on school learning.
iv. Research which considers using informal settings in the classroom
to encourage mathematics.
v. How children learn mathematics.
73
vi. Looking at the mathematics curriculum and what it offers to the
learning process.
vii. Assessment of informal knowledge in mathematics.
viii. Looking at the informal understanding of written symbols in
mathematics.
The wide body of research considers small aspects of what children bring
in terms of subject knowledge to the formal learning experiences, but
does not look at the wider prior knowledge framework that children bring
to bear upon the learning of mathematics. Though it is agreed and
recognised that prior knowledge has an effect, there is no understanding
of how or what this effect is. I argue that this is due to lack of
comprehensive understanding of what is meant by prior knowledge and a
clear definition for it. It is also interesting that this lack of clarity is linked
to the plethora of ideas about how children learn.
There is no single comprehensive theory that
explains how children develop intellectually or
how they learn.
(Clemson & Clemson, 1994, p. 4)
Prior knowledge also forces a theoretical shift to
viewing learning as “conceptual change”. ... it is
impossible to learn without prior knowledge ...
there is widespread agreement that prior
knowledge influences learning, and that learners
construct concepts from prior knowledge.
(Roschelle, 1995)
I would contest that we cannot begin to use prior knowledge effectively if
we do not know what it is and therefore need to define it, and therein lies
74
the gap. However this still leaves unanswered the effects of prior
knowledge on mathematics.
Wakeley (2002) investigates the relationship between low birth weight
and mathematical development. She concludes that the achievement of
lower scores in mathematical tasks is related to support from home. This
is because support from home for early mathematics development
overrides factors such as birth weight and health.
This leads me back to considering the informal mathematics that children
learn and carry out as being the lynchpin to new understanding. There is
focus on informal mathematics that children engage in and how this leads
to learning formal mathematics.
Before entrance to school, children possess
important concepts and skills concerning
mathematics.
(Ginsburg, 1989, p. 20)
This understanding of numbers is based on experiences the children have
had. It is this idea of informal mathematics and its acquisition which forms
the prior knowledge for future mathematics learning. What do I mean by
informal mathematics and what impact does it have on learning
mathematics? Ginsburg (1989) explores many mathematical concepts and
how they are expanded initially before being formalised. He argues that
informal knowledge is gained from different experiences of different
aspects of life. Furthermore the initial informal experience with
mathematics forms the filter for new understanding.
75
From a very young age, children build on intuitive understanding of
mathematics which is based on their environment (Ginsburg, 1989).
Through self-directed practice and errors, children are able to develop
many ideas in mathematics. It is this constructivist school of thought that
dominates the theory of mathematical learning. As stated already, it is the
powerful filter that prior knowledge forms which influences the
development of mathematical understanding.
It is interesting and worth questioning what Roschelle (1995) labelled as
the “paradox of continuity”. So far, I have made the assumption that all
prior knowledge is valid and contributes positively to new learning.
However if I use the filter analogy, it is possible that individuals have
knowledge structures that are erroneous. How then can learning
progress?
Constructivism depends on continuity, because
new knowledge is constructed from old. But how
can students construct knowledge from their
existing concepts if their existing concepts are
flawed?
(Roschelle, 1995)
In order to understand how learning can still take place within the
possibility of incomplete or inaccurate prior knowledge, I need to consider
learning theory. However as there is a common understanding of Piaget,
Vygotsky and Dewey, rather than looking at the principles of each of
these, I want to consider how they aid in allowing this incongruence in
prior knowledge and new learning to be resolved. Within each of their
theories, there is the ability for the learner to develop or change through
time.
76
To summarise therefore, how do we go from informal to formal
understanding in mathematics? Also what is the role of prior knowledge?
In this section, I have noted that prior knowledge in the form of informal
knowledge has an impact on new learning, but need to resolve how
erroneous prior knowledge can still aid new learning and the answer lies in
the major theories linked to learning, those of varied experiences and
practising the skills that are acquired. It is the application of ideas in
many different contexts that will allow the development of new
knowledge.
Piaget suggests that learners overcome the
paradox of continuity with the help of slow,
maturational processes that operate when doing
a task provokes conflict between
accommodation and assimilation, and support
for equilibration between these ... Dewey
overcomes the paradox of continuity by focusing
on the nature of experience under the right
conditions, a learner engaged with a problematic
experience can effect a transformation of prior
knowledge ... Vygotsky can overcome the
paradox of continuity by suggesting that
learning coordinates spontaneous and
specialized concepts in a gradual transformative
process.
(Roschelle, 1995)
Therefore, based on this, prior knowledge affects mathematics learning,
and it does so through maturity, social interaction, experiences, resolving
problems, and addressing contradictions. Therefore I conclude that the
building blocks of prior knowledge based on this line of enquiry are:
Experiences an individual has engaged in;
Maturity – the time that has passed; and
Social interaction an individual has engaged in.
77
These have an enormous effect upon the development of mathematical
knowledge and skills.
If prior knowledge is the informal mathematics that children bring to the
school study, then there is a labyrinth of knowledge that children have
acquired in an informal method. Ginsburg (1989) outlines what babies
already know and use to make crude judgements. He argues that this
knowledge is universal, is full of both weaknesses and strengths, and has
a complex effect on performances.
Prior knowledge has several effects on the understanding and progress of
early mathematics. Every mathematical development is dependent upon
what children bring into the learning situation. Many authors have
classified this as informal mathematics. Its effects on learning
mathematics are to:
allow children to hook new learning to old knowledge;
allow for experiential learning;
allow children to choose many different strategies to be tried out
and learned.
2.7 Teachers’ Understanding of Prior
Knowledge
In this section, I am going to consider teachers’ understanding of prior
knowledge as fundamentally it is this notion that needs clarifying in order
for teachers to be able to use prior knowledge to support children’s
78
learning. Also I will consider what teachers’ understand of children’s prior
knowledge, as this is the motivation for this research.
Throughout history, Piaget has had a great impact on how we view
children and their learning, especially in mathematics.
For some time now, Jean Piaget has been
regarded as one of the leading authorities on
the question of how children learn mathematics.
(Hughes, 1986, p. 12)
The way in which English curriculum and schools are structured is greatly
influenced by Piaget. The notion of Piaget’s age-related developmental
stages has influenced teachers’ understanding of children and their
knowledge. Teachers, to some extent, do not expect the knowledge
structure to be any different in children of similar age. The difficulty which
has been created is the lack of assessment methodology to enable
teachers to establish accurately what children’s actual prior knowledge is
in mathematics. The firm belief that each child will pass through each
stage (as suggested by Piaget) means that there is no need to understand
what children’s prior knowledge state is. In terms of prior subject
knowledge, many methods have been implemented to assess where
children are such as Assessing Pupils’ Progress (APP) and Assessment for
Learning (AfL). However this only offers limited scope for teachers
evaluating prior knowledge. The heavy dependency on structuring
learning through ages and stages has not allowed teachers to build a
picture of what individual children know and to have the ability to assess
them with accuracy.
79
Given prior knowledge’s central role in learning,
there is a surprising lack of research that
explores how teachers – pre-service and in-
service – understand the concept of prior
knowledge and make instructional decisions
based upon their understanding
(Meyer, 2004, p. 971)
The role of teachers is the facilitation of learning and, as established in the
previous sections, one of the most influential factors in the process of
learning is prior knowledge. Therefore understanding, evaluating and
effective planning for prior knowledge are essential in order to be an
effective teacher. The connection between teaching and learning is
intertwined.
Theories of teaching must be based on theories
of learning and also must have a more applied
focus.
(Ausubel et al., 1978, p. 16-17)
Furthermore, Ausubel et al. (1978) have emphasised the importance of
checking prior knowledge and using it in teaching. The inability to do this
or inaccuracy in doing this leads to lack of progress.
It is impossible for teaching to succeed if it does
not address the current forms of students’
understanding of a subject.
(Laurillard, 1993, p. 187)
Prior knowledge can have positive and/or
negative effects on learning.
(Jones, Todorova & Vargo, 2000, p. 206)
I must therefore question how much do teachers really understand prior
knowledge, and how do they use it in their teaching? Meyer (2004) looks
80
at this question in greater detail when he considers how novice and expert
teachers use prior knowledge.
In summary, for the novice teachers prior
knowledge tended to be the result of prior
teaching and could be defined by what students
formally knew about a concept. They saw it
having an important role in learning since a
teacher would want to be sure that the proper
information foundation was in place before new
learning could take place. If students had
misconceptions, then the teacher could replace
the faulty information brick with a new one
before going on in their teaching. On the other
hand, the expert teachers emphasized the role
of students’ ideas and explanations as central to
prior knowledge. Therefore, prior knowledge
was important in learning because it revealed
how students put their ideas together. If the
students had misconceptions then you have to
get them to think a new way about the concept.
(Meyer, 2004, p. 977)
So for each group of teachers, i.e. novices and experts, their
understanding of prior knowledge takes very different shapes. Meyer
(2004) goes on to look at how teachers make use of prior knowledge and
again finds a huge distinction between novice and expert teachers.
Furthermore an interesting point to be noted here is “the novice teachers’
lack of strategies for finding out their students’ prior knowledge” (Meyer,
2004, p. 977).
This could be extended to teachers who are novice not to teaching, but to
the subject matter they are being asked to teach as is often the case in
mathematics.
The understanding that teachers have of prior knowledge and their ability
to use it as a central element of teaching and planning is very much
81
dependent on their own prior knowledge of the subject, children, teaching,
school and other environmental factors. The novice teacher has a very
“superficial conception of knowledge and prior knowledge” (Meyer, 2004,
p. 980). Slightly more experienced teachers were similar to complete
novices in many ways, but “were limited in their focus and because their
own knowledge was poorly organized they interpreted the events in their
classrooms in a limited fashion” (Meyer, 2004, p. 981).
It was the expert teachers who were able to use their experience and
knowledge to focus on their students (Meyer, 2004). This notion of using
experience-based intuition allows expert teachers to be better at the
process of teaching and ensuring that their students engage in effective
learning. This review demonstrates that prior knowledge and teachers’
understanding of prior knowledge is very ad hoc and based on individual
level of experiences.
Studies have shown that the process of planning and how it is carried out
is a key indicator of teachers’ level of understanding of prior knowledge
and the constructivist learning process. Though these studies’ results are
not earth shattering, it does enable me to question the nature of this gap
between novice and expert teachers in their understanding and use of
prior knowledge. One of the key ways I can look more closely at this gap
is by looking at the teachers’ planning process. It is while planning that
teachers should and do introspect about what the teaching process for any
given lesson should constitute. The use of the planning cycle also
inculcates this process further. Yinger (1978) states that planning is part
of the preactive phase of teaching.
82
Preactive teaching takes place before and after
school, during recess, and at other times when
the teacher is alone in the classroom.
(Yinger, 1978, p. 1)
He further argues that it is in this phase that teachers are most reflective.
I would also argue that it is here that teachers can take prior knowledge
into account. So in order to understand and answer my initial question of
what do teachers understand of prior knowledge, I must examine what
goes into their planning. What factors are considered in this process as
this will inform me of the extent to which prior knowledge is understood
and, more importantly, used.
Borko and Livingston (1989) look at how mathematics planning is carried
out by expert and novice teachers. For novice teachers, as also noted by
Yinger, the planning process constitutes the following facets (Borko &
Livingston, 1989):
Strategies for the presentation of content;
No strategy for unpredictable events;
No addressing of students’ comments and questions that may occur
during teaching;
Rigidity leading to less scope for improvisation.
Yinger summarises the results of a study by Peterson, Marx and Clark
(1978) which showed the following behaviour of teachers while planning:
83
i. Teachers spent the largest portion of their
planning time on content (subject matter) to be
taught.
ii. After subject matter, teachers
concentrated their planning on instructional
processes (strategies and activities).
iii. The smallest portion of planning time was
spent on objectives.
(Yinger, 1980, p. 109-110)
Considering the three steps in planning above, it must be noted that
teachers spend most of their planning time in considering the content of
the lesson, irrespective of the children they are teaching.
Teachers engage in many levels of planning, some of which takes place
outside the ebb and flow of the classroom and some in situ. Yinger (1978)
identified five different types of plans – yearly, term, unit, weekly and
daily (p. 18). From the point of view of my research, these are five
different opportunities to account for prior knowledge in the teacher’s
teaching process. The key outcome of Yinger’s (1980) study, which is
pertinent to my discussion here, is that though teachers plan in a very
systematic way, their formal (written) plans did not contain pupils’
characteristics though they were reflected upon during the planning.
Attention to pupils' background characteristics
was evident in this teacher's planning-not in the
plans themselves, but in the planning process.
(Yinger, 1980, p. 124)
To conclude, teachers’ understanding of prior knowledge and its use in the
planning process is heavily dependent on their level of experience, and
their own prior knowledge of students, subject and possible outcomes.
84
Furthermore there are external pressures upon teachers which also
influence how they plan and use prior knowledge within the classroom.
However the overall outcome of this section is the random manner in
which prior knowledge is used by teachers at all levels in planning. The
major way in which teachers use prior knowledge and their understanding
of it is to overcome and implement as closely as possibly the complexity
and unpredictability and the immediacy of the classroom (Yinger, 1978).
That is, they use prior knowledge as a management technique and not as
a way to develop knowledge. Despite establishing that prior knowledge
plays a key role in learning, it is definitely not a key focus in planning for
learning with no formal written consideration for it in majority of teachers’
plans.
The most profound challenges for teachers are
not associated merely with acquiring new skills
but with making personal sense of
constructivism as a basis for instruction.
(Windschitl, 2002, p. 131)
Teaching then requires teachers who understand
students’ existing conceptions and can create
learning experiences that will allow students to
either accommodate or restructure their
knowledge frameworks for new learning.
(Meyer, 2004, p. 971-972)
This does not occur, and I must question why. The answer may lie in the
lack of a definition or structure of prior knowledge as identified in Section
2.5, or the lack of clarity in understanding of prior knowledge. Overall
prior knowledge is based on intuition by teachers as it is by researchers
due to the vagueness of its structure, and must be investigated.
85
2.8 Conclusion
This literature review has considered all aspects of knowledge linked with
the examination of prior knowledge. The structure of my enquiry was
determined by my research objective, which is to provide an
understanding of the structure of prior knowledge of children in the
context of the primary mathematics classroom. Firstly I have been able to
establish an explanation of how I gathered all my information. I feel that
this was essential to allow transparency. The second thing I have been
able to carry out is to give my research a context having looked at the
political, social, cultural and historical background within primary
mathematics education. This has enabled me to frame where my findings
can be placed. For other researchers, this allows an understanding of the
limitations of the findings and the context within which they have been
derived. I have also looked at the primary mathematics classroom in
order to allow a detailed picture to be framed for this key context.
I then went on in this chapter to examine knowledge in order to tease out
the many theoretical arguments and perspectives to establish what my
view was and the view that will inform the outcome of the data collection.
There are many complex possibilities as to what I mean by knowledge. I
conclude that knowledge is not stable, but it is ever changing, and
furthermore has little to do with ability, but more to do with the relevance
of knowledge to context. From this, I examined what is prior knowledge
and there the literature review falls short of providing an answer to my
research objective. By synthesising the literature, I was able to come up
with a fragmented framework for prior knowledge in Section 2.5.4, which
86
will need to be validated and extended through my empirical research. All
the literature unanimously agreed on the pivotal nature of prior
knowledge in the process of learning – there was little disagreement on
this point. Also there was overwhelmingly wide recognition of the notion
of prior knowledge and its positive effects on learning. This points to a
huge gap in literature. There is no clear definition as to what is meant by
prior knowledge in primary mathematics and teaching. This outcome is of
great surprise as prior knowledge is one of the universally accepted
pedagogical notions. Why is it that thus far there have been no attempts
to define it? I feel through the literature that this is due to the intuitive
nature of prior knowledge. It seems that this is a concept that has seeped
so deep into our intuition that though we all have our own understanding
of it, we are unable to define it. I have been able to glean some features
of it through the literature as stated in the conclusion of Section 2.5.4.
This is by no means a definitive outcome, and I will need to examine this
through field research.
I felt it was important to examine the reasons for looking at prior
knowledge and whether it really does have the value I have placed on it.
So I have done this through the point of view of learning mathematics and
teaching, and it seems that the outcome of this reflects the outcome I
established for the definition of prior knowledge. It was agreed that prior
knowledge overall has a huge effect on learning, but teachers were not
able to use it effectively as they did not have an understanding of it, but
were using it intuitively and randomly.
87
Therefore my next step is to investigate and define the real nature of prior
knowledge in the primary mathematics classroom. The following chapters
will look at the process for doing this.
88
3 IDENTIFYING RESEARCH
METHODOLOGY
3.1 Introduction
In the previous chapter, I have provided background information on areas
touched by this research. In this chapter, I explore various research
paradigms and methodologies to identify a suitable methodological
framework for the collection of data. The methodologies for analysing the
data are explored in Chapter 5.
In addition to an introduction and conclusion, this chapter has four main
sections. In Section 3.2, I reiterate the research question – understanding
children’s prior knowledge within the classroom – to provide the context
for the sections which follow.
In Section 3.3, I explore the nature of research, and describe why I have
positioned my research within the qualitative research paradigm. This
positioning and exploration of the philosophical assumptions are crucial to
the identification of a suitable research methodology. The following key
issues are considered:
an exploration of positivist and anti-positivist research paradigms;
a debate on what constitutes good research;
an examination of objectivity and subjectivity and their significance
to my research.
89
In Section 3.4, I describe the chosen methodological framework –
naturalistic research – for understanding children’s prior knowledge within
the classroom. The following key issues are considered:
an exploration of a variety of available research methodologies;
a description of naturalistic research methodology;
the overall implication of the selection of the research methodology
on understanding the data gathering process and the subsequent
analysis.
In Section 3.5, I examine the generalisability and validity of the results.
These are required to ensure that naturalistic research can stand up to
scrutiny.
3.2 Research Question
Before proceeding any further, it is useful to reiterate (as stated in
Chapter 1) the initial motivation for this research – my own experience
within the classroom. I find teaching in a primary school to be not only
rewarding but also personally challenging. The most challenging factor,
and the one which affects all areas of teaching mathematics, is the
difference evident in children’s ability to perform any given mathematical
tasks. This observed difference in children started a process of self-
questioning. Initially, there were vague questions:
Why should there be a difference in children’s
ability? What makes us each different? What is it
about the difference that affects mathematical
ability?
90
Underlying all of them was a recurring question:
What is it about who children are at any given
moment in time that makes them so different in
their ability to perform mathematical tasks?
I felt that I did not have an understanding of the process involved in
creating the differences that manifested themselves in the children.
Furthermore, as my thinking and self-questioning progressed, there were
yet more questions. Through talking to other teachers, I concluded that
this was a question for which many of them felt that they did not have a
clear answer. Additionally (as considered in Chapter 2) wider research
evidence and investigation of the thoughts of other researchers revealed
that there are no definitions, descriptions, structures or processes which
address the specific question of what accounts for the individual
differences in children’s ability within mathematics. However, these
sources did point to prior knowledge as a possible cause for the
differences. This puzzle and my desire as a teacher to somehow bring into
the realms of understanding these abstract and complex everyday notions
of prior knowledge have been pivotal to my research, and to the
identification of a suitable research methodology.
3.3 Nature of Research
Throughout this chapter, my focus is on establishing the methodology for
performing the research. A secondary objective is to present
systematically the steps that I took in arriving at this methodological
framework. This was in two phases. The first phase, discussed in this
section, focuses on the nature of research and the positioning of my
91
research among various research paradigms. This results in the second
phase, discussed in Section 3.4, which focuses on formulating the
research methodology appropriate for the selected research paradigm.
Ontologists have classified research into various paradigms. Each research
paradigm is associated with its own appropriate methodologies which lead
to their own working methods and resulting outcomes.
Research is concerned with understanding the
world and that this is informed by how we view
our world(s), what we take understanding to be,
and what we see as the purposes of
understanding.
(Cohen, Manion & Morrison, 2001, p. 3)
Focusing on this perspective, I must give consideration not only to
providing my definition of the nature of research but also to the position
that I (as the researcher) take in the process of understanding and
establishing a suitable methodology for this research. Cohen et al.’s
(2001) statement implies that it is unrealistic to be objective in research,
that is, it is not possible for researchers to provide an objective view of
the world being considered, to be detached from the research that they
are carrying out, and to report their findings without any interpretations.
This lack of detachment is further reinforced by Smith and Hodkinson
(2002):
We all make judgements and prefer some things
to other things and will continue to do so for as
far as anyone can foresee. It is, in fact,
impossible to imagine any serious concept of
personhood in the absence of judgement and
preference.
(Smith & Hodkinson, 2002, p. 293)
92
This statement implies that my views and opinions have an important role
to play in the process and nature of my research. Thus I need to state
them openly as these views and opinions not only are crucial to
understanding the methodological framework chosen, but also contribute
to its development, implementation and final outcome. Furthermore,
Edwards (2002) contends that the nature of who we are is rooted in our
cultural context, and “as learners, we try to act on a world that is not of
our own making and do so using the conceptual tools available in our
cultures” (p. 161).
Having established the importance of my views, opinions and cultural
context, I now explore various research paradigms with my voice as the
backdrop. Where essential, my views and opinions are stated clearly to
avoid any ambiguity.
Initially my exploration focuses on the positivist and the anti-positivist
paradigms, widely regarded as two dimensions for looking at human
nature. Where does my research fit?
Hitchcock and Hughes (1995, p. 21) propose that ontological assumptions
give rise to epistemological assumptions, which in turn give rise to
methodological considerations, and these in turn give rise to issues of
instrumentation and data collection. The ontological assumptions that I
make about the world around me are rooted within my personal opinions.
These opinions have been formed through various epistemological
experiences. Thus the questions asked by my research are fixed in the
nature of who I am. This, as argued in earlier chapters, is influenced by
93
the culture in which I am situated. Therefore the nature of research and
the researcher cannot be separated.
As described by Comte (Cohen et al., 2001; Turner, Beeghley & Powers,
2012), the positivist/scientific school of thought depends on, and is
structured around, the doctrine that all genuine knowledge is based on
sense experience and can only be advanced by means of observation and
experiment. This view has many implications. Firstly, it implies that
knowledge is in some way hermetically pre-packaged, just waiting to be
discovered through objective experimental research. I think that Comte
offers a limited view of the nature of knowledge as his view does not allow
for knowledge gained through the vicarious experiences of others.
Secondly, Comte’s view implies that knowledge can only be defined
through a process of observation and experimentation, leaving no
opportunity for gaining knowledge through introspection or analysis of the
experiences of others based outside their personal senses. Thirdly,
Comte’s view implies that all knowledge can be classified and has
predetermined properties. Lastly, his view implies that the reactions
towards others and towards situations that are based on an individual’s
knowledge will be the same for each individual placed in similar situations
with similar knowledge. The process of gaining knowledge in the positivist
paradigm assumes that knowledge is like the elements on the periodic
table, all having a fixed place with predictable characteristics to any
intervention to which they are subjected. It further assumes that
knowledge is structured in this preset order and is the same for all
individuals. Comte’s point of view does not allow for individual differences
and opinions based on their understanding and conceptualisation of
94
reality, nor does it consider past experiences or the level of understanding
of those experiences.
The positivist perspective views the real world to be “out there
independent of our interest in, or knowledge of, it. This is a reality that
can be known, at least in principle, as it really is.” (Smith & Hodkinson,
2002, p. 292). This implies that reality is fixed and predetermined and
external to the knower.
Though the above is an oversimplification of the beliefs and values of the
positivist paradigm, it gives some indication about the necessity for
considering and clarifying my position. However the belief that all actions,
reactions and interactions are a direct result of external influences and
would be the same for all individuals is an oversimplification of the
complex nature of humans. There is a wide variation in the interpretations
we all make from what we see and experience. Standing on the same
point, each individual will observe, feel and interpret the same view in
very different ways due to the context he/she is placed in and the
contexts available to him/her due to his/her past experiences and
knowledge. There is a sheer, though perhaps minute, distinction between
whether we view human behaviour as behaviour in response to external
influences and stimuli or whether we view it as actions in relation to what
we assess, think, and feel in conjunction with past experience and past
gained knowledge. This distinction will determine the approach and nature
of the methodology that can be implemented.
95
3.3.1 What is Good Research?
A significant hurdle in determining the methodology is to explore the
notion of what I believe is good research. The positivist research paradigm
considered so far relies on studies with a large number of subjects divided
into control and intervention groups. For most people, including myself,
the initial response when thinking about research is to focus on the
positivist paradigm. Though this is my initial belief, I am struggling to
overcome this initial positivist reaction as my sense of right and wrong in
performing any research stems not only from my internal values, but also
from the values placed upon me by my social and cultural background
(being Indian). My cultural identity places a higher value on quantitative
deductive research (positivist paradigm) as opposed to qualitative
interpretive research (anti-positivist paradigm). This cultural identity
shapes my values and makes me feel less qualified or less productive in
society if I have, in any sense, a leaning towards an interpretive outlook
on research. This strong cultural benchmark and the desire to fit this
mould as a means of self-validation helps in explaining my initial positivist
reaction. This tendency to fit in to reflect the cultural expectations of my
peers and society is also observed by Edwards (2002), who states that “as
researchers, we also interpret and respond in ways that are permitted in
our own research cultures” (p. 163).
The cultural expectation and my initial inclination to consider the positivist
research paradigm (and its accompanying research methodologies) to be
indicative of good research must be questioned in relation to the impact it
has on my research question. This amounts to a psychological tug of war
96
between what is expected culturally, and what my research question
needs. My internal values compel me to focus on the research question,
taking into account the skills and resources needed to address it rather
than dwelling on the culturally acceptable trends in methodological
approaches. In order to resolve this tug of war, I need to explore the
principles underlying the positivist and anti-positivist paradigms, namely
objectivity and subjectivity respectively.
Though the dissection of my values and the cultural influences on it seems
to be a huge indulgence for scant benefit, these experiences have shaped
and structured my prior knowledge and thoughts throughout the
progression of this research. Thus they form a key part of the exploration
of my research methodology and the derivation of its eventual structure.
This gives rise to the following questions. Is the best way to gain new
knowledge through the use of experimental methods and mathematical
deduction of the result using objective methods, or is there benefit to
observing and inductive reasoning using subjective methods? Furthermore
are the results being gained through detailed observations of interactions
within the classroom (naturalistic approach) of any less value than a
syllogistic approach?
3.3.2 Objectivity and Subjectivity
In order to evaluate and understand the values of the various research
paradigms and resolve the questions proposed at the end of the previous
section, I now consider the underlying principles of objectivity and
subjectivity, and their significance to my research. Clarifying my position
97
on these two notions will help to place my research within a suitable
methodological paradigm. The choice must be based on how the chosen
methodological framework will aid in understanding prior knowledge.
Objectivity is both a metaphysical and an
epistemological concept. It pertains to the
relationship of consciousness to existence.
Metaphysically, it is the recognition of the fact
that reality exists independent of any perceiver’s
consciousness. Epistemologically, it is the
recognition of the fact that a perceiver’s (man’s)
consciousness must acquire knowledge of reality
by certain means (reason) in accordance with
certain rules (logic).
(Rand, 1965, p. 7)
Rand’s definition of objectivity implies that reality is fixed and the same
for all within any given context. The tradition of quantitative research is
based on the principle of objectivity, and aims to discover the fixed reality
which answers the research question (Winter, 2000). Since the reality is
independent of any observer, it puts a limit on the role of individual
researchers engaged in quantitative research.
Using Rand’s definition of objectivity, a structure for prior knowledge
within the Year One classroom can be derived through observation and
deduction. If I assume that my research is located in the quantitative
research methodology, then the methodological approaches available to
achieve this are deductive and require a hypothesis to be proved, which is
not the case in my research. Also by definition, any quantitative research
is replicable and the observations performed should give the same
outcome even with a different researcher. Most quantitative scientific
98
research, notably randomised controlled trials, fall within this
methodology.
Having considered objectivity and its implication on my research question,
I now turn my attention to subjectivity.
Subjectivism is the belief that reality is not a
firm absolute, but a fluid, plastic, indeterminate
realm which can be altered, in whole or in parts,
by the consciousness of the perceiver.
(Rand, 1965, p. 7)
This definition of subjectivity allows for the fact that the world exists, but
different people construe it in very different ways. This definition
precludes certain properties inherent in objectivity such as the notion of a
single shared reality or common understanding of variations of reality and
the concepts that constitute this variation.
As soon as subjectivity is represented in any
way, linguistically or through mental imagery, it
becomes intersubjective. One gets an idea of
what another person intends or feels by
implicitly taking the position of that other
person; in other words, by implicitly sensing
what one would feel or intend oneself when
talking in a similar manner.
(Carspecken, 1996, p. 167)
This implies that there are a multitude of truths. This arises from the fact
that subjectivity is based on personal interpretations, and the truth
experienced by one person cannot be ever experienced by another person
in the same manner.
The above discussion demonstrates that objectivity and subjectivity are
opposite extremes. The objectivist outlook allows for only one truth (the
99
same truth for all). The subjectivist outlook allows for many truths and
personal interpretations and recognises the importance of an individual’s
experiences in life. However, as observed by Rand (1965), objective
knowledge, in its purest form, cannot exist.
Knowledge, man merely observes that which is.
When it comes to applying his knowledge, man
decides what he chooses to do, according to
what he has learned.
(Rand, 1965, p. 7)
The notion that man makes choices based on his observations means that
he interprets his knowledge. So if I am concerned with how individuals
respond or choose to respond to the existing reality, then the subjectivist
outlook provides some pathway forward.
As stated earlier, my research is focussed on exploring what is meant by
prior knowledge, and on investigating how prior knowledge is structured
within the classroom. The discussions in Section 3.2 helped me to identify
some of the key requirements to address my research question. These
key requirements include the ability to reflect and self monitor, and the
ability to consider meanings for complex interactions between teacher and
pupils in the context of the mathematics classroom. The methodological
approaches offered by taking a subjectivist outlook allow these
requirements to be taken into account.
The mechanisms required for understanding the structure of prior
knowledge require not only some sense of logic in their explanation, but
also a sense of setting. Jaworski (1994) proposes that, in order to provide
some validity to research using the naturalistic approach, “a researcher
100
needs to embed the research in its total situated context, and that this
includes his or her own experiences and thinking” (p. xiv).
From the perspective of my research, it is vital for me to observe how the
teacher and pupils interact with each other, and change, create and
control their reality and their understanding of reality based on the
cognitive tools that they have. The only way for me to gain this
understanding is by sharing the same frame of reference as the teacher
and pupils. This can only be achieved by embedding myself in their frame
of reference, i.e. the research context which in this case is the classroom.
Carspecken (1996) proposes the way forward for creating a common
understanding of thought.
One must believe that sense objects exist in
such a way as to be open to multiple observers
who will agree on their existence if they share
certain features of a language and a culture.
(Carspecken, 1996, p. 64)
Though Carspecken is referring to a physical reality, the argument
extends to understanding behaviour. Without a common language and a
shared frame of reference (with teachers and children), it is difficult to
assess the observations made in the classroom in relation to the research
question. The central common issue in creating complete detailed
understanding of prior knowledge is the issue of a shared cultural
reference point.
To point out that elucidation of the formal
categories of subjectivity and objectivity does
not depend upon taking a position on the
ultimate nature of objective and subjective
phenomena is not simply to skirt a difficult issue
101
… all validity claims involving objectivity and
subjectivity can be doubted in some way.
(Carspecken, 1996, p. 72-73)
Thus far, I have analysed the root notions of objectivity and subjectivity,
and their implication for my research question. The key conclusion is that
the research methodology needs to provide the ability to gain
understanding within a context that is shared by the researcher and the
researched, and supports interpretation.
3.3.3 Positioning the Research
So the question remains – now that a theoretical backdrop has been
established, where does my research situate itself? Through the debate in
the previous sections, the positivist quantitative paradigm has limited
value in gaining understanding of the complexity of the myriad of
interpretations made in a classroom and defining prior knowledge in the
classroom. There is no methodological or analytical framework within the
positivist paradigm which allows for understanding to be based on
individual interpretations made about reality as it is experienced by the
researcher. Furthermore the positivist paradigm does not account for the
ontological assumptions made so far that a multitude of realities exist due
to varying human experiences, and that the knowledge and views created
as a result of these experiences need to be understood through different
mechanisms such as observation of human behaviour.
On the other hand, the anti-positivist interpretive paradigm provides
methodological and analytical frameworks which allow for understanding
to be based on embedding myself within the research context. This
102
paradigm supports the epistemological understanding that events can be
understood through many processes of analysis and interpretation which
are rooted in context, and lead to the development of new knowledge.
This paradigm also allows me to take into account the complex
interactions between the teacher and the pupils in the context of the
mathematics classroom. Taking all of these into account, I need to situate
my research within the anti-positivist interpretive paradigm.
3.4 Choosing a Research Methodology
The debate in Section 3.3 and its subsections concluded that my research
needs to be situated within the interpretive paradigm. The methodological
framework that I have chosen is based on the key interpretive
methodology known as naturalistic research. The subsections that follow
examine its appropriateness for my research question, and highlight its
key benefits.
3.4.1 Naturalistic Research
My research question requires an approach which allows for the
ontological assumptions that all individuals have various realities with a
shared common understanding of these realities. To facilitate this shared
common understanding, the research methodology needs to provide
modes of communication and descriptions which are familiar to the groups
under observation (i.e. teachers and pupils) and common to groups with
whom the observations are shared (i.e. other researchers). The shared
common understanding of various individual realities by different
observers is known as “multiple access” (Carspecken, 1996, p. 65).
103
The naturalistic research paradigm contains various tools and techniques
that provide a wide range of options for exploring my research question.
It allows for constant shifting of the reality of an individual caused by
gaining new knowledge. It allows for the notion that individuals are
constantly trying to gain understanding of their reality. The individual has,
as stated in previous sections, the ability to evaluate themselves in light
of new knowledge which is in a constant state of change. There are three
broad schools of thought within the naturalistic research paradigm –
phenomenology, ethnomethodology and symbolic interactionism. I now
consider each of these to offer some ideas for identifying the
methodological tools that will aid in the research process.
Phenomenology holds the belief that all understanding and interpretations
about others and their actions is in the subjective consciousness.
Carspecken (1996) believes that understanding is synthesised within
experience, with reflection forming a great part of this approach. By
reflecting we are able to reshape understanding in relation to what we
already know.
Ethnomethodology focuses on the world of everyday life, and how people
make sense of their everyday world. It also allows for focus on what
creates each interaction and what perpetuates these interactions from the
viewpoint of the individual. Erickson and Schultz (1981) extend this notion
of everyday life and make sense of the everyday by being explicit about
the meanings we attach to the occurrences under observation stating
that, “all events are mutually shared and ratified definitions, and the
actions are taken on the basis of those definitions” (p. 147).
104
This wide focus on all situations from the viewpoint of the individual allows
for many perspectives to be taken and to some extent, proved a vast
challenge for me.
Symbolic interactionism allows each individual to act towards things based
on the meanings they have for them (Woods, 1979). This implies that
meaning is being constructed continuously due to the constant change in
the experience and reality of individuals.
My research question demands tools that provide a vehicle to explain,
conceptualise and contextualise the notion of prior knowledge. The debate
so far has described some of the key facets offered by the naturalistic
research approach such as an understanding of the ever-changing nature
of each interaction, and a need for the researcher to be open and part of
the research process. Key features of the naturalistic research approach
include description rather than prediction, induction rather than deduction,
generation rather than verification of theory, construction rather than
enumeration, and subjective rather than objective knowledge (LeCompte
& Preissle, 1993, p. 39-44). The inductive nature of the naturalistic
research approach and these features have proved to be appropriate and
valuable to my research question.
The classroom is a dynamic environment, with interactions occurring
rapidly. Each of these interactions has the potential to change the course
of the pedagogic encounter. Thus, in order to explore the role played by
the prior knowledge of pupils in this complexity, I need to observe with an
open mind to the meaning inherent in each interaction and to the sets of
consequences of the sum of interactions. Actions are determined by the
105
individual’s interpretations of the meaning of others’ actions. This places
me in a potential paradox. To understand, I need to explore, to question
and to delve. However, in the process of exploration, I impact on the
interactions by altering the prior knowledge available. This is the dilemma
of whether I should be a participant or non-participant observer. As a
participant, I may have greater access to essential data, yet I cannot be
certain of how much I will have influenced the very thing being observed.
The challenge for me is to “examine situations through the eyes of the
participants … to grasp the viewpoint of the native, his view of the world
and relation to his life” (Cohen et al., 2001, p. 137).
I am assuming that all readers have been in a classroom before, and have
some notion and conceptual idea of what constitutes a classroom setting.
My particular interest is in the interaction between the knowledge bearer
(teacher) and receiver (pupil). As stated earlier, the nature of this
interaction depends significantly on each pupil’s prior knowledge. Since
my interest is to consider what this prior knowledge looks like and how
teachers elicit it in their interactions with pupils, I need to observe not
only the teacher but also the pupils.
The key features of naturalistic research (LeCompte & Preissle, 1993;
Thomas, 1923) are perfectly suited to the ever-changing face of prior
knowledge. The interpretive nature of naturalistic research allows me to
build a structure of prior knowledge. Unlike quantitative research, there is
no hypothesis to be tested and no intervention to be applied. The outcome
of my research is simply a detailed and full description of prior knowledge
and how it is structured.
106
The tools offered by the naturalistic research paradigm give me great
flexibility and provide me with the possibility to be present in situ and
make observations and experience the cultural interchange which occurs.
The researcher’s role should be to elicit sociocultural knowledge from
participants, rendering social behaviour comprehensible (Spindler &
Spindler, 1992). This dovetails perfectly with the outcome for which I am
striving – to comprehend what prior knowledge is and formulate its
structure.
3.4.2 Summary
To arrive at a satisfactory outcome for my research question, I need to
use the principles and procedures set out by naturalistic research. This
approach allows consideration of prior knowledge in the classroom with
clear boundaries and guidelines to allow for choices to be made at difficult
junctures during the research. The ability to observe others and use those
results in order to reflect and formulate a description of prior knowledge
allows for depth and richness in the resulting prior knowledge description
and framework.
Naturalistic research feeds the notion of reflexive internal thinking and
equips me with the ability to use its protocols to structure naturalistic
observations.
So far I have deliberated on the following aspects of the methodological
debate enveloping this study:
1. The different methodological options available and the needs of the
research question.
107
2. The underlying principles of objectivity and subjectivity and how
they have informed the formulation of my research methodology.
3. The methodological framework of naturalistic research, to provide a
common understanding of the research process.
4. Reconciling theory and the practical needs of the research question.
The methodological choices made through this debate shape not only the
nature of the research process but also the outcome achieved. Having
debated and resolved the theoretical and structural issues involved in the
selection of the most appropriate methodology, it is essential for me to
consider the validity of the methodology. Also it is important to look at the
ability of the methodological framework to stand up to scrutiny. The
process selected for answering the research question must itself be
examined and questioned in terms of the validity of the results it provides.
Section 3.5 focuses on examining the methodology in order to address the
vital issues of generalisation and validity.
3.5 Generalisation and Validity
For my research, I use naturalistic research to discover a description of
prior knowledge and to derive a partial model for it through induction.
This raises the key question of the value or the generalisability of the
research. In the previous sections, I have clarified my views on this
research. These views impact my position on the issues of generalisation
and validity. In the process of selecting my research methodology, I gave
consideration to the issues of internal validity, reliability and external
validity. These are further considered in turn in the sections below.
108
3.5.1 Internal Validity and Reliability
My research has focused on using a traditional research methodology
which is practised and understood by other researchers. This has enabled
me to establish a high degree of openness to my research process.
The goal is not to produce a standardised set of
results that any other careful researcher in the
same situation or studying the same issues
would have produced. Rather it is to produce a
coherent and illuminating description of and
perspective on a situation that is based on and
consistent with detailed study of that situation.
(Schofield, 1993, p. 202)
This implies that one of the goals of this research must be to be
transparent in order to achieve the objective of deeper understanding and
shared common generalisation of meaning. Further, as stated by Merriam
(1995), “notions of validity and reliability must be addressed from the
perspective of the paradigm out of which the study has been conducted”
(p. 52).
Hammersley and Atkinson (2007) argue that validity refers not to the data
but to the inferences drawn. This is a process that Merriam terms:
Member checks – taking data collected from
study participants, and the tentative
interpretations of these data, back to the people
from whom they were derived and asking if the
interpretations are plausible, if they ‘ring true’.
(Merriam, 1995, p. 54)
The traditional quantitative process by which reliability is established
(replication of outcomes through repeated implementation of the study) is
109
not suitable for establishing the validity of this methodology. Lincoln and
Guba (1985b) propose that qualitative research should strive for
dependability or consistency. That is, question whether the results of the
study are consistent with the data collected. In order to achieve this
within my research, presentation of both the study and data must be
transparent and detailed. These then provide the reader with the full
breadth and depth of the contextual setting so that reliability can be
established.
3.5.2 External Validity
A key shortfall of naturalistic research is that the results are not easy to
generalise. Campbell and Stanley (1963) refer to generalisability as an
element of “external validity” (p. 175), combining generalisability and
validity as parts of external validation. Their definition applies to the
results of naturalistic research, as well as to the implications of those
results which cannot be replicated or applied to other settings. Therefore
the key question is whether it is crucial for good research to be
generalisable. I question whether there is any value in diluting the results
gained so that they can be generalised, for example, procedures for
effective teaching (Kincheloe, 2003). Or is there greater benefit in
providing results which are “sufficiently rich data for the readers and users
of research to determine whether transferability is possible” (Cohen et al.,
2001, p. 109).
110
The idea of sampling from a population of sites
in order to generalise to the larger population is
simply and obviously unworkable in all but the
rarest situations.
(Schofield, 1993, p. 205)
I have no expectation that my research and the resulting data can be
applied across the entire population in the state in which they are
presented. The processes that I observed are context-specific and
individualised not only to the teachers, children, school and settings, but
also to the relationship that I built with the teachers. As a result, the
specific outcomes generated from my research are highly contextualised.
However, the overall outcome of interest is the set of elements that I
describe as the building blocks of the partial prior knowledge model (I
have used the terms categories and elements interchangeably throughout
this thesis to describe the components of my prior knowledge model).
These elements are present in varying degrees in the prior knowledge of
every individual. This aspect of the outcome can indeed by applied to
others and is generalisable.
People can learn much that is general from
single cases. They do that partly because they
are familiar with other cases and they add this
one in, thus making a slightly new group from
which to generalise, a new opportunity to
modify old generalisations.
(Stake, 1995, p. 85)
This implies that the generalisation of meaning is possible if the partial
model proposed has enabled me to gather a clearer picture of the complex
situations and individuals during the course of the research. LeCompte
and Preissle (1993) argue that studies based on naturalistic research gain
111
their potential for being applied to other situations by providing what they
call comparability and translatability.
Thus generalisability and validity are achieved through the knowledge and
findings of this study in understanding other similar situations, a process
Stake (1995) terms as “naturalistic generalisations” (p. 85). My research
aims that the partial model formed through this work becomes a flexible
template for understanding prior knowledge. Though there are many
variables, the research provides a vehicle for understanding the structure
of prior knowledge in a classroom through the generation of a partial
theoretical model established from a range of contributory elements.
Becker (1990) claims that generalisation in qualitative research is
achieved through building a theory which makes sense of individualised
contexts, situations and persons studied, and further describes how
similar processes could result in different outcomes in different situations.
The aim, therefore, is to achieve generalisability through a partial model
that is formed from a range of contributory elements, concepts and
conclusions of the study. With the presentation of an initial prior
knowledge model, it is possible for others to draw from this and apply or
add to their array of familiar cases and create a “new group from which to
generalise” (Stake, 1995, p. 85). This aids the process of naturalistic
generalisations. Also the naturalistic research methodology implemented
will aid in gaining results which will add to the knowledge base of the
classroom as a whole thus maximising opportunities for generalisations.
112
3.6 Conclusion
In this chapter, I have considered the theory behind the proposed
methodological framework – naturalistic research. I have considered the
value of using this construct to understand and create a fuller picture of
the structure of prior knowledge. As part of the process, I have considered
theoretical options available to aid in the research process, as well as the
issues of validity and generalisability. There has been some reference to
the methods and options available to implement analysis which are
explored in detail in Chapter 5. Overall this chapter has provided the
foundations and structure for more detailed and accurate collection of
data to allow for greater and clearer understanding of prior knowledge.
Now that the research methodology has been identified from a theoretical
perspective as being naturalistic research, the next chapter focuses on its
practical implementation.
113
114
4 DATA COLLECTION METHOD
4.1 Introduction
In this chapter, I present the method I used for the data collection. I give
consideration to the motivation behind the choices I made with regards to
the research context, i.e. the schools used, and the whole design process
for the data collection. I also examine the logistical issues which are
resolved in order to dovetail theory and practice as part of the design
process.
There are three main sections in this chapter, following this introduction.
In Section 4.2, I take the theoretical framework from Chapter 3 and lay
out the following:
the design process which I undertook for the data collection,
concentrating on establishing the choices made;
how I implemented the data collection including the logistical
decisions made and how these appear in the data collection
process. I reflect on how the practical steps taken relate to the
theoretical framework of naturalistic research.
In Section 4.3, I look briefly at the data focusing on the nature of the data
collected. However, there is no analysis of the data in this chapter, as I
feel that it is vital to consider first the data collected in their entirety in
order to gain a high level understanding and a feel for the picture that is
developing. This will allow me to develop methods for detailed analysis in
subsequent chapters.
115
In Section 4.4, I consider the ethical implications arising out of my
proposed data collection method.
4.2 Design of the Data Collection
There are five main points examined in this section. The first is the
research environment – the schools. I present the schools I have used for
the data collection and the reasons for choosing these schools. The second
point is the teachers that I observed within the chosen schools. The third
point examined is the method of observing the lessons. The fourth point is
the means of recording the lessons and transcribing these recordings. The
fifth point is concerned with other sources of data which need to be
retained.
4.2.1 The Schools
It was important to identify a number of schools in order to be able to
meet the criteria of a multi-sited design (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). The use
of several schools, cases, and situations, especially with some variation,
will allow the results to be applied to a greater range of other similar
situations. In the process of selecting the schools, I looked for variations
in a number of factors – school location (the social context), school type,
class size, number of Year One classes, mixed year group, availability of
teaching assistant (TA) linked with the class, and the values, philosophy
and beliefs of the schools as stated by the schools themselves.
I identified the schools through two means:
116
I reviewed the Ofsted reports for the schools in Worcestershire LEA,
and called the heads of the schools that I felt would fit the needs of
my research question;
I placed a request in the weekly Worcestershire County Council
teaching vacancy newsletter asking for schools that might be
interested in the study.
After the schools had responded, I selected five schools based on the
criteria listed above. Table 4.1 contains the list of schools that I selected
and relevant information about them relating to the criteria above.
Table 4.1 Schools used for data collection
School School # Y1 Class Mixed Location Class-
Type classes size year linked
group TA
Hatton Church of 1 15 No Rural No
England small
First School village
St Paul Church of 3 30 No Small Yes
England town
First School
Voluntary
Aided
Argyle First School 1 30 Yes Small No
Common (Y1/Y2) town
Draycott Church of 1 30 Yes Rural Yes
England (R/Y1) small
First School village
Greenville Community 2 30 No Inner city No
Park Primary
School
In order to assess the values, philosophy and beliefs of the schools, I
looked at their Parents’ Prospectuses and Ofsted reports. I reproduce
below relevant portions from the Parents’ Prospectus for each of the five
117
schools. A summary of the Ofsted reports for these schools can be seen in
Appendix A.
Hatton First School
We are proud of our close links with Hatton Church and the local
village community. We have regular visits by the Rector and the
school celebrates with parents many of the religious festivals at the
church. We encourage your children to develop their talents to the
full within a caring, Christian environment. It is hoped that they will
acquire skills, knowledge, healthy attitudes, insights and
appreciation within the orderly structure of the school. Enjoyment
of the school is an important factor so that your child’s appetite for
learning and pace of work is stimulated.
St Paul First School
We endeavour at St Paul’s to give every child the opportunity to
achieve their maximum potential in all areas of school life by
learning through confidence within a Christian setting. Children will
be treated as individuals while being encouraged to be part of the
school family. Our school strives to be a stable, secure
environment, where all children are seen to be treated fairly and
equally and where high standards of behaviour are expected.
Argyle Common First School
The school aims to provide a caring, friendly environment where
children can develop their full potential, both social and academic.
Great emphasis is placed on each child’s individuality and the
contribution that the child can make to the whole life of the school.
Respect for others, both fellow pupils and adults, is strongly
encouraged along with a caring attitude towards the environment.
All are made welcome at the school, particularly parents who are
encouraged to become part of the school life.
Draycott First School
The aim of Draycott First School is to provide a broad balanced and
relevant curriculum, within the framework of the National
Curriculum, and an excellent all round education. We view each
child as an individual whose needs are met through continuous
assessment, careful planning, varied lessons and continuous
review. We view the parental involvement as an essential part of
our pupils’ schooling, and are always pleased to have new ideas of
ways that parents can become more actively involved.
Greenville Park Community School
The main aim of the school is to educate our children to the best of
their ability. In order to do this, we provide a place where children
118
know how to behave and to think before they act. The classrooms
and corridors provide lively and stimulating surroundings that
encourage children to explore and think as they learn. Each child is
treated according to their individual need and we draw on their
experiences and skills in developing our teaching plans to make
sure that we are equipping all children with the skills necessary to
move onto high school.
These five schools formed the basis for my data collection. As can be seen
from the Parents’ Prospectuses above, the five schools are all very
different in their characteristics. However there are some common themes
such as the desire to provide breadth in the curriculum, their moral
values, and inclusion of parents in the learning process. There are also
differences between church schools and community schools, and the
range of demographics within the schools. The next step of the design
process was to consider the teachers from these schools who will take
part in the data collection, and what I was going to tell them about my
research.
4.2.2 The Teachers
In the five schools that I selected for my data collection, there were nine
Year One teachers with one of the Year One classes at St Paul First School
having a job share. Due to logistical reasons, I could only work with one
of the two teachers in the job share. I requested each of the eight
teachers for a short overview giving me information about themselves.
These overviews (reproduced below) were the only things I knew about
the teachers at the start of my data collection.
Mrs Sally Crane, Hatton First School
I have been teaching for about thirty years with just a few years off
to have my family. I taught Reception for most of those years –
first at an inner city school, and then at a large first school. I then
119
did supply, worked for the special needs service, taught in a private
nursery, and taught art at O level.
I returned to work full time at a middle-sized first school with a
class of 7/8 year olds and finally to Hatton where I have been for
fourteen years (teaching Reception, Year 1 & 2, Reception and Year
1, Year 1, Year 3 & 4, and Year 1)!
I am the Science, Environmental Studies and Health Education co-
ordinator, and responsible for the school in absence of the Head.
My main subjects at college were Art and Science.
Miss Lora Hunter, St Paul First School
I trained and qualified in July 1995. My specialities were Early Years
(3 to 8 years old) and Religious Education. I taught Year 1 for six
years in a village school in Norfolk and two years in a market town
also in Norfolk. I have been at St Paul’s since 1998 teaching Year 3
for one year, supply across whole range, and then Year Two for two
years, and then Year One ever since.
Mrs Rebecca Rice, St Paul First School
I have been teaching for about nineteen years. I have been part
time for the past two years in Year 1. I have taught Reception, Year
1, Year 2 and Year 5. During my career, I had post of responsibility
for Computers, Art, Maths, and PSHE. At a previous school, I was
part of senior management team. My philosophy: children learn
from hands-on experience and the curriculum needs to be geared
more for practical experience rather than paper work!
Mrs Jill Thomas, St Paul First School
I trained at a college on a three-year course and was awarded a
Cert. Ed. in 1976. From 1976 to 1981, I taught at a first school in a
town. From 1982 to 1988, I did supply teaching (mainly in this
area). I have been at St Paul First School since 1988. I have done
the following additional courses: in 1996, English in the primary
school (Open University); in 1997, designated maths course on
primary maths active learning; in 1998, BA (Ed) Hons.
Mrs Jane Marshall, Argyle Common First School
I have been teaching for twelve years. Four years at a 3-11 year
old 600-pupil school in the next town where I taught Year 2. Eight
years at Argyle Common where I’ve mostly taught Year 1 & 2, but
taught Year 4 for two years. As a child I hated Maths – I couldn’t do
it. I had extra tuition to get me through GCSE – poor teaching at
high school. Therefore I always try to make Maths fun and not
seem hard – I feel the children learn more this way.
In 1994, I did a designated maths course. In 2000, I did a four-day
National Numeracy strategy course. This was brilliant and I was
120
made Maths co-ordinator in September 2000. I am pleased as
Maths SAT results have improved dramatically since then.
The National Numeracy strategy has changed my way of teaching –
particularly mental maths – with more emphasis on finding
methods of working out, listening to children’s ideas. I do feel that
it does not give me time to dwell on concepts as I need to get
everything done. In some ways I prefer topic maths, where more
seemed to be done in more depth – link to Art, English, etc. I am
trying to develop more maths in real life where it is back to topic
linking maths skills to other areas to practice and consolidate skills.
We were awarded a grant of £300 to develop a maths trail around
our local area by the Chamber of Commerce. This is almost
finalised and will tie in with maths in real life. I do try to link what
they know with what they are going to do and will begin to use
again system of flow diagrams to see what they know and retry
after a topic to see if ideas have changed.
I think (hope) that most of my children enjoy Maths and that I use
a wide range of teaching styles to get over points of view
depending on the children, i.e. some may need more practice, some
enjoy challenge of mental work, some just plod on! I do worry that
the National Numeracy strategy leaves the slow children behind as
when they are working independently, and I am with a focused
group, nothing gets done – whereas before, I could wander around
and check more!
Mrs Jennie Brooks, Draycott First School
I started teaching many years ago in 1968. My first job was at an
infant school in Birmingham where I taught Year 1. Usually the
Easter intakes, therefore they had only had one term in school. In
1970, I got married and we moved to Warwick where I taught at
another infant school. This was very different to the school in
Birmingham and had a very high immigrant population.
In 1973, my first child was born and I gave up teaching. I spent
twelve very happy years at home looking after my four sons. As the
boys were growing up, I did not want a full-time job, so I taught
adults to read on a one-to-one basis in the evenings. When my
youngest son started school, I decided to do supply teaching, and
spent most of my time at local schools. I came to Draycott in about
1992 as a supply teacher, and have been here ever since. At
Draycott, I have always taught Reception/Year 1.
Mrs Helen Fellows, Greenville Park Community School
I did a PGCE and qualified in 1989. I taught Reception in a 240-
pupil school from 1989 to December 1990, and had responsibility
for English. I moved to Greenville Park Community School in
January 1991 and have taught Reception, Year 1 and Year 2. I am
responsible for Information and Computer Technology (ICT), Design
121
Technology (DT), and am Key Stage 1 Co-ordinator. I have done
some team teaching in Year 3/4 and 5/6. Also I support the NQT.
Mrs Jo Fishily, Greenville Park Community School
In 1988, I completed my A levels. From 1988 to 1989, I was a full-
time nanny for a one-year-old and a six-year-old (both girls). From
1989 to 1990, I spent short periods of time as a mothers help,
dental nurse and catering assistant. From 1990 to 1992, I did an
HND in Public Administration. From 1992 to 1995, I did an English
and Drama degree. From 1995 to 1996, I did a PGCE in Early Years.
From 1996 to 1998, I spent one-and-a-half terms as a supply
teacher, one-and-a-half terms teaching Year 2 in an infant school,
and two terms teaching Year 4. Since 1998, I have been a full-time
teacher at Greenville Park including one term teaching reception,
and then as Year 1 teacher. I have subject responsibility for RE. I
am very happy in Key Stage 1 and love the younger children. I
would be happy to teach Reception again.
I am not a very ambitious teacher in terms of gaining more
responsibility, my only ambition is to become a better teacher,
continually improve and hopefully receive the recognition of being a
good teacher. I also aim to keep stress levels to a minimum and
have developed much better strategies of coping with the job and
having proper leisure time!
The teachers had a diverse set of experiences and values. They all had a
range of prior knowledge which impacts upon their pedagogical choices
and the experiences they provide in the classroom. I arranged to meet
with the teachers at their schools near the end of the summer term. We
discussed the details of my research and the process of observation. The
teachers had common concerns such as who would have access to the
information that I collected, what was I expecting the teachers to do, and
how often would I come in. After talking over their concerns, I established
with the teachers that they were happy to participate in my research and
agreed a timetable for the observations.
122
4.2.3 Lesson Observation
As established in the discussions in Chapter 3, this research is based
within the naturalistic paradigm which provokes the exploration of
understanding the reality as it occurs. One of the ways to achieve this
understanding is through observation of lessons. My experiences as a
classroom teacher has given me insights into the complex nature of
observations, such as how each observation revealed greater detail about
the increasing complexity in the nature of the classroom interactions and
the structure of the classroom.
A key issue to address was whether I would be a participant or non-
participant observer during the data collection process. Taking the role of
a non-participant observer would enable me to step back and observe the
classroom without interacting with the children or the teacher, thus
gaining understanding of prior knowledge in a natural state. However, my
experience as a classroom teacher has indicated to me that I could not
remain detached from the classroom as my presence would mean that I
was involved and no longer a non-participant. The problem of wanting to
be detached and observing without influence on the classroom is
summarised as “the theoretical notions of what constitutes a reality to be
observed, and the disturbance of that reality by activities of the observer”
(Edwards & Westgate, 1994, p. 74).
For me, the terms participant and non-participant did not offer any
guidance to structure my observations. I did not want to participate in the
classroom interactions because it could change the very thing that I
123
wanted to understand. Being a non-participant observer meant that I
would have to be completely detached and have no contact with the
context being observed. I needed to be in the classroom to observe the
interactions and understand the changing context within which I was
observing prior knowledge. Gold (1958) offered some guidance in
classifying the roles that a researcher can take in observation, stating
that, “These range from the complete participant at one extreme to the
complete observer at the other. Between these, but nearer the former, is
the participant-as-an-observer; nearer the latter is the observer-as-a-
participant.” (p. 217).
Defining my position as an observer related not only to the methods used
to carry out the observation, but also to the way in which the context is
framed for the observation. It allowed me to structure my role in the
classroom, i.e. I was part of the classroom but was not there to work
within the classroom. Before starting the data collection, both the
teachers and their classes would be made aware of my role and intention.
Further, as my presence in the classroom would be explained to the
children, it would make me a participant in the classroom but an outsider
to the process of teaching.
Despite discussions with the teachers and their classes, I was aware from
my experience as a classroom teacher that there was a possibility that the
teachers and the children were going to take time to get adjusted to the
process of being observed and this could affect some of the early
observations. I addressed this by performing observations over an entire
124
school year, which led to acclimatisation, observing each teacher at least
once a month where possible.
I recorded my observations as field notes in an unstructured and evolving
document, a running report of the events within the classroom while I was
in situ. I included the time of each major event in the classroom such as
moving from one setting to another (e.g. carpet work to group work on
the tables), length of each event, additional adults and their roles within
the lesson, brief notes on the mood of the children, any special events
which were going on in the school that day, and any deviations from the
daily routine. In the margin, I included annotations with notes of thoughts
prompted by the observation.
4.2.4 Recording and Transcribing
The aim of the observations, lesson recordings and informal interviews
was to be able to reconstruct each lesson for retrospective analysis. I
recorded each teacher by using a small remote microphone as they taught
the lesson I was observing. Recording meant that I could remain in one
place in the classroom and still have a record of all the interactions and
conversations of the teacher. The nature of the recording meant that I
could focus on the visual aspects of the classroom interactions, such as
movements of the teacher and children, and the equipment children chose
to aid them in their tasks.
As I transcribed the recordings, I did some mental analysis of the
recordings. However I did not make any omissions or do any coding as
Edwards & Westgate (1994) state that:
125
Interaction is constructed both through the
participants’ interpretation of many factors not
easily accessible to an outsider, and in ways
which are influenced by the structure of the
discourse itself. Those participants draw on
background knowledge of which the observer
may be unaware, they respond to the
constraints of particular types of discourse at
various stages in the lesson, and they regularly
reinterpret the meaning of what was said in light
of what was then said after it, or make
provisional interpretations while waiting for
further ‘evidence’. All these subtleties are seen
as defying instant coding. Instead, they are
judged to require patient scanning of a
transcript, and also (because any transcript is
itself selective) a willingness to return to the
original recording to check or amplify details.
(Edwards & Westgate, 1994, p. 61)
The layout that I established for the transcripts is shown in the extract
below. There are many features associated with traditional transcription
methods which I did not include in my transcripts as the purpose of my
transcripts was to be able to read the words which were said and to
understand prior knowledge through the interactions. To enable ease of
reading, I did not use a specific code to depict any features such as
multiple children speaking. To balance the complex conversations and the
need for simplified representation, I transcribed in a linear fashion.
The most sympathetic transcribing – that is, the
most attentive to details of intonation, pitch and
so on – is unlikely to make informal spoken
language look coherent because speech and
writing are not different ways of doing the same
thing.
(Edwards & Westgate, 1994, p. 63)
They advise, therefore, to include in the transcript whatever features are
necessary for the research purpose. As my purpose was to be able to
126
consider the lesson and understand prior knowledge from it, complex
transcription methods looking at linguistic features were not needed. I
asked each teacher to verify the accuracy of some of their lessons by
reviewing the transcript to ensure that they had been informed, and that
my transcripts were as accurate as possible, and ‘rang true’ (as stated by
Merriam in Section 3.5.1). Given below is an extract from a transcript.
Extract from transcript of seventh lesson by Mrs Sally Crane at
Hatton First School
Teacher: Sixteen good boy well done … I was looking to see who I
could tell was counting in their heads and that was very good
indeed well done … and Olivia I noticed you suddenly stopped
and you realised that you‘ve got to carry on and you did
really well there good girl that was excellent … let’s have one
more go
Few children: Oh
Teacher: Right what number is this?
Few children: Twelve
Teacher: Twelve all right … now this is quite a hard one to stop at so
let’s see if you’re being really clever this morning … are we
ready then
(Children clap)
Teacher: No … no … no … it will be no good if we don’t all start
together ready … and
(Teacher and most children clap twelve times ... some
children clap thirteen)
Teacher: Ah I told you number twelve is a difficult one I don’t know
why we get going to ten
Child: I know it’s cause
4.2.5 Other Data
Spradley (1979) and Kirk and Miller (1986) recommend keeping four sets
of observation data:
1. Notes made in situ.
127
2. Expanded notes that are made as soon as possible after the initial
observations. (This was in the form of tape recordings which were
transcribed in their entirety without omissions.)
3. Journal notes to record issues, ideas, and difficulties which arise
during the field work. (This was done in the margin of my field
notes in order to keep the context of the thoughts.)
4. A developing, tentative, running record of ongoing analysis and
interpretations. (In the case of this research, this was done
throughout the classroom observations and the transcribing process
by formulating a pictorial representation of the emerging model.)
In order to rebuild the classroom interaction at a later date with some
degree of accuracy, I also collected lesson plans and notes that the
teachers had made about children involved in the lesson being observed.
Where possible, I talked with the teachers after the lesson to understand
their view of the lesson (this too was recorded and transcribed). I carried
out unstructured interviews with the teachers to establish the accuracy of
my transcriptions, and to understand from them how they viewed their
teaching and their knowledge of the children.
4.3 The Data
The data set consists of sixty lesson observations, fifteen informal
interviews, notes on informal conversations with the teachers, lesson
plans for each lesson, and notes on various children that teachers had
made for the purpose of sharing with me during the academic year. The
observations were done over the course of a school year (from September
128
to the following July). Each lesson was approximately forty-five minutes
long. All the lessons and informal interviews were recorded and
transcribed. Additionally, I also had observation notes from the lessons.
During the process of transcription, I was beginning to analyse the data
and look for some indication of understanding prior knowledge. The
different forms of the data, namely the audio recording and the notes,
provided different angles of perspective for each observed interaction.
When the lesson transcripts are augmented with my written notes, lesson
plans and notes on the children, they enable me to reconstruct the
interactions in the classroom.
Therefore to analyse and understand the interactions involves the
reconstruction of the classroom using all the different viewpoints and
making sure that they all tessellate together. This multi-pronged approach
forms the basis of my analysis which is considered in greater detail in the
next chapter.
4.4 Ethical Considerations
A multitude of ethical considerations were taken into account in the
theoretical and practical design of the data collection, as per the ethical
guidelines for educational research from the British Educational Research
Association (2004). They are detailed in Table 4.2 below.
Table 4.2 Discussion on ethical issues in data collection
Ethical issue Discussion
Selection of As stated in Section 4.2.1, schools were identified from
schools among those who responded to a request for
129
Ethical issue Discussion
participation. Good research practice dictated that I
should get the widest variation in the schools that I chose
to ensure generalisability of the results. There was a risk
that all schools that may want to take part would be
similar e.g. over 40% of the primary schools in
Worcestershire were church schools.
To avoid this, I selected from the responding schools by
looking at their Ofsted reports to ensure there were a
range of schools which fit the factors stated in Section
4.2.1. I achieved this by selecting five of the eight schools
who responded to the request. The three schools which
were not selected were very similar to the schools that
were selected.
As the schools chose to reply to a call for participation,
this was a random self-selecting group and avoided any
sampling issues.
Though all the schools were local to me, I had no prior
involvement with any of the schools in either a
professional or a personal capacity.
Selection of In order to ensure that teachers were fully informed
teachers about what it meant to take part in the study before
taking agreeing to do so, the following were carried out:
I met with each of them individually to talk over
the research project, the data collection process
and how the data would be used subsequently;
all the teachers were able to ask questions about
the research throughout my involvement with
them, as this openness did not hinder my data
collection;
the teachers were made aware that they could
withdraw at any point without any consequence to
themselves or their school;
the teachers understood that they had open access
to all my data at any point in the research process
(and indeed, they helped out by reviewing the
transcripts for accuracy, privacy and anonymity);
we talked about the nature of the study and
considered some of the concerns that teachers had
such as how many others were involved in the
study.
After considering all the concerns and issues, the teachers
were given time from the summer term till autumn to
consider taking part in the research. Throughout this
time, they were able to ask questions in order to support
their choices. As reported in Section 4.2.2, all nine
130
Ethical issue Discussion
teachers chose to participate in the study, though I had to
turn one of them down due to logistical considerations.
I am aware that there was no gender variation in the
teachers. Nationally 87% of the teachers in primary
schools are female (Department for Education, 2008) –
which means that I would have at most had one male
teacher in any case. However none of the schools that I
chose had male Year One teachers, making it impossible
for me to ensure that the teachers I chose reflected the
national gender distribution.
Though all the teachers were local to me, I had no prior
involvement with any of them in either a professional or a
personal capacity.
Consent / The head at each school provided voluntary informed
participation consent on behalf of the school. The teachers’ consent
was implied by the fact that they chose to be part of the
research after understanding all the information
mentioned above.
The parents of the children were informed of the process
and purpose of my research through the systems that
each school had in place. As part of this, they were given
the option to ask further questions of myself or to
withdraw their child at any point, though none of them
chose to do so.
Before starting the data collection, I ensured that I had
adhered to all the guidelines for voluntary informed
consent laid out by the British Educational Research
Association (2004).
Incentives There were no incentives offered to any of the
participants (schools, teachers, children or parents).
Privacy Privacy was upheld throughout the process by ensuring
the following:
schools were not aware of which other schools
were taking part;
teachers were not aware which other teachers
outside of their school were taking part;
as part of the initial discussion with the teachers,
they were informed that any personal information
that they revealed would not form part of the study
without their consent;
names of schools, teachers and children were
anonymised throughout to ensure that no data
could be linked back to an institution or an
131
Ethical issue Discussion
individual;
teachers reviewed all the raw data to ensure that
the children’s and their privacy was maintained;
the data in its raw form (i.e. the transcripts) were
only stored on my home computer which no one
else could access.
Impact of As the dominant tool for data collection was the recording
participation of lessons by the teacher wearing a small recording
device, it did not impede on their ability to teach or carry
out any other classroom activities.
Since I was interested in the entire class rather than any
particular group of children within the class, there was no
impact on the children in terms of any bias towards any
particular group.
Disclosure I established from the outset that information collected in
the classroom would not be shared with anyone else not
connected to that class (including other teachers from the
same school irrespective of whether they were
participating in my research or not), unless something
occurred that needed to be addressed in relation to issues
of child protection or any other criminal reasons. It was
agreed in discussion that I would only use the data for the
purposes of my research.
Observation My role as a participant or non-participant is addressed in
detail in Section 4.2.3.
Transcribing Transcriptions were made as accurately as possible. This
was enhanced by getting the teachers to review the
transcripts for accuracy.
Other data All of this data were similarly anonymised, and were
shared with the teachers in discussions.
132
5 ANALYSIS METHODOLOGY
5.1 Introduction
The focus of this chapter is the exploration, evaluation and explanation
underpinning the selection of a methodology for analysing the qualitative
data gathered for this thesis. In the previous chapter, I have described
how these data were gathered and recorded. The debate now revolves
around how to make sense of the vast quantity of qualitative data
generated in order to address my research question. The initial prompt for
my research question was my own experience in the classroom and the
desire to understand why there were such variations in children’s ability to
perform a range of mathematical tasks. What accounts for the wide
variety of differences when, at first sight, children have so much in
common which should lead to a smaller degree of variation in their
mathematical abilities, especially in the current homogeneous nature of
schools and the curriculum that they deliver? The overall aim of this
chapter is to select a methodology by which my data can be analysed. The
steps taken to select the methodology must be described explicitly so that
they can be scrutinised and the outcome is transparent. Vitally, the
selected methodology must filter through pertinent information without
losing any context of the classroom where children are working in their
natural environment.
The methodology used for analysis should assist in developing a
comprehensive understanding of prior knowledge. It is crucial to the
133
shape, strength and value of this thesis that any methodology used for
analysing the data is rigorous enough to give an accurate picture and
understanding of all the nuances that may exist in prior knowledge. This is
analogous to doing a chemical analysis of DNA – I want to define not only
the key components of prior knowledge, but also the structures of these
components and how they work together in a learning situation.
As considered in Chapter 4, the data were gathered through recording
teachers while working with a class. The recordings formed naturalistic
transcripts of what was picked up by the teacher while in a mathematics
lesson. The transcripts are what the teachers would have also heard from
their interaction with the children. It is important to understand that this
data gathering method does not give a complete picture of any one child,
nor does it offer a before-and-after idea of the ability of the children. The
decision to not follow such a scientific process in a controlled environment
is an intentional omission. I want to be able to develop a model of prior
knowledge within a realistic environment and from the teacher’s
perspective. It is not the concern of this thesis to consider the effect that
prior knowledge has on mathematical ability – just to define it. It is the
situated nature of the data which will make it most relevant to defining
prior knowledge and being able to apply it within the classroom context.
Using what the teacher can hear in children’s conversations not only gives
the teacher’s perspective, but also allows us to notice what the children
choose to share and bring to their learning of mathematics. Thus using
the scientific research paradigm would not have allowed for any
understanding of how prior knowledge manifests itself in the classroom
environment. This firmly places my research within the qualitative
134
research paradigm. Qualitative research is “somewhat difficult to define,
as specific practice that covers a variety of studies” (Wiersma & Jurs,
2005, p. 13).
There is overall agreement within the education community that
qualitative data takes a whole range of shapes. Essentially it comprises
information that is not numerical information in its raw form. Therefore it
is any data generated as a result of interviews, observations or written
text. It is noteworthy that each of these data gathering methods can also
produce quantitative data. However they are predominantly used to
understand everyday phenomena as a result of human behaviour, and to
address not just what is happening, but more crucially how. These
approaches all offer a deeper understanding of a microcosm of human
behaviour. The key need that qualitative data address is the need to
understand human behaviour in minute detail and to formulate theories or
models to address the observations gathered. The complexity which the
data generated bring makes it essential to consider how best to analyse
and understand what the data are trying to express. It does not measure
but merely describes, and once analysed, attempts to define the nature of
human behaviour, in this case prior knowledge.
The ethnomethodology implemented for data gathering, while being well-
suited to understanding the everyday behaviour of children in the
contextual sense, leads to “massive volumes of data typical of qualitative
research” (Dey, 1993, p. 86).
It is essential to note that such complex, interlinked and varied data in
their raw state “won’t speak for themselves if left in the form in which you
135
collect them … these raw data do not constitute the findings of the
research” (Ryan, 2006, p. 92).
Therefore it is essential to analyse the data to deal with the enormity of
the information presented in its raw form. In order to understand how
best to do the analysis, this chapter will consider what methodologies are
available for analysing the data. The selected methodology will allow me
to see what emerges from the mass of information. There must be a
substantial process of scrutiny and assessment of the methodologies
available.
Therefore this chapter will contain:
1. Criteria for selecting the analysis methodology
2. Exploration and evaluation of possible analysis methodologies
3. Selection of analysis methodology
4. A worked example of the analysis process
5.2 Criteria for Selecting the Analysis
Methodology
The purpose of data analysis is to translate the
evidence into a form which allows the
researcher to make clear and concise
statements of description and/or association.
(Anderson & Burns, 1989, p. 200)
The selected analysis methodology must:
be suitable for the type of data gathered;
136
provide the framework for understanding the data in relation to the
questions asked;
also establish a solid evidence base linking the understanding to the
source data;
address what counts as evidence;
supporting following for the raw data;.
organizing them, breaking them into
manageable units, coding them, synthesizing
them, and searching for patterns
(Bogdan & Biklen, 2007, p. 159)
support all three generic stages of the data analysis and model
generation process;
data reduction, data display, and conclusion
drawing and verification
(Miles & Huberman, 1994, p. 10-11)
allow for the exploration of ideas for categorising the data allowing
for the emergence of any relationships between categories;
enable key themes to emerge from these categories in a
transparent manner without changing the true nature and meaning
of the data collected;
be robust enough for reanalysis and refinement of categories and
themes;
allow for understanding of the interpretations and assumptions
made of the raw data;
allow for choices to be made about what data can be omitted and
why;
137
allow a refined narrative of how and why the analysis was
performed and further allow understanding of the resultant model
or theory;
allow consideration to be given to the ethics of the data analysis
process.
There is much demand on this process for finding an answer from the
information gathered. Therefore the methodology “requires that the data
be organised, scrutinised, selected, described, theorised, interpreted,
discussed and presented to a readership” (Ryan, 2006, p. 95) and
understanding that the information needs to provide coherence,
consensus and validity to the raw data collected. Thus the analysis
methodology must be selected with care, and the outcomes of the
analysis must be explained in detail with a transparent trail back to the
raw data which can then offer a simple narrative to support the outcomes.
The criteria above make the choice of methodology easier in many ways.
From the start of the research process, the nature of the question and the
subsequent data gathering methods involved place this study firmly in the
naturalistic / ethnomethodological realms of research. Therefore statistical
methods, which are valuable for quantitative data, offer little benefit and
will not be considered.
The special task of the social scientist in each
generation is to pin down the contemporary
facts. Beyond that, he shares with the
humanistic scholar and the artist in the effort to
gain insight into contemporary relationships
(Cronbach, 1975, p. 126)
138
Where quantitative researchers seek causal
determination, prediction, and generalization of
findings, qualitative researchers seek instead
illumination, understanding, and extrapolation
to similar situations. Qualitative analysis results
in a different type of knowledge than does
quantitative inquiry.
(Hoepfl, 1997, p. 48)
The nature of the data gathered and the subsequent process of analysis
will result in this different type of knowledge being gained, filling the gaps
identified in the literature in Chapter 2 and leading to an understanding of
prior knowledge.
5.3 Exploration and Evaluation of
Possible Analysis Methodologies
There are many methodologies for the analysis of qualitative data such as
hermeneutical analysis, domain analysis, typological analysis, analytic
induction, content analysis, phenomenological / heuristic analysis,
metaphoric analysis, and grounded theory. These methodologies have a
lot to offer to the process of understanding the everyday behaviour of
individuals. I will be considering some of these methodologies with the
criteria presented in Section 5.2. There are no quantitative data to
consider.
Qualitative research: research that describes
phenomena in words instead of numbers or
measures.
(Krathwohl, 1993, p. 740)
139
In my case, it is based in a naturalistic paradigm. Therefore analysis
requires a lot of consistent interpretation of the evidence presented. It is
worth looking at how the various qualitative methodologies work, and
considering if they meet the criteria outlined earlier.
5.3.1 Hermeneutical Analysis
Hermeneutical analysis is the art of interpretation, more so interpretation
of text and language.
It seeks to understand situations through the
eye of participants … Hermeneutical analysis
involves recapturing the meanings of interacting
others, recovering and reconstructing the
intentions of the other actors in a situation.
(Cohen et al., 2001, p. 29)
That is to say, looking at language to explicitly express what the meaning
behind the text really is “rather than the phenomena” (Cohen et al., 2001,
p. 29).
Although hermeneutical analysis developed from the analysis of ancient
scriptures and other historical legal documentation, it has been developed
by Dilthey, Gadamer and others (Cohen et al., 2001) into a general theory
of human understanding through the use of literary text.
The process of analysing texts takes into account the context not only of
the author, but also of the text itself considering historical, cultural and
philosophical contexts to allow interpretation of meanings, meanings that
allow fundamental understanding to be developed about human nature.
This process of analysis does not aim to generalise but, in some sense,
140
merely report literally what is written and its intent as a way of
understanding human nature.
This method places restrictions upon how my data can be understood.
A social science that restricted itself to
hermeneutic interpretation would be radically
incomplete. It would exclude from the scope of
social science research the whole range of
causal relationships and structural influences on
action.
(Little, 2008)
Therefore this methodology would not allow for the identification of any
patterns, and hence the formulation of categories from analysing data.
Consequently there would be no understanding of the interrelationships
between categories, if any. As a result, this methodology does not meet
the selection criteria outlined earlier.
However this methodology has something to offer in the way of
understanding my data. The transcriptions I have, which are written text,
are not truly literary but are mere written representation of the spoken
word. They do allow the slowing down of speech and the ability to
consider and reconsider how what was said explains the variation in
mathematical ability. The methodology raises awareness of the continuous
attention that must be given to the layers that make up the analysis of
the text in order to extrapolate meaning and intent. The layers – social,
historical, cultural, time and place of writing – are important to consider
when analysing other data which are not in the form of text. Furthermore
consideration needs to be given to the context not only of the text, but
also of the author and the reader. Understanding the interplay between
141
these three elements is vital when using this form of analysis. While
considering my transcription data, similar issues and layers must be
understood. The elements – children, teacher, classroom and researcher –
each have their own context which will influence the outcome.
5.3.2 Domain Analysis
As stated earlier, essentially all methods for analysing qualitative data are
concerned with organising and sorting the vast volume of information
generated by observations and interviews into an understandable and
applicable format. Domain analysis is one such approach. Spradley
(1979), the prominent author of The Ethnographic Interview, looks at how
to understand the linguistic ideas expressed by individuals and put them
into manageable chunks which allow researchers to describe social
situations and cultural patterns that may be within these ideas. This
understanding, as Spradley (1979) sees it, can be gained through
categorising the data through the lens of predefined semantic
relationships which allow me to sort the ideas into categories, and then
further sort these categories into domains. Spradley (1979) states, “A
domain is any symbolic category that includes other categories” (p. 100).
In some ways, it is similar to the process that botanists may use to sort
the wide variety of plants. There is an overall predetermined domain e.g.
evergreens. Within this domain, there are several smaller groups of plants
which are grouped into categories for their common features and other
similar properties. This form of sorting allows several key things to
happen. Firstly, it makes the comprehension of the data gathered easier,
142
as it allows for critically understanding the nature of the data. Secondly, it
allows for consideration to be given to the relationship, if any, between
each individual category. Lastly, it allows descriptions to be developed in
relation to the domain. For this method to work, it relies on the
researcher’s ability to sort the data using the nine predetermined
semantic relationships defined by Spradley (1979).
The basic idea behind creating domains is to find
categories by reading the data with specific
semantic relationships in mind.
(Hatch, 2002, p. 166)
Spradley (1979) offers steps to help with the process of creating domains.
However domain analysis is limited in supporting understanding of my
data as they can only be sorted by the use of semantic relationships which
in themselves are linear. This in itself is not an issue in understanding any
data, however in the case of my research, it does not provide a way of
sorting the data without considering any relationship. It presupposes that
there will be some relationship between domains which follows a set
pattern, thus making no provision for simply sorting the data and allowing
relationships to emerge. Furthermore this approach assumes that once a
relationship has been established within the domains, these relationships
cannot be changed and are constant. E.g. a fir tree is a type of evergreen,
and through the use of domain analysis, it cannot be categorised as any
other. There is much to be learnt from the nine relationships that Spradley
(1979) defines as a starting point to considering the data gathered.
However it does not allow for the complex ever-changing nature of human
behaviour to be understood and presumes that reality exists in nature
143
waiting to be discovered (Hatch, 2002). Hence domain analysis alone will
not provide the answer to my question. There needs to be further support
from other processes which will allow the clarity needed from the complex
data set. It is not enough to use the process of sorting and categorising,
there needs to be greater understanding of the interplay between domains
and further steps may need to be taken to establish that.
5.3.3 Typological Analysis
This method requires the processing and classifying of data. LeCompte
and Preissle (1993) defined the process as “dividing everything observed
into groups or categories on the basis of some canon for disaggregating
the whole phenomenon under study” (p. 257).
The approach towards the data is quite different when carrying out
typological analysis. The data set is split into broad predetermined
exhaustive categories, in contrast to domain analysis which is more
inductive in nature. Typological analysis already presumes a theory /
research objective / an idea of what the data may show. In order to use
this method of analysis, the first step is the identification of a typology.
If typological analysis is the appropriate data
analysis strategy for a study, the selection of
typologies should be fairly obvious as well.
(Hatch, 2002, p. 152)
In relation to my data, on first consideration, there are no obvious
typologies which can be identified. Therefore it makes the use of this
method difficult. Also having looked at the data, it is of little help to set
out with preconceived ideas of what the patterns are, as that is the
144
essence of the questions being asked. There is little to be gained by using
this process as the thesis, in some ways, is asking for a typology of prior
knowledge to be developed. Therefore the use of this deductive approach
is not a natural fit to the demands of my data.
Typological analysis only has utility when initial
groupings of data and beginning categories for
analysis are easy to identify and justify.
(Hatch, 2002, p. 152)
This is not the case in my research. However it is important to consider if
this approach has anything to offer. The deductive nature of the
methodology expects there to be stronger understanding not only of the
data, but also of the behaviour being studied and the formulation of
generalised rules which will allow me to understand the data collected. It
is these overall predetermined exhaustive categories which allow the mass
of data to be processed and some sense to be made of what the
information gathered is trying to tell us.
For this research, the process is reversed and is, in part, inductive in that
it is hoped that through looking at the data, some categories should start
to emerge and then the rest of the data can be put through the filters of
the emergent categories to allow testing for validity. Mouly (1978)
suggests that there is a relationship between inductive and deductive
which is interdependent. It is this
back-and-forth movement in which the
investigator first operates inductively from
observations to hypotheses, and then
deductively from the hypotheses to their
implications in order to check their validity from
145
the standpoint of compatibility with accepted
knowledge.
(Mouly, 1978, p. 5)
Though in its purest form typological analysis will draw a dead end in
understanding my data, it does prompt the need to categorise which will
allow all the data to be sorted into more manageable chunks and tested
for robustness against the whole of my data.
The primary strength of typological analysis is
its efficiency. Starting with predetermined
typologies takes much less time than
“discovering” categories inductively. The
potential weakness is that applying
predetermined categories will blind the
researcher to other important dimensions of the
data.
(Hatch, 2002, p. 161)
5.3.4 Analytic Induction
The key proponents of this methodology – Znaniceki, Howard, and Katz –
offer steps in understanding the data, and also using the data analysis
process systematically to formulate a theoretical basis for the phenomena
being examined.
In order to carry out analytic induction, it is necessary to consider some of
the other approaches available to organise and review the vast quantity of
qualitative data. The process of analytic induction allows me to test the
strength of the partial model developed. LeCompte and Preissle (1993)
suggest that data must be filtered to create manageable categories, and
the categories must be examined to see how they relate to each other.
Many of the methods discussed in this chapter e.g. domain analysis, can
146
be used to get to this point. Essentially it is this sorting, categorising and
grouping which holds the key to understanding the data and what they
have to tell us. Denzin (1989) goes further to recommend that it is not
merely enough to categorise and filter the data to get to a model, but the
researcher also needs to examine what does not quite fit with the overall
model. Any data that do not follow a particular pattern must force the
reformulation of the categories. The process of analytic induction
encourages deliberate seeking of disconfirming cases (Bogdan & Biklen,
2007). It is this search for disconfirming cases and consequent re-
examination of the data that will ensure robust applicability and accuracy
of the model which is generated.
Therefore analytic induction is not a mechanism for categorising or
organising data, but is the next step in ensuring that the data are
presented and evaluated thoroughly. The process focuses on using
disconfirming data to enhance the robustness of the model which makes
this a good second step in the process of analysing my data. However, as
an overall method for analysis, it does not support one of my criteria for
selecting the analysis methodology.
supporting following for the raw data;.
organizing them, breaking them into
manageable units, coding them, synthesizing
them, and searching for patterns
(Bogdan & Biklen, 2007, p. 159)
The question still remains – what is the best tool for organising the
complex data collected? So far only domain analysis and typological
147
analysis have offered some methodological support to categorise the data.
However they have not fulfilled the criteria set out for choosing an
analysis methodology.
5.3.5 Content Analysis
The process of content analysis is predominantly concerned with looking
at the content of written text or people’s speech in various media.
Research using qualitative content analysis
focuses on the characteristics of language as
communication with attention to the content or
contextual meaning of the text.
(Hsieh & Shannon, 2005, p. 1278)
The main idea in this type of analysis is to define and measure carefully
the content in order to allow categories to be determined. In the case of
my research, the text is in the form of transcripts and observation notes
which allow slowing of speech down in order to examine it in more detail.
Content analysis itself has been defined as a
multipurpose research method developed
specifically for investigating a broad spectrum of
problems in which the content of communication
serves as a basis of inference.
(Cohen et al., 2001, p. 164)
Rosengren (1981) gives a broader definition, “Content analysis describes
a family of analytic approaches ranging from impressionistic, intuitive,
interpretive analyses to systematic, strict textual analyses” (Hsieh &
Shannon, 2005, p. 1277).
148
It is this ability to explore a broad spectrum of problems which makes this
method suitable for dealing with qualitative data. There is much to be
gained by the flexibility this approach has to offer. Hsieh and Shannon
(2005) have identified “three distinct approaches: conventional, directed,
or summative” (p. 1277).
Conventional approach starts from observations; direct approach starts
with a pre-formulated theory; and summative approach starts with
predetermined keywords for categorising the data. Weber (1990) states
that, “Investigators must judge what methods are appropriate for their
substantive problems” (p. 69).
The conventional approach is most suitable for my research, as it starts
by considering the observations, and then coding and defining these
observations through the analysis process. However Hsieh and Shannon
(2005) have identified that “the conventional approach to content analysis
is limited in both theory development and description of the lived
experience, because both sampling and analysis procedures make the
theoretical relationship between concepts difficult to infer from findings”
(p. 1281).
Content analysis does have something to offer in terms of understanding
the phenomenon of prior knowledge. However its lack of ability to support
formulations of links between concepts is a major shortfall. When
considering the data through the lens of conventional content analysis, it
allows for understanding the data in a literal form, but provides no ability
for formulating a deeper understanding of how the data may be
149
connected. However, as an overall method for analysis, it does not
support one of my criteria for selecting the method of analysis.
allow for the exploration of ideas for categorising the data allowing
for the emergence of any relationships between categories;
which is crucial to understanding any phenomena being observed.
5.3.6 Phenomenological Analysis
Phenomenology asks, “What is this kind of
experience like?”, “What does the experience
mean”, “How does the lived world present itself
to me (or to my participant)?”
(Finlay, 2008)
The study of how we experience our world, phenomenological analysis
stresses “the careful description of phenomena from the perspective of
those who experience the phenomena” (Wiersma & Jurs, 2005, p. 243).
There is an intense need to understand how/why everyday
actions/behaviour occur. Burrell and Morgan (1979) wanted to question
“the ‘taken for granted’ assumptions of everyday life” (p. 193).
Phenomenological analysis approach offers a methodology for
understanding the deep-rooted meanings which individuals place on the
world around them. The difficulties for the researcher in understanding
the real world is how to extract understanding from observations,
interviews and other methods used to gather information on the world
that surrounds us. There are multiple layers of complex actions and
reactions within different contexts, with the added variable of the
researcher’s own experiences and context make this simple need to
150
understand the everyday one of the most complex processes. It is not the
place of this section to consider the methods for the collection of data
using the phenomenological methodology as this has been covered in
Chapter 4. Here I need to consider how to understand from this data what
is going on in this everyday experience being examined. How to go from
capture of information through various methods to understanding the
phenomena? The process of analysis is dependent on the key premise that
“phenomena should be studied without preconceived notions” (Hatch,
2002, p. 29).
Husserl termed this practice as bracketing which “means holding a
phenomenon up for inspection while suspending presuppositions and
avoiding interpretations” (Hatch, 2002, p. 86).
Bracketing requires that we become aware of
our own assumptions, feelings, and
preconceptions, and then, that we strive to put
them aside – to bracket them – in order to be
open and receptive to what we attempting to
understand.
(Ely et al., 1991, p. 50)
The key idea being that the phenomena is able “to present itself to us
instead of us imposing preconceived ideas on it. This openness needs to
be maintained throughout the entire research process, not just at the
start.” (Finlay, 2008).
Therefore the process of analysis is based on an inductive school of
thought, looking at what the data relay about each individual and their
experience while being observed. The process of understanding this type
of data involves some level of interpretation on behalf of the researcher,
151
and also the need to understand how to organise the mass of data
collected. In order to gain understanding of the data, it is possible to use
both inductive/deductive methodology to establish understanding. The
methods for organising the data can also vary depending on the nature of
the question being asked. However it must be noted that any analysis,
organising and reporting of the data is carried out with the key principle of
detachment from the situation.
This form of analysis creates a difficult paradox for the researcher, one
where there is the need for interpretation, but in order to interpret there
is some degree of personal experience involved in the process. It is worth
considering the nature, method and process of interpretation which will be
made in order to carry out this analysis. Clearly interpretation is making
sense of the observation data collected. In order to carry out any
interpretation, there will need to be some explanation for what is going on
within the situation being observed (Hatch, 2002). The researcher is
central to this process of understanding and explaining what is being
observed. Therein lies the contradiction – the phenomenological analysis
process requires that the data are allowed to reveal themselves, but this
cannot take place without interpretation from the researcher. The way to
meet the complex need for complete detachment is to clarify, as part of
the process of analysis, what the individual researcher’s context is in
order to allow the data to then be understood with this in mind. The
researcher must play this balancing act between being objective and
acting as a mere lens for the data to be understood through and the need
to make sense of what is being seen. The approach for this analysis
therefore must again start with a clear question which is being asked of
152
the data and consider all possible outcomes using a systematic
methodology for going through the observations.
In relation to my research this approach goes some way to allow
understanding and analysis of data. There needs to be greater structure
and this is not provided by this approach.
5.3.7 Metaphor Analysis
Metaphor analysis offers a creative dimension to understanding and filling
the possible shortfall identified by some of the analysis methodologies,
that of understanding the complex relationships between categories. As a
researcher, a key outcome is to form some clarity in understanding of the
phenomenon being observed, and also formulate some conceptual
understanding that can be simplified and shared by others. The use of
metaphors within language offers the mechanism for this simplification
and conceptualisation to occur seamlessly.
Cameron (2003), in her research, identifies the value of searching for
metaphors as the core approach for understanding how people think – the
metaphors that people use can reveal something of their ideas. This key
notion drives metaphor analysis and offers a possible process for
understanding prior knowledge. The steps in metaphor analysis are similar
to that of many of the others considered so far – locating the data,
identifying key ideas (in this case, identification of metaphors as a unit of
data), organising metaphors into categories, and finding patterns. The
identification of these metaphors are directly from the qualitative data
generated.
153
On the face of it, this seems to be a valuable tool for gaining insight into
the thinking of individuals, and therefore perfect for in-depth
understanding of prior knowledge of individuals. The underlying
assumption of this approach is that all individuals use metaphors in their
dialogue and speech. This in itself is a problematic assumption as it is not
always the case. Furthermore if the process of analysis only looks for
metaphors as a way of understanding any of these phenomena, then
there is a significant possibility that some pivotal ideas may be missed.
The other more pertinent issue for my research is dependent on the pure
nature of a metaphor. Metaphors are complex linguistic tools which are
developed by individuals through experiences of the world linked to
sophisticated development of language and vocabulary. E.g. to use /
understand what is meant by the metaphor “life is a journey” (a metaphor
examined by Lakoff and Johnson (2003) in their work on metaphor
analysis), there are many layers of complexities which are only
understood through experiences which most children, due to their age, do
not have. Figure 5.1 depicts some of the conceptual notions which must
be grasped before one can understand this metaphor and gain its true
meaning and appropriate application. There needs to be a vast amount of
other knowledge and experience which will not be present in children.
Therefore to expect them to speak in such a complex manner is
misguided. Thus this approach has little to offer in terms of a tool for
analysis of my data.
154
Life as a process
Life Life as an abstract concept
Unpredictability of life
Stages
Route
Direction
Getting lost
Journey Different ideas about a journey Destination
Mapping
Planning
New routes and plans
Change of direction
Figure 5.1 Breakdown of life is a journey metaphor
However I do feel that it is by the use of metaphors that we can explain
the ideas found in the data. Also it allows some strong images to be
formed by readers which tap into their experiences in order to allow true
individual understanding of my research to be formed. LeCompte and
Preissle (1993) argue strongly for the value of metaphor, simile and
analysis as a vehicle for exploring and explaining ideas presented in the
data.
Though the tool of metaphor analysis is not one which provides me any
value, the debate has allowed the emergence of a tool to aid the
description of the prior knowledge model.
5.3.8 Grounded Theory
Grounded theory is the most well known methodology for collection and
analysis of qualitative data. Its mass use in understanding qualitative data
provides me with many benefits, one of these being that there is much
support in literature for its implementation. On the other hand, this
popularity means that there are many interpretations of the same theory.
155
These interpretations and variations make it very difficult to ensure that
the process being used is essentially as intended by the core ideas
provided by the initial theory.
Glaser and Strauss’ (1967) seminal work defines grounded theory as “the
discovery of theory from data systematically obtained from social
research” (p. 2).
The aim of grounded theory is to consider the observations made and to
use them to explain or answer the questions posed by the researcher
which firmly places this methodology in the inductive paradigm for
understanding the world around us. The methodology aims to develop a
theory which meets four pivotal criteria – fit, understanding, generality
and control (Strauss & Corbin, 1990). The essence of grounded theory is
to try and make sense of the world in a systematic manner. In order to
gain a complete understanding of the meaning of grounded theory, I must
consider the meaning of theory.
Theory in sociology is a strategy for handling
data in research, providing modes of
conceptualization for describing and explaining.
(Glaser & Strauss, 1967, p. 3)
Theory is a comprehensive explanation of the phenomenon being
observed. This, in terms of grounded theory, is derived from the data
itself. In grounded theory, the role of the researcher is quite different
compared to the other analysis methodologies. The use of grounded
theory demands that the researcher is open minded with no preconceived
ideas, but has skills in the area being studied. Glaser and Strauss (1967)
156
define this characteristic as being “theoretically sensitive” (p. 46). It is
through this ability to be theoretically sensitive that discoveries or
understanding can not only emerge but also be recognised and developed.
Central to grounded theory is the maxim that the data shines a path to
the answer and understanding of the phenomena.
There are many stages to carrying out grounded theory. At this point the
methodology parts into different directions. The original process proposed
by Glaser and Strauss (1967) was modified further by Strauss and Corbin
(1990) resulting in two different approaches to grounded theory. There
are some key philosophical differences, summarised in Table 5.1.
Table 5.1 Key differences in grounded theory approaches (Onions,
2006, p. 8-9)
Glaserian Straussian
Beginning with general wonderment Having a general idea of where to
(an empty mind) begin
Emerging theory, with neutral Forcing the theory, with structured
questions questions
Development of a conceptual theory Conceptual description (description
of situations)
Theoretical sensitivity (the ability to Theoretical sensitivity comes from
perceive variables and methods and tools
relationships) comes from
immersion in the data
The theory is grounded in the data The theory is interpreted by an
observer
The credibility of the theory, or The credibility of the theory comes
verification, is derived from its from the rigour of the method
grounding in the data
A basic social process should be Basic social processes need not be
identified identified
The researcher is passive, The researcher is active
157
Glaserian Straussian
exhibiting disciplined restraint
Data reveal the theory Data are structured to reveal the
theory
Coding is less rigorous, a constant Coding is more rigorous and defined
comparison of incident to incident, by technique. The nature of making
with neutral questions and comparisons varies with the coding
categories and properties evolving. technique. Labels are carefully
Take care not to ‘over- crafted at the time. Codes are
conceptualise’, identify key points derived from ‘micro-analysis which
consists of analysis data word-by-
word’
Two coding phases or types, simple Three types of coding, open
(fracture the data then conceptually (identifying, naming, categorising
group it) and substantive (open or and describing phenomena), axial
selective, to produce categories and (the process of relating codes to
properties) each other) and selective (choosing
a core category and relating other
categories to that)
Regarded by some as the only ‘true’ Regarded by some as a form of
grounded theory method qualitative data analysis
Considering the synthesis presented in the table above, the Straussian
approach does not meet my criteria as it does not allow, due to the coding
paradigm it prescribes, the pure emergence of categories. There is
presupposition of what the data are going to show. The researcher is
already charged with some clear idea of what the coding structure will
entail. This does not allow for creativity in the discoveries made.
The best way to understand the minute but key difference between the
two approaches is to consider the role creativity plays in allowing the
emergence of theory. Looking at Karl Duncker’s (1945) candle problem
helps to distinguish the differences. You are presented with a candle, a
box of matches and a box of drawing pins, and are asked to place the
candle on the wall. At first, you may use the drawing pins to try and fix
158
the candle to the wall, or melt the candle via the matches, and eventually
you may come up with a better solution which is to empty the box of
drawing pins, fix the box to the wall and place the candle in the box. In
the Glaserian approach, there is no clear starting point and many
possibilities are explored until a solution is found. In the Straussian
approach, the problem is presented with the drawing pins already outside
the box, and with you already having some idea that the box may hold
the solution to the problem, thus reducing the need to try many
possibilities as an answer is obvious. This reduces creativity as it focuses
the researcher on one way of thinking which may prevent the true
discovery of theory. Also it depends on the researcher already having
formulated some ideas about the end outcome, maybe through reviewing
the literature.
In the Straussian approach, there is less freedom to be creative and to
really allow the data to say their own narrative. On the other hand, the
Glaserian approach is less structured and allows the researcher to be led
by the narrative from the data. It assumes that the researcher has some
knowledge or skills to consider and understand the content, but no idea of
how to formulate a theory which will explain the question being asked.
The researcher will discover the answer by using the constant comparison
method, unrestricted by what has been learnt before and being only led
by the data.
The main intellectual tool is comparison. The
method of comparing and contrasting is used for
practically all intellectual tasks during analysis:
forming categories, establishing the boundaries
of the categories, assigning the segments to
categories, summarizing the content of each
159
category, finding negative evidence, etc. The
goal is to discern conceptual similarities, to
refine the discriminative power of categories,
and to discover patterns.
(Tesch, 1990, p. 96)
5.4 Selection of Analysis Methodology
So far, I have considered eight methodologies which fall into deductive or
inductive processes for understanding any phenomena. It is worth noting
that I have not considered all possible qualitative analysis methodologies
e.g. matrix analysis, event analysis, discourse analysis, semiotic analysis,
narrative analysis, and many others. It is of little value to consider these
methodologies as they are not extending the tools already on offer, but
are merely providing a different starting point for analysis, a different way
to consider the same data or are not applicable to my data.
The eight methodologies reveal the complexity of understanding
qualitative data. The common theme throughout all these analysis
methodologies is a set of generic stages for analysis. All the
methodologies advocate some form of collection, sorting, categorising,
making links between categories, leading to the outcome. However not all
methodologies provide adequate tools for all these stages.
The collection of qualitative data in evaluation is
common. However, knowledge about strategies
for efficient and defendable procedures for
analyzing qualitative data is less common.
(Thomas, 2006, p. 237)
Hence no single methodology will enable me to answer my research
question. This leads me to conclude that I need to use a blended approach
160
in the selection of analysis methodologies. If I accept the generic stages
for analysis together with the criteria for selecting the analysis
methodology (Section 5.2), then the eight methodologies are sufficient for
selecting and defining the blended approach.
Therefore I have selected Glaserian Grounded Theory together with
content analysis as the way to sort and categorise my data, identify links
between categories and answer my research question. These two
methodologies together meet the criteria set for selecting the analysis
methodology. Grounded theory supports the generic stages for analysis,
and content analysis supports the coding process by enabling the constant
comparison of data in order to fulfil the grounded theory approach.
5.5 Worked Examples of the Analysis
Process
In this section I am going to illustrate, through extracts from my
transcripts, how I have used the blended approach of content analysis and
grounded theory to analyse my data to answer my research question. I
have used grounded theory as the framework for the analysis and content
analysis to understand the meaning of each transcript so that the
resulting interpretations may be organised using the framework of
grounded theory. I have made no attempt to give the final outcome as it
is the focus of the next chapter.
This section is very procedural and descriptive. Alongside the process, I
have explained some of the choices I have made in interpreting my data.
161
In line with Glaserian grounded theory, choices have been led by the data
and the direction that these choices have taken. The detailed description
of the analysis process ensures that it is completely transparent and clear
in how the outcome is established, as being transparent will set the
context for the model being proposed. Furthermore this enables
reproducibility and applicability in a wider variety of contexts, or in
grounded theory terms – generality. Most importantly, going through the
steps taken and using actual data helps to tell the all-important narrative
of the research process.
The central premise of Glaserian grounded theory is that there is no
theory to verify, but for the researcher to be “generating grounded theory
is a way of arriving at a theory suited to its supposed uses” (Glaser &
Strauss, 1967, p. 3).
The starting point is not from any a priori assumptions. The blended
approach forces me to take a step back and look at prior knowledge de
novo and not be influenced by any meaning of prior knowledge pre-
established due to the common use of this term.
Figure 5.2 is a diagrammatic representation of my analysis process and
how it fits in with the data collection.
162
Figure 5.2 Data collection and analysis process
5.5.1 Theoretical Sampling
As discussed, grounded theory is an open-ended analysis process and can
be implemented in many different ways. Grounded theory states that
analysis starts from data collection, as the data being gathered are
continuously interpreted by the researcher and shape the choices made
for further data collection and analysis. Glaser and Strauss (1967) term
this process as theoretical sampling.
Theoretical sampling is the process of data
collection for generating theory whereby the
analyst jointly collects, codes, and analyzes his
data and decides what data to collect next and
163
where to find them, in order to develop his
theory as it emerges.
(Glaser & Strauss, 1967, p. 45)
Theoretical sampling allows the researcher to be creative and question the
data as they are gathered to arrive at a comprehensive understanding.
Therefore theoretical sampling can be seen as a method for formulating
live instructions for data collection; a guide for the direction to be taken to
ensure that the most suitable data is collected. This method of constant
analysis is in tune with how I develop my thinking. As a researcher, it is
impossible to gather data and not to start letting them influence my views
and understanding which in turn affects my data collection. The analysis
revealed the need for immediacy in shaping the data collection. Without
the responsive nature of theoretical sampling, I would be left with a static
understanding of a constantly changing phenomenon.
Theoretical sampling is a very organic and evolutionary process which
enhances and allows for magnification and analysis of data. The actual
data collection mechanism has been described elsewhere (Chapter 4) and
will not be described here again. Instead, I will focus on the key stages of
identifying events, concept development and categorising before the
ultimate stage of theory production, and how these stages influence and
nurture the overall data collection and theory development. When using
theoretical sampling, the overall data collection is determined as the
process is carried out. Therefore there is no indication from the outset as
to what the data set will look like and how much data will be needed. Only
the analysis will determine what is gained from the data and whether
more data are needed.
164
5.5.2 Analysis
Before seeing worked examples of my analysis, it is essential to define the
key terms used during the analysis process.
Events These are all incidents within lessons in which children are
engaging in mathematics. They are not labelled or defined.
They are just the identification of possible areas of interest in
terms of helping develop an understanding of prior
knowledge.
Concepts These are groups of events which have similar properties and
are similar in their function. Therefore any number of events
can be grouped to form concepts.
Categories This is further classification of ideas in order to start to
develop an understanding of how the ideas being considered
function. Number of concepts may function in a similar
manner or may be shaped by a similar force, and therefore
form a category. Developing categories may allow me to
understand not only how prior knowledge may be structured,
but also how it may function and formulate a model of prior
knowledge.
Memoing The annotations made throughout the data collection and
analysis to record my thoughts and ideas related to what I
was observing or analysing. These formed prompts when
later considering events, concepts and formulation of
categories.
165
5.5.3 Identifying Events
In order to analyse the data gathered, the constant comparison
methodology with the procedural mechanism of content analysis was
used. This allowed each recorded lesson observation to become part of a
larger trail of ideas. I started with each lesson observation and transcribed
it so that it would become easier to identify events, evaluate and
understand what is taking place when children are engaging in
mathematics. For me, the process of transcription was the first step in
analysis as I was listening to each part of the lesson in great detail. So I
tended to use this opportunity to consider the following general questions:
What is going on in these lessons?
What are the different situations that present themselves in these
lessons?
What are the children engaged in?
How are the children managing the mathematics they are being
asked to engage in?
What are the children bringing to each mathematical task to
support their understanding?
These questions led to other questions, which in terms of analysis are
crucial:
What are the key events that can help my understanding of prior
knowledge?
Are there any groups or characteristics suggested by the talk taking
place within the lesson being considered?
166
Figure 5.3 Identifying relevant events
The answers to these questions were dependent not only on what was on
the audio, but also on me being physically present in the classroom at the
time of recording and my notes (memos). The notes prompted and added
important context and depth to the analysis. When looking at the data at
this stage, I initially sorted the data and started to group them
conceptually. The transcripts of the lesson observations allowed me to
identify all events that may be relevant to my original question. Figure 5.3
shows the broad grouping at this very early stage of analysis.
I considered in the first instance all events that were related to
mathematics which occurred in a lesson. At this early stage there was a
mass of data all having some possible connections. The transcript extract
below shows the broad nature of the coding at this first stage. I simply
highlighted all conversations that had any mathematical content.
167
Extract from transcript of fifth lesson by Mrs Helen Fellows at
Greenville Park Community School
Teacher: Estimate is a good guess … too many, too few, about, less
than, more than, roughly, those are the sort of words we
might use ok … two games to play today about estimation …
ok here is the first one … I have a number in my head it’s
between five and eleven … ok and to help me remember the
words we are going to use I am going to keep the key
vocabulary here
Child: I know what it is
Teacher: Put your hand up if you can estimate or have a good guess
which number I have got in my head … thinking caps on
Child: Between five and eleven?
Teacher: Correct … ok team Demi?
Child: Umm eight
Teacher: Ohhh less than eight … Wesley?
Child: Seven
Teacher: Spot on … thumbs up to Wesley … ok I am going to close my
eyes I’ve thought of another number … ok it is between five
and eleven … and I am going to use this vocabulary to help
you work out the number … would you put a teddy in the
teddy jar Wesley … that was spot on … ok Angel?
Child: Umm is it five
Teacher: More than … Richard?
Child: Is it six
Teacher: That’s too few
Child: I know I know four
Teacher: It’s between five and eleven so … too few … Amber ?
Child: Ten
Teacher: Ohhh thumbs to Amber she’s earned a teddy in the teddy jar
… well done … going to close my eyes ready to do it once
more cause you’ve got this one sussed very good … I am
thinking of a number umm right … I’ve got a number I am a
number I am between five
Child: Eleven
Teacher: Five and eleven … that was a very good estimate working it
out … ok can you give me a number please … Jenna?
Child: Sixteen
Teacher: Ohhh that’s too many cause the top number I’ve got in my
head at the moment is eleven
Child: You can’t get bigger
168
Teacher: Ok this is how we are going to play the game … I’ll say too
big too small sometimes yes
Child: Is it nine?
Teacher: Toooo many … Dominic?
Child: Eight
Teacher: Umm too many … Melissa good girl for having your hand up
Child: Is it one
Teacher: Too few remember we were stating the lowest number we
could have is five and the highest number we could have is
eleven … let’s choose someone else with their hand up Kya?
Child: Twelve
Teacher: Too many remember the highest number we are talking
about at the moment is eleven … Bethany?
Child: Six
Teacher: Spot on good girl put a teddy in the teddy jar … ok
Child: I was going to say that
Child: No you were not
Child: Can we count them
Teacher: We’ll count them in a minute … Bethany could you estimate
how many teddies we’ve got in that teddy jar?
Child: Umm twenty
Teacher: You think twenty we’ll see at the end of the session thank
you very much indeed … right the next game we’re going to
play is called pick a card
Child: Pick a card
Teacher: Pick a card and I am going to ask Mr Collins to choose
someone who is sat beautifully on their bottom and didn’t
shout out … to come and pick five cards from here
Teaching Assistant: Azaad
Teacher: Well done Azaad … I want five cards ok … come and pick five
cards
Child: You’re not allowed to look
Teacher: All right ok thank you … pick a card … thank you
Child: You are not allowed to look
Teacher: Shhh no peaking … I’ll huff and I’ll puff
Child: I can see em
169
Teacher: There we go ok shhh shhh … (long pause) … that one ok …
and we need one more … all right that’s lovely go and sit
down then … we’re going to play with only those cards and
I’ll just show you what is on the other cards … right just to
show you we’ve got spots on the other cards … I’ll show you
and you are going to guess … estimate how many spots there
are … ready … ok how many
Child: Five
Teacher: Oh that means that’s too slow I am showing you then …
that’s dead easy … right again
Child: Ten
Teacher: Ok right … now I am going to show you one of these cards
really quickly … and I mean just like (click)
Child: That
Teacher: And I want I would like you to estimate how many spots
Charlie
Child: It has got
Teacher: It has got … and I am going to write your estimations down
on the whiteboard … (whispers) … if you have a go at
estimating what you do need to do Kya? … (child puts hand
up) … thumbs up to Kya please … (stops whispering) … right
… are you ready
Child: I know what it is
Teacher: Haven’t shown you yet … right on your bottoms time to
Few children: Look listen and concentrate
Teacher: I think Jenna and Leanne need a bit more help come over
here Leanne come and sit by Wesley and Jenna come and sit
by Kya cause they are looking and listening brilliantly … quick
… right ok you have to look really quickly … and it goes like …
that
Child: Four
Teacher: Put your hands up don’t shout out … estimate Wesley
Child: Four
Teacher: Ok … ok … Dominic
Child: Four
Teacher: You think four Angel?
Child: Four
Teacher: Four ok
Child: It is four
Child: It is four I saw it
170
Teacher: Let’s have a look then … oh well done ready … let’s do
another one … I was going to try and catch you out here be a
bit mean
Child: He sawer it
Child: It is four I told you … I knew it
Teacher: Now it was quite easy to do that one … because it was in an
easy pattern
Child: Do a tricky pattern
Teacher: I might at the end do a tricky pattern … who is sitting
beautifully Angel … thank you let’s have a look … put your
hands up please Richard
Child: Seven
Teacher: Ok let’s put that there our estimation Angel
Child: Nine
Teacher: Nine
Child: I know
Teacher: Another estimation Gemma … (long pause) … not sure
Thomas?
Child: Eleven
Teacher: Eleven
Child: She thought that
Teacher: Another estimation Bethany?
Child: Umm six
Teacher: Six
Child: I know one
Teacher: Another estimation Umar?
Child: Ten
The next step was to consider what was important to leave out of the
analysis and why. Using the constant comparison loop revealed that there
were incidents appearing in the transcripts which did not support any
understanding of the way in which children were engaging in
mathematics, and hence the development of a theory. These incidents
comprised conversations linked to classroom routine or logistical
procedures such as instructions in relation to how children should move
171
about the classroom between areas of learning. Also any conversations in
relation to behaviour management were not included for analysis. The
transcript extract below shows what was omitted from the data set, with
the omissions highlighted.
Extract from transcript of sixth lesson by Mrs Jill Thomas at St
Paul First School
Teacher: Right can you move just a little way please … (long pause) …
there we are … come on Kaitlin and Jonathan B!!! … I want
someone to hold … have you nearly finished Evie? ok can you
hurry up and put your milk carton in the bin … come and join
us … thank you Evie umm I’ll have Jordan
Some children: He’s done it before
Teacher: You’ve done it before!!
Child: Yes
Teacher: All right we’ll get someone else then … come on Jessie
Child: She’s done it too
Child: No I haven’t
Teacher: Let me see … no she hasn’t … right now remember I am
going to say one number you have got to give me the
number that together with it makes … ten … (more children
come in) come in quickly
Child: Where
Teacher: There we haven’t started yet … right not yet not yet … we’ve
got to beat seventeen … in one minute … ok … right quickly
sit down Evie
Child: I think we can do a hundred
Teacher: I don’t think you’ll be able to do that many not with this …
because it’s only one minute … right … shh shh … ready
steady … shh … go seven
(Children shout the answer when asked)
Child: Two
Teacher: No … seven
Child: Three
Teacher: Yes nine
Child: Five
Teacher: No … nine
Child: One
172
Teacher: Yes five
Child: Five
Teacher: Yes ten
Child: None
Child: Oh I was going to say that
Teacher: Yes three
Child: Six … no eight … no seven
Teacher: Yes two … Jack?
Child: Eight
Teacher: Yes four … (long pause)
Child: Oh six
Teacher: Yes three Chris
Child: Seven
Teacher: Four … four
Child: Six
Teacher: Six yes six six
Child: Four
Teacher: Yes two
Child: Eight
Teacher: Yes four … (long pause)
Child: Oh six
Teacher: Good girl yes … three … Reece?
Child: Seven
Teacher: Yes seven … seven … Jack
Child: Three
Teacher: Yes five … five
Child: Five
Teacher: Yes ten … ten
Child: Zero
Teacher: Yes zero
Child: Ten
Teacher: Yes four
Child: Six
Teacher: Yes five
Child: Five
Teacher: Yes two … two … (long pause)
173
Child: We’ve run out
Teacher: Oh right stop … how many … five ten fifteen sixteen
seventeen
Teacher and some children: Eighteen nineteen
Teacher: Wow give yourselves a clap … very good and I think … I think
that we could improve on that score because at the
beginning … there were one or two children who were a bit
unsure so I think we can try and beat nineteen next time …
thank you Jessie
Child: I think we could get loads
Teacher: Thank you Kaitlin
Child: If every single one of us played then we
Child: Then we could get loads
Teacher: Yes but remember we’ve only got one minute … (long pause)
… you’ve only got one minute
Child: We could go faster … and quicker
Child: Can we practice now
Teacher: We’ll try it again later
Child: How much is a minute … this one’s got three we could use
that
Teacher: Well that one is three minutes but a minute is long enough
for what we want to do … all right then … (long pause) … now
we’re going to see … Brian what are you doing?
Child: Going there … he is shoving me
At this stage, I amassed a large set of events which were increasing
through the continuing data collection. There was no structure or pattern
to the events that I could discern at this stage. It was through the
collection and repetition of the analysis process of many more lessons that
a pattern began to emerge which allowed grouping of different parts of
the transcripts which were similar.
5.5.4 Creating Concepts
The complexity at this stage was figuring out how to be completely true to
the grounded theory approach which calls for the removal of oneself in an
174
attempt to be led purely by the data. However, as Charmaz (2007)
suggests, suspending one’s knowledge and experience is impossible and
often undesirable, especially as the researcher is investigating something
she is drawn to out of interest or experience. Therefore I have used my
experience of being in the classroom to support the analysis.
No effort should be made to put aside ideas or
assumptions about the situation being studied,
on the contrary, the researcher should draw on
previous knowledge and experience to
understand better the process under
investigation.
(Baker, Wuest, & Stern, 1992)
In addition, the analysis process was also given some direction and
orientation by the initial literature review supported by the recognition by
Glaser in relation to the use of literature where he stated that “all is data”
(2001, p. 145). Despite this availability of literature as an analytical aid, I
kept the key grounded theory principles of open-mindedness and
objectivity at the forefront. Hence the focus of my analysis was to
consider what the transcripts illuminated in terms of what children were
bringing to bear upon tasks. Furthermore I looked for the nuances and
tried to understand the subtlety of what was being expressed through the
transcripts, and was led by what the data were showing in terms of
understanding prior knowledge.
Alongside the event identification process described in Section 5.5.3, the
mass of events needed to be ordered into manageable groups which
would allow me to examine in detail what children really brought to bear
upon each mathematical task. In this section, I will consider stage by
175
stage how concepts emerged through the constant reviewing and
comparison of all events.
Figure 5.4 Initial sorting of mathematical events
Figure 5.4 shows the next stage in analysing the data. The dashed boxes
are from Figure 5.3, and support the identification of all possible relevant
events. Examining the transcripts closely revealed the different types of
responses and conversations that the children were having while engaging
in mathematics. These could be grouped into smaller manageable
concepts – group responses, short responses or lengthier responses. At
this stage, all the relevant events were put into these three concepts and
no event was left unsorted. It is important to note that as more data were
being generated, there was constant refinement through the constant
comparison process of which events were in each of these three concepts.
The data revealed events comprising responses given by children while
working as a whole class, mostly in the mental/oral starter section of the
lesson. I labelled these as group responses. The transcript extract below is
one such example from my data. Through closer examination and trying
176
to identify what children were saying individually, I made the decision at
that stage to omit all group responses from my data set as it was not
possible to clearly attribute responses to individual children. Furthermore
group responses tended to be responses which had been rehearsed (e.g.
the transcript extract below includes counting from different starting
numbers and counting odd numbers), and gave no hint in order to
understand the nature of prior knowledge of individual children.
Extract from transcript of sixth lesson by Mrs Rebecca Rice at St
Paul First School
Teacher: Can you shut this please … right … (long pause as the class
settles in) … shh shhh move up move there thanks … right …
let’s see if you can count for me from umm let me see ten to
eighteen please …
Most children: Ten eleven twelve thirteen fourteen fifteen sixteen
seventeen eighteen …
Few children: Nineteen …
Teacher: Oh you are not listening eighteen … can you count from umm
let me see … seventeen up to twenty-four
Most children: Seventeen eighteen nineteen twenty twenty-one
twenty-two twenty-three twenty-four … twenty-five …
Teacher: Uhh you’ve got to listen twenty-four … can you count from
nine toooo twenty-five
Most children and teacher: Nine ten eleven twelve thirteen fourteen
fifteen sixteen seventeen
Most children: Eighteen nineteen twenty twenty-one twenty-two
twenty-three twenty-four twenty-five … twenty-six twenty-
seven …
Teacher: (clicking all children) … right odd numbers from three up to
eleven …
Most children and teacher: Three five seven nine eleven …
Teacher: Jolly good odd numbers from one up to thirteen
Most children: One three five seven nine eleven thirteen
Another group of events were responses (correct or incorrect) where no
elaboration or explanation was offered by children as to how the child
derived the answer. I labelled these as short responses. The transcript
177
extract below is one such example from my data. All events labelled as
short responses could potentially offer some understanding of the child’s
thinking process, though at this early stage of the analysis it was not clear
what this understanding might be.
Extract from transcript of sixth lesson by Mrs Jo Fishily at
Greenville Park Community School
Teacher: Yes solid … yes … right now … what I want you to do first is
can you tell me what all these shapes are called? we’ll start
with the easier one what’s this one called?
Child: Rectangle
Teacher: What’s this one?
Child: Square
Teacher: Brilliant now it gets a little bit harder … so concentrate …
Child: Cube …
Teacher: Cube very good …
Child: Cone
Teacher: Excellent
Child: Cill
Teacher: Cylinder cuuub
Child and teacher: Cuboid
Teacher: And …
Child: Circle
The remaining set of events comprised responses where children gave
more detailed explanations and conversations to support their thinking. I
labelled these as lengthier responses. The transcript extract below is one
such example from my data (the highlighted sections are lengthy
responses of two different children – Damian and Rhian). This concept
formed the basis of majority of the focus for my analysis, and is
considered in more detail through the rest of this section.
178
Extract from transcript of second lesson by Mrs Jane Marshall at
Argyle Common First School
Teacher: It costs two pence … If I wanted to buy three plants and they
cost two pence each how much would it cost? One flower
cost two pence how much would it cost to buy three
Child: Umm three … oh
Teacher: Jay?
Child: Three pence
Teacher: If they are two pence each I wanted to buy three …
Child: One pence
Teacher: Sharna can you help him out?
Child: Six pence
Teacher: Why would it be six?
Child: Umm umm well it’s … (long pause)
Teacher: Why six … Damian?
Child: Cause you know you’ve got three flowers if you’ve got two
lots … it is double three … you add another three pence on if
there was 1p it would be three pence on if there were 2p it
would be six pence because you are adding another three on
… cause if you had six plants all at one three would be at two
be same as three plants all at 1p
Teacher: Right so you have halved this number oh that is a bit of
tricky thinking … Rhian is there another was of working it
out?
Child: Well yes cause 2p is one more than 1p you can just figure it
out by counting in two up
Teacher: Right I could just count in two’s … ok I want three plants so it
would be two four six … so if one plant … is 2p all right …
that’s my one plant … there it is ok and I want to buy three I
am going to need another plant and another plant ok so I’ve
got
Child: 2p and 2p and 2p
The next step in the analysis process was to look at all the lengthier
responses. Through constant comparison of each of these responses and
looking at new relevant events being identified, I looked for patterns and
similarities which could be used to sort the large volume of lengthier
responses into meaningful similar groups to support further detailed
analysis and theory formulation. The data slowly revealed that among the
179
lengthier responses, there were different ways in which children were
responding to the tasks they were tackling. At this stage, I put these
responses into different concepts based on their similarities, as can be
seen in Figure 5.5.
Figure 5.5 Different subsets of lengthier responses
Figure 5.5 also shows how these concepts link with the previous step in
the analysis (represented using dashed boxes). Below are brief
descriptions and examples from transcripts to exemplify each of the
concepts within lengthier responses.
Responses with no explanations were related and similar to short
responses, in that children did give an answer, and when probed were not
able to give further explanation of how they were able to address the
question. Therefore these were considered and grouped together with the
short responses. The transcript extract below shows one such example
from my data.
Extract from transcript of fifth lesson by Mrs Jane Marshall at
Argyle Common First School
Teacher: Sorry three and what makes ten … three and what makes ten
… right let’s see … three there and four five six seven eight
nine ten … Elliott count both hands fingers on both hands …
show me … what am I going to add to three to get to ten …
Child: Seven
180
Teacher: Well done you’ve got it … right shhh shhh ready … ten
Child: Ten that’s no …
Teacher: Shhh … shhh … (long pause) … show me … (long pause) …
right you should have nothing cause you can’t add you’ve got
to ten already … right next one I am getting quicker … five
Child: Five and …
Teacher: Five and what makes ten? … (long pause) …
Child: That’s easy five …
Teacher: Yes five and five make ten … five and … show me how…
Child: Five and five make ten …
In the transcript above, the child did not give any further detail as to how
he arrived at the answer. When probed, he again only responded with the
answer. There is nothing in this interchange that would inform me further
about the child’s way of thinking. So I deemed this similar to a short
response, and grouped it as such.
Continuing the examination of the events revealed that there were a
group where children’s responses were negative to the task. In this case,
children either stated they could not do the task even with support, or
were not familiar with what was being asked. The transcript extract below
is an example from my data where the child stated that he could not do
the task.
Extract from transcript of fifth lesson by Mrs Jo Fishily at
Greenville Park Community School
Teacher: I’ll be there in a second … ok ready … right … now show them
Aiden … ok everyone write down their guess … under my
guess … let me see … you think seven and Martha says …
Child: I can’t do it …
Teacher: Have a go …
Child: I can’t do this …
Teacher: Have a guess …
Child: Six
181
Teacher: OK … right shall we count them now
Few children in group: One two three four five six seven …
Teacher: Seven … so in that box you write … seven … well done spot
on … Martha was
Child: Too less …
Teacher: One too few … and Aiden you were a bit too high … and you
did it …
In the transcript extract above, there is some understanding on the child’s
part that they are not able to do the task. But in this example, no reason
is given as to why – just that they were not able to do the task. All such
events were grouped together.
The other type of negative response which was noted in a few of the
events were children who expressed no familiarity or understanding of
what the task was and thus could not do it. The transcript extract below is
an example from my data where the child was not at all familiar with the
task involving quarter, half and whole turns, and needed much support.
Even after this, she found the task difficult.
Extract from transcript of sixth lesson by Mrs Rebecca Rice at St
Paul First School
Child: I don’t know what this is
Teacher: What don’t you understand?
Child: This one here …
Teacher: Right let’s have a look a minute … umm right can we all stop
a minute … we need to think about what strategies … what
ideas we can use to help us to find out what we … what ball
is going to go in the gap … ok so what is Anna doing to find
out where her piece … what was she doing with her jigsaw …
can that help …
Child: She was turning them
182
Teacher: She was turning them around wasn’t she Megan … so can
that help us figure out what we need to do … so let’s have a
look at this first one here … we want to know which one will
go here … so what you can do is if you get one of them and
put it on top exactly the same as the ball as the pattern if
you then … can you just watch for a second then you can try
it … if you then look at the next one and then turn yours
round to look at the next one … you can see if you have done
a whole turn … that is much easier now isn’t it?
Child: Umm
Teacher: Right so have I done a whole turn … a quarter turn or a half
turn
Child: A half turn … a whole turn
Teacher: A whole turn!! a whole turn would be this … look it would go
weeee … like this … weee … weee that’s a whole turn … ok so
let’s see half a turn … a half turn would do that … is that
right? is that about right?
Child: Ummm
Teacher: Is that matching … is that right … is that matching that?
Child: No
Teacher: No it’s not no that’s right … so start again ready all watch
again Robbie … turn it round like that a quarter turn … is that
matching now?
Child: Yes
Teacher: Yes that’s right so this first line is a quarter turn … so you’ve
only got to turn it a quarter turn … so I’ll put that on there
Robbie you can do a quarter turn for me … do a quarter turn
for me … go ahead I am watching … (long pause)
Child: What do I do?
Teacher: Well you need to make a quarter turn … so that is going to
go there … all right? … if we do another quarter turn … what
will it look like?
Analysing further events showed more patterns, one such being that in
order to carry out the task, children were recalling something from the
past. I labelled these as have done the task before. This recollection
spanned various timescales from the immediate based on what had just
been carried out to ideas from further back. Upon further examination, it
emerged that the nature of what the children were recalling varied in its
forms. Though children were recalling ideas, concepts or procedures, the
183
nature of what they recalled was very different. At this stage, any events
which had any recollection were grouped together. In the transcript
extract below, we can see an example of ideas that children are using
from previous maths lessons.
Extract from transcript of fifth lesson by Mrs Rebecca Rice at St
Paul First School
Teacher: Right well done ok … (long pause) … now I am going to add
something … what am I going to do? ok I’ve come down in
my spaceship and I’ve landed from the planet Zorb … and I
don’t know what to do … someone’s told me I’ve got to do
that sum and I don’t know how to do it … someone’s said
that I’ve got to put these two numbers together
Child: Easy … you told us before
Teacher: (long pause for writing on the board) and I don’t know how
to do it … who can put up their hand and help me … I don’t
want the answer yet … I’ve got to find out how to do it first
before I can find the answer … Isaac what have I got to do?
Child: Umm add it up
Teacher: I don’t know what add it up means … what do I have to get
the numbers and do this?
Child: No
Teacher: How do I add it up … it is a very funny word add
Child: I know
Teacher: Joshua?
Child: Put it together
Teacher: Put it together … ok umm it won’t go … (trying to push the
number on the board) … Georgina?
Child: Count your fingers … like we always do
Teacher: Count my fingers … one two three four five six seven eight
nine ten … (long pause children laughing) … Maisie
Child: Put three on one hand and two on the other one
Within the lengthier responses, there were also responses which were
intertwined with work that children had done as a small group or in pairs,
and this had supported the child’s understanding of the task and the
consequent response. I labelled these as worked with others. The
184
transcript extract below gives an example where Daniel explains to
Harvey why he is wrong and how he should perform the task. These
conversations were very different in nature to group responses which I
have discussed earlier and argued for omission. The key difference is that
these events were not whole class and clearly gave a greater level of
detail in how the interaction between children supported their
understanding. On the other hand, group responses were short responses
by generally the whole class to rehearsed ideas and therefore shed no
light on individual thinking or understanding.
Extract from transcript of seventh lesson by Mrs Rebecca Rice at
St Paul First School
Teacher: You are right you have got the same … umm (shouts)
everybody do this … everybody do this (clicking) shh
everybody do this (tapping her head) oh Hannah, Isaac,
Edward, (long pause stops shouting) I know you’re all
working extremely hard and I can see that green group are
enjoying their game … and I see blue and red group working
hard but we are far too noisy … (long pause) … we are going
to carry on working for five more minutes and this time I
don’t want to have to stop for the noise … I can’t hear the
children I am working with on the floor and they are right
next to me … whisper … right … (long pause) … Joshua you
say you’ve got six pence Charlotte’s got six pence you’ve got
the same amount of money haven’t you … what about you
umm Alex what do you think? have you got the same amount
as everybody else or have you got more money than
everybody else?
Child: The same
Teacher: The same as Joshua … has Joshua got the same amount as
everybody else then or are you richer than everybody else?
Child: We’re both richer …
Teacher: You’re both richer … why do you think you’re both richer?
Child: We’ve got more coins … look one two three
Teacher: You’ve got more money? Harvey’s got six pence … Harvey’s
got six pence Daniel’s got six pence … how much have you
got?
185
Child: No I have 3 [Harvey counts the number of coins he has – 2p
2p 2p – making 3 coins] and Daniel has 2 [Harvey counts 1p
and 5p coins as 2 coins]
Child: No look 1 … 2 3 4 5 6 that’s 6p [Daniel adds value of 1p and
5p to make 6p]
Child: But I have 3 … 1 2 3 [Harvey again counting the coins]
Child: No the numbers on it are 2 2 2 so 6 [Daniel asking Harvey to
use the value of each coin]
Child: Oh six pence
Teacher: Six pence … is that more or less or the same as everybody
else?
Child: Same
Teacher: The same … does that make you richer?
Child: Same
Teacher: What have Alex and Josh got more than everybody on the
floor?
Child: Coins
The last concept identified within the lengthier responses related to other
ideas that children brought to bear upon the task to support their
understanding. I labelled these as other ideas for tackling task. There
were a large number of events which showed that children were not just
recalling or giving responses, but were using some other experiential
ideas to support the understanding and eventual solution to the question.
The transcript extracts below are three such examples from my data
where children used the idea of cakes at their fête, a number track in the
playground, and a hundred square to support how they addressed the
question being asked.
Extract from transcript of seventh lesson by Mrs Jill Thomas at St
Paul First School
Teacher: Umm now we’re going to see how good you are at listening
and how you can try and work out the answers to these
number stories … you could use adding … or taking away so
let’s try … ready ok? … Ok let’s think I had ten cakes and I
ate three of them … how many cakes did I have left … Lucy?
186
Child: From the cake sale yesterday … but you were helping at the
table miss.
Teacher: Yes at the cake sale … I had ten cakes and I ate three of
them … how many cakes did I have left …
Child: Seven
Teacher: Seven good girl … I have five pencils … if I put five more in
my tin … five pencils in my tin I put five more in my tin how
many altogether Molly
Child: That’s my job to sort the pencils..
Teacher: Ok can we just work out the answer to the number stories …
I have five pencils … if I put five more in my tin
Child: It is ten that is how many you have in there now …
Teacher: How did you work out that the answer was ten?
Child: I can see them from here … hehe … we had to tidy up
yesterday … remember?
Teacher: You had five and you counted five more good girl … right I
had nine bananas if I gave three of them to my brother how
many bananas were left? … (long pause)
Child: My brother does not like bananas…
Teacher: Ok
Child: Six
In the transcript extract below, Jo has used the understanding she has of
moving on a snake in the playground to understand the question and
work out the answer.
Extract from transcript of sixth lesson by Miss Lora Hunter at St
Paul First School
Teacher: Let’s ask Jo to see if we can work out how to do this … Jo
Child: I got seven on the number line then hopped on three more
like I do on the playground snake on ten
In the transcript below, Peter points to the poster of the hundred square
in the classroom to extend his understanding of counting.
187
Extract from transcript of second lesson by Mrs Jane Marshall at
Argyle Common First School
Teacher: Then this afternoon we are going to some umm maths again
like we did last week … all right a bit of a maths day today …
right start off let’s do some counting … let’s start at three
and we are going to count in tens ok so we all need to be
looking and sitting where we can see the board … ok are we
ready off we go
Teacher and most children: Three
Most children: Thirteen twenty-three thirty-three forty-three fifty-
three sixty-three seventy- three eighty-three ninety-three a
hundred and three
Child: We’ve run out
Teacher: What would come next? After a hundred and three … Devon
Child: Two hundred and three
Teacher: No that’s counting in hundreds counting is tens … look at the
clues three thirteen twenty-three a hundred and three …
Emily
Child: Two hundred and three
Teacher: No that’s counting in hundred Liam?
Child: A hundred and thirteen
Teacher: A hundred and thirteen … what would come next after a
hundred and thirteen? Jay?
Child: A hundred and twenty-three
Teacher: Thank you a hundred and twenty-three … what would come
after a hundred and twenty-three?
Child: Hundred and
Teacher: James hundred and thirty-three
Child: A hundred and thirty-three
Teacher: Brilliant next … Peter
Child: Hundred and forty-three … you can see it is the same [child
points to the number square on the wall]
Teacher: Excellent … ok
Child: I know what’s next hundred and fifty-three
Teacher: Just cos it’s not there doesn’t mean you can’t do it you’ve got
to use the clues all right … it’s our know one thing and get
another thing for nothing … ok six in tens off you go
So far the events have been analysed in the following manner:
firstly into all the relevant events (maths and not maths);
188
then into three broad concepts (group responses, short responses
and lengthier responses);
finally events labelled as lengthier responses above were analysed
again individually and further sorted into concepts depending on the
nature of the responses into – responses with no explanations
which were regrouped with short responses; negative responses to
a task (which revealed that they could be grouped as tasks that
children expressed no familiarity with or stated could not do at all);
responses in which children expressed that they had done the task
before; responses in which children worked with others; and lastly
responses which used other ideas to tackle the task. The last three
concepts were complex with no cohesive understanding emerging,
and hence needed further analysis.
One unavoidable feature of the process is the messy cumbersome moving
of events from one pot to another. This stage of analysis has a lot of
critical theorising and testing of ideas in terms of how these concepts are
formed. The ideas and structure offered by theoretical sampling allowed
me to continue to collect additional data and use content analysis to
exemplify and further understand what was going on in terms of children’s
engagement with mathematics.
Comparison between the explanatory adequacy
of the theoretical constructs and these
additional empirical indicators go on
continuously.
(Draucker, Martsolf, Ross & Rusk, 2007, p. 1137)
189
As a result of this constant reviewing of data already gathered in
conjunction with sorting new data, the concepts developed in even greater
detail and it this that I will consider in the next step of analysis.
Figure 5.6 Granular concepts for have done the task before
Within the lengthier responses (as seen in Figure 5.5), there were many
events in which children were referring to how they understood the tasks
in relation to what they had already experienced of similar tasks (which I
labelled as have done the task before). Within these events, deeper
analysis further revealed that there were different aspects of past
experience that children were using to understand and respond to the
tasks they were being set, as can be seen in Figure 5.6. Below are brief
descriptions of each of these granular concepts illustrated with relevant
extracts from transcripts.
The first granular concept in Figure 5.6 (done before at school) relates to
all of the events which reveal that children are attempting tasks using
their past experience within work that they have carried out in school. The
transcript extract below shows that the way in which Jack was able to
tackle the question being asked was by remembering an exercise that he
190
carried out before in the classroom which supported his understanding of
2D and 3D shapes.
Extract from transcript of third lesson by Mrs Jo Fishily at
Greenville Park Community School
Teacher: Brilliant well done is that what you were going to say Hannah
… Everybody together …
Most children and teacher: Learning about 2D and 3D shapes …
Teacher: Right do that …
Child: We got this before when we did on those things [child waves
hands in a circle] before here on the carpet.
Teacher: Yes yes it was … put your hand up if you can tell me a 2D
shape … put your hand up if you can tell me a 2D shape …
uhh Jack a 2D shape
Child: It’s got to be flat
The next granular concept in Figure 5.6 (done before but not at school)
relates to all of the events which reveal that children are able to attempt
tasks based on their past experience outside school. In the transcript
extract below, the way in which Martha is able to understand and explain
the concept of addition is linked to putting sweets in a cup. This is not an
experience in school, but has supported her understanding of addition.
Extract from transcript of fourth lesson by Mrs Jo Fishily at
Greenville Park Community School
Teacher: That’s ok really don’t fuss … right you are fine … right sitting
up straight right Kealee … Megan what do we mean by
adding what do we do if we are adding?
Child: Adding on
Teacher: Say that once more please …
Child: Adding on
Teacher: What happens if you were adding what are you doing?
Child: Taking away a number
Teacher: Shhh Megan this time …
Child: Taking away …
191
Teacher: Let’s ask someone else … umm Martha do we do if we are
adding … you had your hands up nicely well done …
Child: We put two numbers together to make them bigger
Teacher: You know what Martha said we put two numbers together to
make them bigger … that is true Martha but you can add
more than two numbers we can add two or three or four …
Child: You know on a Friday me and Tom got sweets and put them
all in a cup and there are lots but only on some Friday and
that is lots
Teacher: Ok Martha can add more than two number
Child: Or ten
The next granular concept in Figure 5.6 (done in past) relates to all of the
events in which children are able to attempt tasks based on their past
experience, but are unable to recollect where the experience took place.
In the transcript extract below, the child has not given any more detail in
their answer as to how they know and interpret ideas being explored
other than that they have done lots of these before.
Extract from transcript of sixth lesson by Mrs Sally Crane at Hatton
First School
Child: A hundred a hundred and ten a hundred and twenty a
hundred and thirty a hundred and forty a hundred and fifty a
hundred and sixty a hundred and seventy a hundred and
eighty a hundred and ninety two hundred …
Teacher: I think we better give him a clap for that don’t you (class
clap)
Child: That’s good
Teacher: I think so yes … how did you know how to do it? how did you
know because the number aren’t there … for you to read
Child: Umm well umm
Teacher: Shhh
Child: Umm because umm it is just well it is a hundred and the rest
are down there so you go just go like a hundred and nine and
ten I have done this lots already so it is easy
Teacher: So the numbers are exactly the same aren’t they over a
hundred … it doesn’t matter whether they are over hundred
two hundred or three hundred it’s still ten twenty thirty forty
fifty sixty seventy eighty ninety a hundred …
192
The next granular concept in Figure 5.6 (linked to other people’s
influence) emerging as a result of further analysis of the transcripts was
the influence of interaction with other people that children expressed as a
trigger for remembering ideas which supported understanding of tasks.
Upon closer inspection of the events, there were two key groups of people
who influenced children’s understanding leading to this concept being split
into two further concepts – people in school and people outside school. I
could have left the concept at the stage of remembering as a result of
other people’s influence. However this would not have accurately reflected
the distinct difference between the influences that the children referred to
and were revealed in the data.
In the first transcript extract below, the child has used their experience
with someone from school i.e. the teaching assistant Mrs MacDonald, to
support her ability to add. In the second transcript extract below, Victoria
recalls having skipped with her child minder Charlotte as a way of
remembering odd numbers.
Extract from transcript of fifth lesson by Mrs Jennie Brooks at
Draycott First School
Teacher: Or ten you can do that but we are just going to do two … Yes
so Martha said you put two numbers together to make them
bigger … that’s adding … adding up numbers … right listen
right … write down on your board … ready Harry three …
write a number three … come along … write it three add two
add four … who can work it out for me?
Child: Mrs MacDonald told me before that it is nine
193
Extract from transcript of seventh lesson by Mrs Sally Crane at
Hatton First School
Teacher: Now I just want to spend two minutes seeing if we can work
out and remember what we did yesterday with the odd and
the even numbers … can anybody tell me what the odd
numbers were that we looked at when we first looked at
them between zero and up to ten … which are the odd
numbers? Victoria?
Child: One three five seven nine
Teacher: Good girl that’s very good well remembered … yes let’s say
them together …
Child: I know all of them I skip with Charlotte and we count
[Charlotte is her child minder]
The final granular concept in Figure 5.6 (wrong before and now
remembered) relates to events which show self-correction from what the
children recalled and the mistakes they had made. In the case of the
transcript extract below, Megan self-corrects in relation to the units she is
referring to and also clearly understands that she has difficulty recalling
and using correct units.
Extract from transcript of sixth lesson by Mrs Rebecca Rice at St
Paul First School
Teacher: Oh we’re doing doubles Megan … so you’ve got seven in one
hand you are going to have …
Child: Seven
Teacher: Because a double is exactly the same number isn’t it? yeah?
so if I’ve got … oh it’s really really heavy … five hundred in
this hand … what am I going to have in this hand?
Child: Five … no no I know I keep forgetting the other bit …
hundred
In summary, all the granular concepts in Figure 5.6 are recollections of
having done something similar before, but are very different in nature
leading to the granular concepts that I have just described and illustrated.
However this did not account for all the events within the lengthier
responses.
194
Figure 5.7 Granular concepts for worked with others and other
ideas for tackling task
The last two concepts under lengthier responses labelled worked with
others and other ideas for tackling task have been introduced earlier in
this section with some sample transcript extracts. However as the process
of analysis continued and events were being reviewed, it emerged that
these needed further filtering and separating into greater detail as there
were aspects within each of these concepts which gave further insight into
what children brought to bear upon the tasks they were performing. I will
now consider each of the three resulting granular concepts – some form of
model or image, different interpretations and child got wrong answer –
separately (Figure 5.7).
There were a group of events which highlighted that when children carried
out a task, they were using some form of model or image to support their
understanding. These were often complex and linked to how children had
understood the ideas originally. As there were many different types of
models or images that children were using, I was able to sort them
initially into crude groups such as in school and out of school, then
consider further what each of these were telling me about how children
195
approached the task. This content analysis resulted in the concepts shown
in Figure 5.8 below.
Figure 5.8 Granular concepts of some form of model or image
There were events where children used words to support the meaning that
they derived about the task at hand. As can be seen in the transcript
extract below, the child has linked to the word lose, in this case by
motioning to put pens in the bin, to understand subtraction.
Extract from transcript of sixth lesson by Mrs Jill Thomas at St
Paul First School
Teacher: You added three more … all right I think if we knew we had
ten to begin with … no I’ll leave that bit … I’ll leave that …
let’s think … I have got six …
Child: Six what …
Teacher: Six felt pens … if I lose three of them Jonathan how many
will I have left?
Child: Umm … (long pause) … can you say it again …
Teacher: I’ll say it again I had six felt pens if I lose three of them how
many will I have left?
Child: Put them in the bin [child makes a motion of throwing
something away] ok lost them … so they have gone away …
three
There were instances in the events which highlighted the use of objects to
support children’s understanding of the questions being asked. They also
supported the explanations that children gave to rationalise the answer.
196
In the transcript extract below, the understanding of counting in 5’s is
linked to the child’s image of a clock.
Extract from transcript of seventh lesson by Mrs Jennie Brooks at
Draycott First School
Teacher: No right what did you notice about the numbers you counted
and the numbers they counted
Child: (shouts out) I know they had the clock number and we had
the tens …
Teacher: Well done you mean they had the fives …
Continuing to look at a range of events, there were a cluster of events
appearing where children had used the images of people in various ways
to support tackling the task. However this was very different to the
previously identified influence of people (Figure 5.6). In that case, people
were a direct reminder of what they had done before. In this concept, the
events highlighted that people formed an image or model to support
understanding. The model being used in this set of events relies on people
within school and is referenced to school. In contrast, there were events
where children had linked their understanding of the task and how they
would answer it to people outside of school. In the first transcript extract
below, Mitchell is using the class and the daily routine of working out how
many dinners to understand counting on. In the second transcript below,
Josh is using the image of his father (a person outside school) to
determine the bigger number.
Extract from transcript of fifth lesson by Mrs Jane Marshall at
Argyle Common First School
Teacher: … eighteen and what makes twenty? you need to put
eighteen into our heads and count on until we get to twenty
… … (long pause) … I can see some people really wanting to
join in Mitchell what do you think … ?
Child: Two
197
Teacher: Can you show me how you did it?
Child: Umm twenty dinners [child points around the room] and
sometimes Jade and Poppy have sandwiches so I counted
back
Teacher: Counted back from where?
Child: Twenty dinners
Teacher: Right so can you do it so it’s …
Child: Two …
Teacher: Hang on Mitchell cause twenty’s got to go in your head … so
…
Teacher and child: Twenty … nineteen eighteen [child points to two
children as he counts] …
Extract from transcript of seventh lesson by Mrs Jill Thomas at St
Paul First School
Teacher: Let’s look at which number is bigger can you hold up with
your number fans the number which is bigger … 31 or 27…
(long pause) good Josh why do you have 31
Child: My daddy is 31 and he is big
As the events were being filtered through the different stages of sorting,
there were a range of events which showed that children use physical
places to support their understanding. Furthermore, the data led me to
subdivide these events into school and outside school. In the first
transcript extract below, Emily uses the classroom routine to help with
counting. In the second transcript extract below, the child uses the racing
game in his room to describe a shape with no straight lines as the answer.
Extract from transcript of fifth lesson by Mrs Jane Marshall at
Argyle Common First School
Teacher and few children: Nineteen eighteen seventeen sixteen fifteen
…
Teacher: Emily
Child: Fifteen
Teacher: Well done … roll the dice … right Emily … two … what number
am I going to put in my head to start with?
Child: Eighteen
198
Teacher: Why?
Child: We do that when we register and count the class
Teacher: Umm what number am I going to put in my head to start
with? Jay
Extract from transcript of sixth lesson by Mrs Jo Fishily at
Greenville Park Community School
Teacher: Corner wasn’t it? you said some of them have got corners
and some of them have got one smooth side … so which ones
got the smooth side? you tell me
Child: Rectangles
Teacher: What’s got one smooth curved side … no points no corners …
no what are these called?
Teacher: No stttraight sides … straight lines has a circle got any
straight lines?
Child: No …
Teacher: Right if you’ve so that shape has got no straight sides
Child: It’s got curvy line like this [child makes a swerving pattern] I
have that in a racing game in my room …
The last concept – similarity – refers to what has been seen in the task as
a very similar image for the child to one they have already developed. In
the transcript extract below, the teacher starts by developing an image of
a grid with the children to support their understanding of division. The
child towards the end of the transcript extract has linked this grid to his
understanding and the similarity between arrays and multiplication to
arrive at the answer for the division sum.
Extract from transcript of ninth lesson by Mrs Jane Marshall at
Argyle Common First School
[The class were doing division using a grid]
Teacher: What am I going to do now that I have drawn the grid … to
find the first number in my sum?
Child: Count the squares
Teacher: Good come on then …
Child: One two three four five six seven eight nine ten eleven
twelve thirteen fourteen fifteen sixteen seventeen eighteen
199
Teacher: Brilliant eighteen put an eighteen here Shannon one number
per square …
Child: They are like Mr Marshall’s eight
Teacher: Much better than Mr Marshall’s eight (laughs) he does two
circles that’s naughty … so what am I going to do next …
Henry what do you think?
Child: You need to put the divided sign
Teacher: Brilliant so it’s eighteen shared by … divided by … shared
between ok so what’s the next thing I am going to do …?
Nathan?
Child: Count down … umm (laughs)
Teacher: Count down ok
Child: Six
Child: You know this it’s the one on to that’s three and then you go
down there and there’s three on the top and then on the
bottom it will be six
Teacher: Ok what am I going to put next …
Child: Six
Teacher: Ok well done and what am I going to do find the answer
Child: Count the number across
Teacher: Ok
Child: Three
Teacher: Now excellent so now I’ve got eighteen squares altogether …
I counted how many down?
Child: Six
Teacher: And how many across?
Child: Three
Teacher: And that’s my divide sign my share between sign … but look
do you remember when you did it with grids to make it times
I counted down
Child: Yep
Child: I knew that sum all along … it was because I did a times
making eighteen … six times three equals eighteen …
As I continued with the constant comparison approach, there was another
pattern appearing in the data which was quite different to the concepts
discussed so far – have done the task before and some form of model or
image. In this case, it was the way in which children had interpreted the
200
tasks in that they were not using something they recalled or an image or
model, but in some sense translating what they were being asked to do. I
was able to sort these into granular concepts, as seen in Figure 5.9. Using
examples from the transcripts, I will now illustrate the nature of each of
these concepts.
Figure 5.9 Granular concepts of different interpretations
Sensory experiences were events in which children used their physical
experiences such as things they had done or seen or heard or felt to make
sense of their mathematics. In the transcript extract below, the child has
interpreted addition in terms of a physical act which is linked to his
experience of playing football. Removing the zero and adding a five is not
used as a model or an image in this case, but as a physical sensory
process which has helped him to address the need of the task.
Extract from transcript of eighth lesson by Mrs Rebecca Rice at St
Paul First School
Teacher: You know we did this before you do ten add ten and then
take away three it’s …
Child: Seventeen
Teacher: Ok so what is ten add five
Child: Kick the zero off like a football and put the five there … [child
brushes his hand and draws a 1 then moves the 5 in the air
next to the 1 he has drawn in the air] … fifteen
Teacher: Interesting … what were you doing
Child: Adding
201
Words were events where children were making their interpretations
based on the way in which they understood the words in different tasks.
In the transcript extract below, there is a literal interpretation of the word
up as the child has moved up the hundred square, and thus not been able
to complete the task. The hundred square does present a particular
problem in its layout as is illustrated here. The numbers go up in value as
you move down the hundred square, leading to the confusion experienced
by the child in the words used by the teacher to set the task. There is a
clear misinterpretation of the word up based on the child’s understanding
in terms of physical movement and how it is different to the movement on
a hundred square.
Extract from transcript of seventh lesson by Mrs Sally Crane at
Hatton First School
Teacher: Yes I know … now then let’s just you two turn round Jake
and Owain and look at the hundred square for a minute …
Ellie … now on here we’ve got all the numbers that we’ve just
been umm counting and we’ve got a pattern … if you
remember when we did a pattern of tens … last term we
noticed it was just one line didn’t it … now what we are going
to do is we are going to work out our pattern of fives and see
whether we can see something happening on our hundred
square … Ellie am I going to have to have you sitting by me
Child: But there is a spider on there
Teacher: All right ok we’ll put him outside now then let’s count first of
all let’s count in fives using the number square …
Child: Five ten fifteen …
Teacher: Right we’ve got to number fifteen I am going to put a circle
round number fifteen let’s count up another five ok
Child: I am off the hundred square.
Teacher: What do you mean?
Child: Look [child starts counting going up the hundred square from
fifteen to five as one count] … see no more space
Teacher: What number do we get to?
Child: Ten
Teacher: To ten ok …
202
Child: There is a spider there
Teacher: Never mind … now let’s see if we can count on … Oliver
another five … for me one
Few children: … two three
Teacher: Excuse me I said Oliver … come on Oli
Child: One two three four five …
Teacher: And what number do I get to?
Similarities were ideas and interpretations that children had already made
about other notions of mathematics and were applying them to new tasks.
Similarities refer to the ability to use related and unrelated known facts
and interpret them to support the task. In the transcript extract below,
understanding the similarity between 4 + ? = 10 and 14 + ? = 20 has
been used by this child to address the task. He has interpreted the two
possible ways of looking at the task as being similar in supporting the
outcome.
Extract from transcript of fifth lesson by Mrs Jane Marshall at
Argyle Common First School
Teacher: You were right weren’t you … seven … (long pause) … let’s
try this one? … (long pause) … fourteen add what makes
twenty? so you put fourteen in your head shhh … put it down
you can’t have number fans in your hands cause you need
them for counting … fourteen in your heads and count on till
you get to twenty … (long pause) …
Child: Mrs Marshall it is easy it’s …
Teacher: I’ll come to you in a moment I know what you’re going to say
… Liam?
Child: Umm six
Teacher: You are well on the ball now … you’ve got it haven’t you
Emily? … it is six Devon what are you going say?
Child: It’s changing the fourteen over to a four and then it’s easy …
The set of further events to fall into the concept of perceived challenge
are events where children have made their own interpretations as to the
203
nature and level of difficulty of a given task. In the transcript extract
below, the level of difficulty perceived by Emma has been done by
observing that the number ten contains two digits. Emma does not want
to attempt to consider possible answers as she has interpreted the task to
be of a higher level and difficult. This has stopped her from engaging in
the task.
Extract from transcript of seventh lesson by Mrs Rebecca Rice at
St Paul First School
Teacher: Good boy that is called reversing it isn’t it? changing it
around … (long pause) … so is that it? one two three four five
six seven eight nine … well done shall we do number ten the
last one … ok very very quickly then … tens Emily oh sorry
Emma …
Child: Umm … (long pause) … ten … that has two numbers so … it is
not the same … that is harder …
Teacher: Someone else have a try number bonds for ten
Child: Ten add nothing … (long pause) … ok James … ?
Child: Five and five
Teacher: Good boy!! that’s the double isn’t it … five add five fantastic
… Robbie?
Child: Zero and ten
Teacher: Zero and ten right that’s that one reversed …
Child: Six and four
Teacher: Six and four brilliant … (long pause) … Ella?
Child: Umm … (long pause) … four and six …
Teacher: Four and six that’s that one reversed well done …
Child: Six and eight
Teacher: Six and eight make fourteen … it’s a bit too big … (long
pause) … Joe?
Child: Three and seven
Teacher: Three and seven … Philip can you reverse that one for me
change that round
Child: (long pause) … seven and three
204
Nature and presentation refers to events where the way in which the task
has been set has influenced directly the way in which the child has
interpreted the task. In the transcript extract below, the initial
presentation of the task has forced the child concerned to make very
interesting interpretations in relation to the question being asked about
the value of money. The child brings to bear upon the task the idea that
ten pence have ten pennies squeezed into them, and this has caused
complications for further questions asked as the nature with which the
idea was presented instigated a particular interpretation of what value
means in terms of coins.
Extract from transcript of seventh lesson by Mrs Rebecca Rice at
St Paul First School
Teacher: (working with a small group on the carpet) … shh shh shh
shh shh shh you’ve worked really fast … with your number
bonds and we do a quick introduction on money then we get
to our tables and do our work ok … all right I am going to
hold up some coins and I want you to tell me what coin I’m
holding up if you can whisper so we don’t bother everyone …
(whispers) … ok let’s start off with that one …
Child: (whispers) One ‘p’
Teacher: A one ‘p’ that’s right a one penny piece … (whispers) … what
about …
Child: (whispers) Ten
Teacher: A ten penny piece … Philip thought it might have been a two
… why isn’t it a two?
Child: (whispers) Cause it’s silver
Child: (whispers) Cause it’s round
Teacher: Cause it’s silver Ellis? what colour would a two ‘p’ piece be
Ellis
Child: Umm brown and that’s a ten ‘p’
Teacher: How do you know it’s a ten ‘p’
Child: Cause the number
Teacher: Oh what number can you see Ellis?
Child: Ten …
Teacher: Oh very good does that mean ten bananas?
205
Child: No
Teacher: What does that mean … ten what?
Child: Ten ‘p’
Teacher: Ten ‘p’ so inside this … well not really but we could pretend
inside this ten pence someone in the shh factory has got ten
little pennies and gone uhhhgggg and squeezed them right
into that ten pence piece …
Child: Do they really do that … so how come it is silver?
Teacher: Well it is just pretend … it just means that this is the same as
having ten little pennies ok … shh right … let’s find that two
pence piece then here we are … here’s a two pence piece …
so how many little pennies are squashed into a two pence
Isaac?
Child: (long pause) … ten …
Teacher: Ten pennies?...
Child: It is big and you can squash ten pennies
Teacher: Try again Ellis
Child: Two
Teacher: That’s right two pennies have been squashed … right here is
a very very tiny coin which always get caught in the corner of
my purse … so I can never find them … what is this …
Child: A five
Teacher: A five pence piece … that’s right … this is a five pence piece
… Isaac how many pennies is a five pence piece worth … ?
right if I said to you I’ll give you pennies we’ll do a swap how
many pennies would I give you to give this to me?
Child: Five
Teacher: Good boy … five … what about this funny shaped coin?
Child: Twenty
Teacher: Well done Charlotte … it’s a twenty pence piece … you’re
quite right … a twenty pence piece and how many pennies
are squashed into this Isaac?
Child: It is very small so I think … five pennies
Within the concept have done the task before (Figure 5.9), there are a
number of events which were attributed to having done a similar task
before and therefore needed further unravelling as there was already a
detailed mechanism for considering in depth what such events revealed.
206
They were considered through the have done the task before pathway
discussed earlier in conjunction with Figure 5.6.
Figure 5.10 Granular concepts of child got answer wrong
The last cluster of events were events where children got the answer
incorrect (Figure 5.10). In the process of analysis, I left all such responses
initially to one side as it was not clear what they would have to offer. But
as the analysis process continued and I examined these events closely, it
was evident that there were a number of events where children were
confident in what they were saying and did not know that they were
incorrect. These could be further explored depending on the nature of the
explanation through the different pathways already mentioned. There was
another group of emerging events in which children had a clear idea of the
error they had made and what this error may have been. These events
could also be filtered through the same pathway as any of the other
events had been for the lengthier responses. It was not important to the
understanding of prior knowledge whether children got the answer right or
wrong, but more important was to understand the journey that the child
had made to gain their own understanding. Using the transcripts to listen
207
to children and how they established their understanding was key to
making sense of the data.
As with any research, an important question was when to stop the
analysis process for my research. This is a very simple problem to solve. I
stopped analysing data when no more new concepts were being formed.
As I progressed through the analysis, each new transcript contained many
new events but these fell into the same established concepts. Therefore
the only purpose being served through greater analysis was an increasing
volume of events, but no new understanding. After analysing 46
transcripts, there was no new information being gained through the data
and therefore theoretical saturation had been reached.
Additional analysis no longer contributes to
anything new about a concept. In this way, the
resulting theory is considered conceptually
dense and grounded in the data
(Schwandt, 2001, p. 111)
In order to pull all of these threads of analysis together, it is important at
this stage to look at where we are. The constant comparison method,
which encourages reflective and analytical thinking, supported the sorting,
analysing and rationalisation of the data into concepts. Many different
events ended up at the same point, thus forming concepts and ideas of
how and what children were using to support their understanding and
tackling of mathematical tasks. The process was refined many times and
the paths presented here are a result of these stages of refinement. It
should be possible, using these paths, to take any new event from the
classroom and define the path it takes to reach the endpoint of a
208
particular concept. At this stage of the analysis, I was able to sort all of
the transcripts ensuring that each event has been placed into a concept
which could not be refined any further, and all the events in any endpoint
concept have the same features. Now we need to understand the role that
these endpoint concepts play in defining prior knowledge.
Table 5.2 List of endpoint concepts
Response with no Not familiar Cannot do it
explanation
Done it before in Done it before but not Done it in the past
school in school
People in school People outside school Was wrong before and
have now remembered
Words Objects School
Outside school Sensory experiences Similarity
Perceived challenge Nature and
presentation
Thus far, the process of analysing the transcripts has been done using
content analysis to dictate where they should be grouped. I have relied on
my experience in the classroom to interpret and to some extent identify
these concepts. This is consistent with the discussion at the start of this
section that within grounded theory, the researcher can use her
knowledge to guide the analysis process. I next considered each of the
events within a concept and across concepts to determine if these
concepts could be grouped and labelled to attain a best fit description of
what my data were suggesting in order to understand prior knowledge
and how it may function within an individual.
209
5.5.5 Developing the Model
This section will consider all the endpoint concepts (Table 5.2) and how
they support the emergence of categories for the structural and functional
understanding of prior knowledge. To understand what each of the
endpoint concepts was revealing about how children were dealing with the
tasks presented, it was essential to consider the commonalities,
differences and characteristics through constant comparison to allow the
nature of prior knowledge to emerge. This was achieved by looking at
each group of concepts and assessing them based on the following
criteria:
What are the common properties, if any, in each of these concepts?
Should these concepts be combined?
Do the data show that there is interdependency between concepts?
Do the data show any interaction between concepts?
Having many disparate concepts did not allow understanding to be gained
in a comprehensive manner as to what was taking place when children
were attempting tasks. So far, from careful listening to children, it has
emerged that children bring many factors in order to address how they
approach a task. Also different children did not bring the same methods to
support their understanding, but a variety of mechanisms as can be seen
by the concepts developed. At this stage, it was important to see if there
is any pattern in these concepts to try and establish a working framework
of what constitutes prior knowledge. There is a need to begin to describe
what is taking place in each of these concepts, since at present they are
210
just a collection of common events. Analysing the function of each
concept, and giving a label to concepts which have commonalities will
support description and allow further exploration of ideas in terms of
understanding prior knowledge.
When looking at all the data, the dominant commonality which ran
through each event, and thus all concepts, was the notion of recollection.
Children were recollecting from memory what they needed to address
each of the tasks. This can be seen in the transcripts in the previous
section. E.g. in the transcript on page 178, the child is able to respond to
the question without any support, thus recollecting from memory; in the
transcript on page 179, the child is working out using various stages and
is supported by what he is recalling to complete the task set. There is
dependency upon memory in each event identified in the analysis process.
Through the data, it has emerged that there is a difference in the nature
of what is being recalled with each task and therefore it is important to
describe each of these concepts through the use of a shared common
language. Being led by the data, the formation of the emerging prior
knowledge model has two key steps:
i. To look for common patterns between concepts through use of the
criteria posed earlier and to formulate categories (groups of related
concepts) to allow understanding of prior knowledge to emerge.
ii. After establishing the categories, to describe and explore them in a
way that supports understanding of prior knowledge.
Using the process of comparing the characteristics of each concept, I was
able to group concepts and to start describing what was taking place. The
211
evidence from listening to children as to how they were understanding
and developing methods to deal with tasks was being revealed. In order
to see how categories emerged through comparison of concepts, I am
going to consider in detail how one category was established.
First, I considered all the events within the concept cannot do it, two of
which can be seen in the transcript extracts below.
Extract from transcript of fifth lesson by Mrs Jo Fishily at
Greenville Park Community School
Teacher: I’ll be there in a second … ok ready … right … now show them
Aiden … ok everyone write down their guess … under my
guess … let me see … you think seven and Martha says …
Child: I can’t do it …
Teacher: Have a go …
Child: I can’t do this …
Teacher: Have a guess …
Child: Six
Teacher: OK … right shall we count them now
Few children in group: One two three four five six seven …
Teacher: Seven … so in that box you write … seven … well done spot
on … Martha was
Child: Too less …
Teacher: One too few … and Aiden you were a bit too high … and you
did it …
Extract from transcript of sixth lesson by Mrs Jo Fishily at
Greenville Park Community School
Teacher: Cylinder … right can you put your hand up if you notice
anything about what is left on my white board this morning …
Jack … what do you notice about what is left on my white
board [the question put on the board was 9+3 =11 children
were asked to consider the question] this morning cause
you’re talking … (long pause) … what do you notice Jack?
Child: Umm I don’t know…it’s too hard… I don’t know…
Teacher: Make a guess
Child: I don’t know
Teacher: Right anyone help … Jack right Hannah what do you notice
212
In these two transcript extracts (the first transcript extract was also seen
earlier on page 181), the children have decided that the task is too
difficult or beyond their ability, and as a result have made none or limited
effort to address the task. They are unable to recollect any approach that
may support them in addressing the task. The perceived level of difficulty
is based on their inability to recall ideas to help address the task or to
decipher the question. This on its own does not give any further
understanding of prior knowledge other than the obvious conclusion that
there are tasks which children find incomprehensible and therefore make
a limited attempt to solve.
Secondly, when carrying out comparison between the characteristics of
this concept and the remaining concepts, commonalities emerged
between cannot do it and perceived challenge. The two transcript extracts
below comprise some of the events analysed as the latter.
Extract from transcript of first lesson by Mrs Jo Fishily at
Greenville Park Community School
[Children are using a 100 square playing various games]
Teacher: Right who could roll the dice for me? … then we’re gonna
move the button … that many times ok … we’re going
forwards … counting … Josh would you like to roll? just stay
where you are, stay where you are and see if you can roll it
onto the floor … oh what’s it landed on?
Some children: (shout) Six
Teacher: Right, who can put their hand up and guess where I’m going
to have to move button to? … uh let me ask somebody with
their hand up … Louise
Child: Six
Teacher: Yeah, shall we see if you are right? Can you count with me?
Some children: One two three four five six
213
Teacher: Good girl Louise, right … (whispers) who can roll the dice this
time? … (normal) shh … let’s have Kealee can you roll it onto
the dice onto the snake, ready? … ok … oops pass it to
Kealee … ok don’t worry, you’re gonna have your own dice in
a minute if you don’t get a turn now … ooh … what’s that
landed on?
Some children: (shout) Four
Child: Easy … are we going to get to play this today?
Teacher: Yes four (child makes a fist and punches the air with a smile)
… right put your hand up if you can work out already where
my blue bead’s going to be? … let me ask somebody with
their hand up … let me ask Aiden
Child: Worked it out already its ten …
Extract from transcript of seventh lesson by Mrs Rebecca Rice at
St Paul First School
Teacher: Good boy that is called reversing it isn’t it? changing it
around … (long pause) … so is that it? one two three four five
six seven eight nine … well done shall we do number ten the
last one … ok very very quickly then … tens Emily oh sorry
Emma …
Child: Umm … (long pause) … ten … that has two numbers so … it is
not the same … that is harder …
Teacher: Someone else have a try number bonds for ten
In the first extract, we see that the way in which Aiden approaches the
task is dependent upon the level of ease that he perceives the question to
have, which in this case is positive and easy. In the second extract (part
of this extract was also seen earlier on page 204), Emma has approached
the task with a preconceived notion based on her experience that a two-
digit number will make the question too difficult for her to attempt.
Though these two concepts are very different – cannot do it is a clear
statement from children without any explanation or detailed
understanding of what they cannot do or why; perceived challenge is an
indication of the level of ease or difficulty with which the child perceives a
task which then calibrates the attempt that children make – they both
214
have a common dimension in the approaches that children are using to
determine the outcome in terms of the level of effort they put into a task.
In order to describe what is going on, there are two key factors – first the
individual, and second the level of motivation that is derived by the
individual when interpreting the task at hand. These two factors are
apparent in both concepts and common to both concepts. Therefore, I
have grouped them into the category individual motivation. This label best
describes the characteristics of the two concepts in that the dominant
factor in how and what is being recalled is linked to the level of motivation
that the individual feels as a result of looking at the task.
Within these two concepts, there were many other events which were
similar to the examples quoted above. All the events are related to how
the children were motivated by their view of the task. It could be argued
that these events are about how children are perceiving the task, and
these concepts should be grouped with other concepts which show
different interpretations of the tasks made by children based on
perception, e.g. words, objects, or similarity. However when comparing
the events within the concepts where children’s perception of the task is
also considered, as can be seen in the transcript extract below (which was
analysed as belonging to the concept objects), the clear difference
identified is that in this example the child’s thoughts are not structured by
their perception of individual ability, but by how they relate the task to the
object ribbons. Therefore events within the concepts of cannot do it and
perceived challenge are very different in nature to events within the
concepts words, objects and similarity, as they are reliant upon the
motivation derived from considering the task itself.
215
Extract from transcript of first lesson by Mrs Helen Fellows at
Greenville Park Community School
Teacher: It is the shortest piece … but why don’t you think it is the
shortest? … why have you got a different idea Katie?
Child: Because it is longer than that piece
Teacher: It’s longer than which piece Katie?
Child: This
Teacher: Good girl … even though it’s a short piece it is longer than
others so this one must be the shortest … Mr Collins and Pam
could you be the stands for the washing line for a minute? …
John … does it matter that the washing line has moved?
Child: Yes
Teacher: Have ribbons gone any different sizes?
Child: Yes … look that one looks shorter now….
Teacher: Have we cut any off?
Child: No
Teacher: No have we put any in the bin?
Child: Its washing it got small in the wash… so now it is smaller.
Teacher: So let’s check a minute who’s sitting really beautifully … Kurt
can you tell me which is the shortest? … can you pick
someone to find which is the shortest? … we can measure it
in… in … what
Child: Ruler
Teacher: Not quite what do we call this measure?
Child: Meter
In order to allow for common understanding of all these concepts, I have
applied a best-fit label for groups of concepts which have similar
properties. The collection of concepts were analysed by using this
constant comparison method for all of my concepts. I identified similarities
and common properties across them and derived eight categories which
described what children were bringing at the point of tackling tasks. The
categories are abstraction, acculturation, cognition, context,
individual motivation, metacognition, perception and social group.
The mapping from concepts to categories can be seen in Table 5.3 below.
216
Table 5.3 Mapping from concepts to categories
Concepts Categories
Nature and presentation Abstraction
Sensory experiences
Done before at school Acculturation
People in school
School
Response with no explanation Cognition
Not familiar
Done in the past
Done before but not at school Context
Cannot do it Individual motivation
Perceived challenge
Was wrong before and have now remembered Metacognition
Words Perception
Objects
Similarity
People outside school Social group
Outside school
Having considered all the concepts using the method described above,
and assessing them for commonalities and differences, I have organised
them into the eight categories listed earlier. The eight categories are
interlinked, and are all linked to the central category of memory. This
concludes the description of the analysis process, and the next section
examines the ethical considerations arising from the analysis process.
217
5.6 Ethical Considerations
A multitude of ethical considerations were taken into account in the
analysis of the data, as per the ethical guidelines for educational research
from the British Educational Research Association (2004). The process of
using grounded theory and content analysis to analyse my data raises
some key ethical issues, which have also been identified by Lincoln and
Guba (1985a) in relation to qualitative data analysis – credibility,
transferability, dependability, and confirmability of the data. Described
below is how I have addressed each of these in my analysis process.
Credibility of the data was established by spending prolonged time (i.e.
regular visits over one whole academic year) in each classroom. This
allowed me to become oriented to and appreciate the nature and culture
of each of the classrooms. A further benefit was that it allowed me to
blend into the classroom and ensure that teachers and the children felt
comfortable with my presence. This consistent presence meant that, as
debated in Section 4.2.3, I was an “observer-as-a-participant” (Gold,
1958, p. 217) and the frequency of my visits to the classroom ensured
that I blended into the culture of the setting and allowed me to gather
data in its truest form. Also by asking teachers to review the transcripts, it
allowed them to establish that I was interested in portraying the truth and
establishing accuracy.
Transferability is important in ensuring that the data gathered has scope
in wider understanding. I have achieved this by using what Lincoln and
Guba (1985a) call “thick description” (p. 125). Thick description entails
218
giving a detailed picture of the data and context that allows any reader to
be able to completely place themselves and understand the positioning of
the research. The ideal way in which this could be achieved is to report all
data as recorded with as many points of reference which allow us to build
an accurate picture of the context in which the data were gathered
without any alterations to the data presented. However this directly
contradicts the need for anonymity of the participants and settings. I
settled this dilemma in my research by choosing carefully how I
anonymised my data. E.g. the revised names for the teachers were
chosen to be culturally identical to their actual names in order not to
change the nature of the possible picture that may be established by the
reader. The school details presented in Section 4.2.1 and Appendix A were
altered to ensure that they could not be identified while keeping intact the
actual nature of the schools. Furthermore the data to be presented as
findings were done so with the focus on providing a clear and full picture
of the points being considered and in no way to identify the school,
individual teachers or children. I aimed to provide the data in as full a
form as possible so that readers could come to their own understanding of
what the data are showing and how, so that they may, if needed, use the
outcomes in their own practice.
Dependability during the process of analysis is achieved by revisiting ideas
and concepts constantly. As shown in Figure 5.2, there was a constant
process of checking and comparison to ensure that interpretations made
against new data were consistent and could be repeated. The process of
theory generation requires repetition of the sorting and concept forming
process. The data in my research were analysed, sorted and compared
219
many times in order to ensure that the interpretations made of the data
were in line with all the data collected. Grounded theory procedures force
me to ensure, through the constant comparison mechanism, that there is
dependability in the outcome.
Confirmability requires me to be as neutral as possible and ensure that
there is no bias in the process of analysis and complete traceability in the
use of data. Though clearly I am interested in looking to gain an
understanding of prior knowledge in the mathematics classroom, this in
itself leads to a bias in terms of which aspects of the data I will be
considering as not all data collected would be of use. However in the
process of collecting the data, I was clear to my participants that I was
looking at only the interactions relating to mathematics within the lesson
and also ensured that they were constantly aware of the developing
theory. Furthermore, through the initial meetings with the participants, I
shared my research perspective, beliefs, values and position in relation to
the research I was carrying out. Also I shared with them some of my
ontological and epistemological assumptions, and how these have led to
the methodology selected for the research. In terms of my analysis
process, I have maintained complete traceability from my raw data (i.e.
transcripts) to events to concepts to categories.
5.7 Summary
There were two great challenges in this chapter. The first was to select an
appropriate methodology from a range of qualitative data analysis
methodologies to support the understanding and development of prior
220
knowledge. The selected methodology had to meet a set of criteria which
were established from the outset. The second challenge was to explain
how the data were analysed through the methodology selected and any
ethical considerations arising out of the analysis. I explored different ideas
provided by dominant paradigms for analysing my data and found a
blended approach which suited the criteria – an approach which used
content analysis to understand what was being said and grounded theory
to order, structure and support formulation of a theory.
This chapter has been procedural in merely giving the instructions for
developing the partial model, and not the model itself. These instructions
are not prescriptive, but are descriptive to help understand the broad
range of data being considered, how sense is made of these data through
theoretical sampling, and use of constant comparison as it is consistent
with the principles of grounded theory. Through the use of examples from
transcripts, I have illustrated the process of how I carried out the actual
analysis which has resulted in the partial model established from a range
of contributory elements presented in the next chapter.
221
222
6 PRIOR KNOWLEDGE MODEL
6.1 Introduction
This chapter explains the overall outcome from the research and analysis
carried out for this thesis. I am going to define this entity that I am calling
prior knowledge, and present the structure of my partial prior knowledge
model which has emerged from my data, looking at its form, function and
key features.
I will look at my model from its core to its periphery, looking first at the
individual categories (or elements) that have emerged through the
analysis of events and concepts, then considering how all of these
categories link together and function, finally exploring a possible structure
for prior knowledge. The individual categories are not considered in any
particular sequence as they do not have any order or hierarchy within the
partial model. I will define my model starting with the central category of
memory and then the three categories –acculturation, context and
metacognition – which emerged strongly in my data. For each of these
categories, I will first examine its theoretical underpinning by presenting a
thumbnail of the extensive work carried out by generations of researchers
in that area. For each category, I am fitting the understanding of the
category into existing theoretical frameworks in order to give an
overarching picture of the links between my category and theory. This
theoretical perspective is followed by my own definition of the category
illustrated empirically using numerous extracts from the transcripts.
223
Furthermore, I will look at five other categories which are also emerging
through the data – abstraction, cognition, individual motivation,
perception and social group. These will be discussed in a similar structure
as the first three categories, but in far less depth.
It is important to note that the proposed eight categories cannot be
claimed to be a definitive list of features of prior knowledge, but can only
be a partial model which has been established through the range of
contributory elements in the context of my data. Though the description
of my partial prior knowledge model is linear due to the limitations of the
presentation medium (this paper-based thesis), the actual prior
knowledge model itself is complex and multi-dimensional.
6.2 Memory
6.2.1 Theoretical Perspective
Research into memory and how it functions is extensive and broad. As far
back as Plato and Aristotle, thought has been given to how we were able
to learn, build our understanding and make links with what we
experience. There are a number of disciplines and views on what memory
is and how it functions – biological, psychological, social and cultural.
Though there is much complexity in the form and function of memory and
many subtle definitions, overall memory is about the retention,
reactivation and reconstruction of experiences. Memory contains two
components – the behavioural or conscious level, and the underpinning
physical neural changes – which impact on what is recalled, or in very
simplistic terms, encoding, storage and retrieval (Dudai, 2007).
224
The word “memory” is misleading. Being a
single word, it creates the impression that it
refers to a single entity. ... Memory is not
unitary. There are many dimensions along which
different types of memory can be classified.
(Yuret, 1995, p. 1)
On a simplistic level, all types of memory are influenced and built upon
through experiences and the construction of ideas through these
experiences.
‘Memory’ labels a diverse set of cognitive
capacities by which we retain information and
reconstruct past experiences, usually for present
purposes.
(Sutton, 2010)
Of greater interest, how is the information organised and developed in the
brain or how is memory modified? My data suggest that there is no
consistent method or logical process to the organisation of ideas.
A picture of interlocked systems have started to
emerge that support human memory function.
(Yuret, 1995, p. 9)
6.2.2 Definition
For the purposes of my prior knowledge model, the function of memory in
its elemental form, defined earlier as a mechanism for retention,
reactivation and reconstruction of experiences, is adequate.
Emerging from my data, the first noticeable link between all events
identified through the analysis was that children were recalling
information from their memory to support them in their mathematical
225
tasks. Every event relied on some form of recollection from memory.
Therefore the central category for my prior knowledge model is memory.
The question to consider next is – what are the factors that are shaping
memory – as whatever is modifying memory shapes what children bring
to bear on each task, and I am calling this prior knowledge. The data
show that there are many different ways in which children solve similar
mathematical tasks. They do not consistently use one method to manage
the tasks they are being set. That is to say, data did not reveal a
consistent element or process which children are recalling to tackle similar
tasks. This leads to the conclusion that not only is memory modified and
constructed with each task, but also that there are some forces
influencing this reshaping as can be seen in Figure 6.1.
Elements of prior knowledge
influencing constantly change the
shape of memory
Memory
Different aspects of
memory are drawn
upon to achieve the
outcome of the task
Figure 6.1 Forces influencing memory
6.2.3 Empirical Evidence
Below are some examples from my data which show a range of events
and how they all depend on what children are able to recall.
226
Extract from transcript of fifth lesson by Mrs Jane Marshall at
Argyle Common First School
[Children working with number fans]
Teacher: Show me … good right let’s just have a think how did we
work it out six and what makes ten … how can I work it out
Henry?
Child: Umm I am not sure … if you know five and five makes ten
then one less one is six
Teacher: Right right … I see … um Emily ?
Child: I worked it out
Teacher: How?
Child: Umm get six and go like this … [child uses her fingers] … you
can put six in the air and count on four
Extract from transcript of fifth lesson by Mrs Jane Marshall at
Argyle Common First School
Teacher: Right close up your fans Mitchell come on … what can you tell
me about five and five Emily?
Child: It’s a double
Teacher: What about this one … (long pause) … eighteen add what
makes twenty?
Child: Oh I know
Teacher: I don’t want to know the answer but who can tell me a way
of working it out … can you put it on the floor please … a way
of working it out … Devon
Child: What you can do is take the one off the end and umm take
the two and put it in the box … and then you’ve got the
answer
Teacher: Where did you get the two from sweetheart?
Child: The twenty?
Child: Yes
227
Extract from transcript of third lesson by Mrs Jill Thomas at St
Paul First School
Teacher and few children: Let’s start by counting in one’s … one two
three four five six seven eight nine ten eleven twelve thirteen
fourteen fifteen sixteen seventeen eighteen nineteen twenty
twenty-one twenty-two twenty-three twenty-four twenty-five
twenty-six twenty-seven twenty-eight twenty-nine thirty … I
think some of you are asleep this morning … there were
some children who were not joining in there were some
children not sitting properly so let’s sit up straight … right
just look at the person who is sitting next to you just and
just check they are awake you don’t need to say anything to
them … just look and check that they are awake and let’s
count up to thirty once more everyone joining in ready
Teacher and most children: One two three four five six seven
Teacher: Stop being silly
Teacher and most children: Eight nine ten eleven twelve thirteen
fourteen fifteen sixteen seventeen eighteen nineteen twenty
twenty-one twenty-two twenty-three twenty-four
Most children: Twenty-five twenty-six twenty-seven twenty-eight
twenty-nine thirty
Teacher: Well done right we are going to count in two’s from four to
sixteen … ready
Teacher and most children: Four six eight ten twelve fourteen sixteen
Extract from transcript of sixth lesson by Mrs Rebecca Rice at St
Paul First School
Teacher: Now get your fingers show me ten fingers and take away two
… (long pause child counts)
Child: (whispers) one two three four five six eight nine
Teacher: No count them again you’re nearly right … ten put your
fingers up for me like this … shh shh take two away … and
how many are standing up nice and tall? count your fingers
Child: One two three four five six seven eight
Teacher: Good boy well done … eight … double four makes?
Child: Eight
Teacher: Double three makes
Child: Six
Teacher: Double two makes
Child: Four
Teacher: One and one makes
Child: Eleven…..oh no… silly me it’s two
228
The small sample above is representative of the whole data set and shows
that children’s memory is a key feature of the prior knowledge that they
bring to each mathematical task. The transcripts also show that children
had very different recollections while engaged in mathematical tasks. Also
children changed what they were using to address each task, with such
changes, at times, occurring during the task.
The data showed no consistent pattern in what was being recalled or used
by children on similar tasks. This lack of consistency suggests that
children are drawing on different aspects of their own individual unique
memory to support each task.
The fourth transcript above reveals that children’s memory is changing
shape and is different to how it was at the start of the task. This discovery
in itself is not ground breaking, as constructivists would argue that we
build our understanding of the world and our knowledge by constructing,
changing and modifying the memory store we have. However it is ground
breaking that my data reveal eight elements that shape memory. The rest
of this chapter will focus on each of these elements in turn.
6.3 Context
6.3.1 Theoretical Perspective
As the data were analysed, a significant number of events emerged in
which children were relying on some models or images to understand the
task. Within these, there were events which relied upon experiences which
specifically took place outside of school and formed a framework to help
229
interpret the task. These all had some commonalities such as physical
spaces and objects that allowed children to comprehend the task.
In order to understand how these (physical spaces and objects) contexts
support children with their mathematical tasks, I must consider research
around the concept of context in mathematics and develop a definition
which helps to understand the data. As events were sorted, the
commonalities which were present were the use of physical spaces and
objects outside of the school. These spaces and objects, or contexts,
outside of school supported children to understand and unravel the
demands of their mathematical tasks. By using the context, children were
able to contextualise the problem which they were attempting. The
contexts which were expressed by children were part of their individual
reality and experiences, were present prior to the task being attempted,
and were drawn upon to understand the demands of the task, thus
forming part of their prior knowledge. The role of context as a conduit to
making meaning was a strong element of the data collected. It is
important that some reflection on context is carried out. There needs to
be a thorough examination of the role that context plays and why it forms
part of prior knowledge.
Examining the research during the process of analysis allowed me to
evaluate and focus on events which demonstrated a clear presence of
context. Research considers and defines the role of context within primary
mathematics in two ways – one view being the framing of mathematical
questions in a real-life context to aid understanding; the other view being
the environment in which learning takes place. Both these perspectives
230
need to be examined in order to evaluate the understanding they brought
to the events in my data.
It is only by considering our present understanding of existing research
that we can layer new understanding as revealed by the data. Evaluating
the data will allow for a definition to be determined and develop an
understanding of context as a facet of prior knowledge and its forms and
functions within prior knowledge. Therefore looking wider than my data
set initially allows me to consider what the concept of context means in
common understanding of the mathematics classroom within current
literature.
One understanding researchers have is to consider context within
mathematics as an enabler for the development of understanding.
We define “context” as the situation in which the
problem is embedded. The main role of the
context seems to be that of providing the
problem solver with the information that may
enable the solution of the problem.
(Borasi, 1986)
This perspective considers context as a way to pose mathematical
problems to children, where the development of the context is in the
control of the teacher. This is seen within the primary classroom as a way
to frame questions within a narrative.
The practice of embedding school mathematics
into some “real” context supports learning.
(Sullivan, Zevenbergen & Mousley, 2003, p. 109)
231
This embedding of a context within mathematics when asking questions is
often an attempt by the teacher to link with the children’s prior knowledge
to support rationalisation of the questions being posed. Teachers
sometimes lead the formulation of context and make assumptions about
children’s prior knowledge. If the context implied by the teacher is also
part of the child’s prior knowledge, then it supports easy understanding of
the task. On the other hand, if the child is not familiar with the context
being implied by the teacher, then the child will try and interpret the
context based on their own prior knowledge, thus hindering understanding
of the task. Therefore contexts used and reflected by the children are an
insight into the world of the child and where they are placed in their
understanding of mathematics.
In the transcript extract below, this is a mismatch between the teacher’s
expectation of what children’s prior knowledge is in relation to the word
pattern and their actual experience. For Chris, a pattern is a pictorial
representation of a repeated pattern on paper with some understanding of
numbers that appear while counting in 2’s. On the other hand, the teacher
expects the children to be able to relate the word pattern to numerical
sequences.
Extract from transcript of ninth lesson by Mrs Jill Thomas at St
Paul First School
Teacher: Well done when we are counting in two’s … who can tell me
something about the pattern for counting in two’s … what is
the pattern for counting in two’s … Chris?
Child: Pattern? … like a wavy line … I was doing that at home … you
know I had 4 colours and made four lines at the same time …
you get to 4 when you count in 2’s
Teacher: Not a wavy line … try again … what is the pattern when we
count in two’s?
232
Child: Ummm … it’s like this … [child puts dots in the air]
Teacher: No I would like you to think what is the number pattern when
we count in two’s
Child: Two four six eight ten …
Teacher: That is right … we do say two four six eight ten … what do we
call these numbers … Jo?
Child: They are all even … they’re all even numbers
Teacher: That’s it … the pattern when we count in two’s is that they
are all even numbers
Furthermore Cooper and Dunne (1998), through their research into how
social class affects children’s approach to mathematical tasks, also argue
that this contextualising of mathematics may create a layer of complexity
for students in that the experiences that children bring to school may not
be reflected in the contexts used by teachers to frame tasks. At this
juncture, it is vital to consider why this may be the case and question the
use and deployment of contextualisation which causes this difficulty. Also
what can be done to aid children when mathematical tasks are set in a
real-world context to support understanding. The paradox is that teachers
use a set of contexts in asking questions while they are not aware of the
individual’s prior knowledge and therefore cannot predict how the context
will be interpreted. Therefore the use of context to support children in the
mathematics they are engaging in often seems an arbitrary tool which
does little to develop understanding. Furthermore my data support this in
that they show children’s efforts to use the context in which the
mathematics is framed requires them to realign their understanding. They
first link the embedded context to their existing contextual experience.
They then process the questions by reframing them for themselves as
there is not always a perfect match between the external context used
and the internal contextual lexicon or the children’s individual prior
233
knowledge. This can be seen in the transcript extract earlier in this
section.
Children have their own internal contexts that have been developed
through their experiences which form a part of their prior knowledge.
They use these contexts to understand and make meaning of the
mathematics they are engaged in. Their individual contexts form a sort of
translator for what is being presented to them within the classroom, that
of context being an internal narrative which supports understanding of
external structures that are used to frame mathematics. Thus context
within my model as revealed by the data is this internal tool which allows
children to make sense of the external processes of mathematics.
I would further argue that this layer of complexity is there due to the
teachers’ lack of awareness of children’s own personal contexts and
experiences, thus requiring children to decode not only the mathematics,
but also the contextual information provided by the teacher. Therefore
children’s engagement with the mathematics is hindered through the use
of teacher-led contextualisation. Considering this area of research has
proved useful in two ways – one which clarifies the way in which the term
context is currently used; the other which offers a path to linking this
understanding to what the data are revealing, that of children using
teacher-led contextual framing in a variety of ways. This is due to a
mismatch as discussed earlier in understanding children’s experiential
base.
The other understanding of context has been developed by the work of
Lave (1988) and Walkerdine (1990) when they consider the effects of the
234
environmental context upon the ability of individuals to perform
mathematical tasks. By this they mean the physical spaces in which
mathematics is situated. This body of research suggests that there is a
connection between the procedures and skills used by individuals to
perform a mathematical task and the individual’s situated context while
carrying out these tasks. One of the ways in which individuals approach
mathematical tasks is influenced by where they are physically situated.
The studies further go on to show that there is a difference in the way in
which children approach mathematical tasks in different environmental
contexts and physical spaces. This implies that, in some form, the physical
space influences the way in which children interact with the mathematics.
The interesting question is why or what is it about the space that
influences the relationship between the mathematics they are given and
the way in which they approach it. The data from my study show that
children layer their interpretation of what is being asked upon the
narrative that they have formulated from past contexts.
In the transcript extract below, Nathan is using his experience of clearing
a table as a way to understand repeated subtraction to support the
calculation being asked. For Nathan, the concept of division is linked with
the idea of the table and counting in pairs while removing objects.
Extract from transcript of fifth lesson by Mrs Jane Marshall at
Argyle Common First School
Teacher: Brilliant so it’s eighteen shared by … divided by … shared
between two … ok so what’s the next thing I am going to do?
Nathan?
Child: Count down … umm (laughs)
Teacher: Count down ok?
235
Child: Eighteen, sixteen, fourteen … [child does the action of
removing things from the table]
Teacher: Why are you counting down?
Child: Clearing the table … you take two things away at a time …
Teacher: Can you explain?
Child: Take two away each time to know how many you can share
Each child has an evolving relationship between the different contexts in
which they have experienced mathematics and their interpretation of
these experiences which are shaped by different factors such as setting.
Lave’s “Adult Math Project” (1988) illustrated this interrelation between
individuals and settings, concluding that the physical context instigated
choice in the mathematical process used. Therefore it is reasonable to
conclude that in part it is the spaces that individuals have interacted with
that influence the way in which they attempt a given task. Also that
context is not just the physicality of the experience, but also the way in
which experiences have shaped understanding. Data revealed that
children approach tasks with their own sense of context, having engaged
with something they see as familiar, and therefore constructing their own
meaning of the mathematics they are given based on the nature of their
personal contextual lexicon. Also as it is present before the task and used
to rationalise the task presented, we see that different personal, social
and physical environments have a different effect upon the way in which
children carry out mathematical tasks.
In the transcript extract below, the child has seen the clock face in a
similar position and related it to her sleeping time (based on seeing the
moon on the clock) and thus can read it again with ease. However when
asked how she worked out that it was seven o’clock, she was not able to
236
explain in any detail where the hands of the clock should be to represent
on the hour. So her ability to solve this task was dependent upon and
fixed within her memory of the clock she has at home.
Extract from transcript of tenth lesson by Mrs Jennie Brooks at
Draycott First School
Teacher: What time does the clock say now?
Child: Ooh ooh I know that is easy … it’s the time I go to bed … 7
o’clock
Teacher: How do you know it says seven o’clock … can you tell the rest
of the class how you worked that out?
Child: I have a clock in my room and there is a moon on the
number 7 and it means bedtime … so I know it is seven
o’clock
My data not only support both perspectives described above, but also
allow us to understand the reasons why children demonstrate different
approaches to similar tasks. There is an interlinking of the contextual
experiences that children bring to a task and the way in which these direct
their thinking, making context a crucial element of how children are able
to carry out mathematical tasks. Children formulate their own unique
understanding of the task by using their own experiential contexts. It is
these contextual experiences which are an amalgamation of both the
physical contexts, thus in line with Lave’s findings, and the links between
number and the experiences children have which creates an individual set
of contexts for every situation that each child faces. Therefore within prior
knowledge the notion of context is neither the physical nor the
conceptual, but more the remoulding of the two to provide a unique lens
through which tasks are interpreted.
237
6.3.2 Definition
In my prior knowledge model, my data show the importance of the
meaning that children are bringing to their mathematical tasks using in
part their experiences of and in physical spaces (e.g. parks, roads, playing
in the garden, eating dinner in the kitchen, etc.) and how these have an
influence upon the conceptualisation and comprehension of mathematical
tasks. This element of prior knowledge is what I call context.
Children use their seemingly unconnected contextual links to help answer
questions. The way in which children use and manipulate the external
context is influenced by their internal contextual map. Within the data,
there is a complex weaving of how prior knowledge is made up and one of
the crossovers is that of contextual experience within school and outside
of school. Having teased out the relationship between children’s
experiences of context and how this may influence what they are able to
bring to bear upon mathematical tasks, data show that there is distinction
between the way in which context within school and context outside of
school influences children in their approach to mathematical tasks.
Emerging through the data are two aspects of children’s contextual
experiences – one of these being the formal experiences of school
(acculturation which will be considered in Section 6.4); the other being
informal experiences outside of school. Therefore context, in terms of
prior knowledge, is a distinctive feature which is not shaped by
experiences linked to formal educational settings. From this, context is a
key part of prior knowledge and is defined as the amalgam of all
contextualised experiences children have had outside of school which they
238
draw upon to support the understanding of mathematics by allowing them
to view tasks through the lens of previous contextual ideas which have
been rationalised.
Therefore my definition of context, as I define it here, does not include
school, playground or any other areas connected with formal educational
settings such as forest school areas or nurseries as these comprise the
prior knowledge element of acculturation (see Section 6.4). When
analysing the data, it emerged that children referred to and used context
in very different ways to understand the questions being asked, thus
leading to context forming a distinctive part of prior knowledge.
6.3.3 Further Empirical Evidence
When I consider the transcripts below, the data show that in order to
understand a task, children search for ways to make meaning. One
mechanism that they rely upon is to search for similar situations that they
have been in before physically.
In the first transcript, Rowena has used the idea of playing snakes and
ladders to achieve the mathematical task. It seems that for her, somehow
memory of how to take away five from eight is inextricably linked to her
past experience of playing a game. Part of the structure of her prior
knowledge is influenced by the sensory and emotional experiences that
she has had in the context of playing snakes and ladders.
The data show that it is not a simple connection between spaces and
mathematics, but a wide variety of ways in which the contextual
experiences children have shapes their memory and therefore forms an
239
element of prior knowledge that they bring to support their tasks. Looking
further into this extract, we notice that in order to be able to perform the
calculation, Rowena relies upon understanding gained from the playing of
snakes and ladders and has an image from the physicality of moving up
and down the board which has formed part of her prior knowledge.
Extract from transcript of first lesson by Mrs Sally Crane at Hatton
First School
Child: Eight take away five leaves us with
Teacher: Hang on … eight … I can only write that fast … so you think
it’s eight take away
Child: Five
Teacher: Eight take away five right
Child: Gives us
Teacher: Shh shh let Rowena finish the whole sum if she can eight
take away five equals
Child: Three
Teacher: Three that’s brilliant … why … did you how did you know
that? … how did you see that … why did you think that? …
you’re right
Child: Because you know I play this game snakes and ladders at
home and in that you go up and down
The images formed in Rowena’s mind of the snakes and ladders board has
been drawn upon while looking at a task in school. Thus for Rowena, the
context of snakes and ladders has supported her in addressing the
mathematical challenge. Therefore Rowena is drawing on the context
developed within her prior knowledge.
In the following transcript, the child’s experience of physically standing in
circles has allowed them to develop their understanding of the nature of
shapes. This understanding is further enhanced by the ideas of how a
point on a triangle may feel. Children’s understanding of shapes and their
240
properties depend on the many different contexts in which they have
already seen these shapes. This knowledge is not built up by simply
showing children pictures of the shapes, but the many different
experiences that children will have had up to this point which supports
their development of a set of personal definitions about shapes. In order
to fully form prior knowledge for this child, the physical context has been
merged to form a new context through which some understanding of
shape has been developed. This is one of the mechanisms used by some
children to develop personal definitions of each area of mathematics.
Extract from transcript of second lesson by Mrs Rebecca Rice at St
Paul First School
Teacher: Ah but there is one shape that has less corners than the
triangle … which one is that?
Child: You can’t stand in any corners in a round… it just goes round
and round like in the park … [child makes a circle in the air]
Teacher: Circle … because how many corners has that got?
Child: None
Teacher: None … so you are quite right the triangle has less corners
than the square and the rectangle but it’s got more corners
than the circle … another difference
Child: If the circle balloon falls on the pointy bit it will pop
Teacher: That’s quite right if the circle balloon fell on top of the
triangle it might pop … shh … shh the balloon would pop …
what other differences can you see? … what about this shape
here? I can see some differences between this shape and this
shape … Isaac
Child: The rectangle has got more … longer … it’s a bit squashed …
a bit longer that way a bit ummm … like the TV
Teacher: So the rectangle’s got longer sides top and bottom than what
… the square … yes if you look at the square top and bottom
they’ve got short sides top and bottom … whereas the
rectangles got long sides … what about the ends of the
rectangle … Megan?
The child needed to refer to the TV as a way to allow the explanation of a
rectangle to make sense. We can extrapolate from this that their prior
241
knowledge of shapes (rectangular) is of a TV and the child draws upon
this context to develop further understanding.
In the third transcript, Aiden has used the context of home to solve the
question asked. He is able to use the idea of having done this at home to
support his understanding of what is being asked.
Extract from transcript of first lesson by Mrs Jo Fishily at
Greenville Park Community School
[Children are playing a game of snakes and ladders as a
whole class the counter is on 6 and the dice is rolled again]
Teacher: Yes four (child makes a fist and punches the air with a smile)
… right put your hand up if you can work out already where
my blue bead’s going to be? … let me ask somebody with
their hand up … let me ask Aiden
Child: Worked it out already its ten …
Teacher: Ooh how did you work that out Aiden, how did you know?
Child: Cause um I always works out my number at home
Teacher: What did you think in your head though so that you knew
that answer?
Child: Um cause um I knew it was um cause I know the number
Though not able to explain in detail how the understanding is formed,
there is some rudimentary recognition of the fact that Aiden is drawing
upon experiences that have been informed by the context of home –
again the contextualisation is forming part of prior knowledge.
In the next transcript, Paul has used his experience of various road signs
to understand and answer his question. There is reliance on physical
contexts of how children are forming understanding, and this
understanding is then being referred to when engaged in mathematical
tasks.
242
Extract from transcript of second lesson by Mrs Rebecca Rice at St
Paul First School
Teacher: You are super stars aren’t you … right over the next three
days we’re going to be looking at shapes … now you’ve
already looked at fat shapes haven’t you? shapes that we call
3D … and this time we are going to be looking at flat shapes
… shapes that we call 2D … the sort of shapes that we draw
on pieces of paper like those ones ok … Paul … shh … shh …
James … Paul could you tell me what that shape is called?
Child: It’s the same as the sign outside on the road where the man
is digging
Teacher: Good what is the name of that shape?
Child: Umm … triangle
In the final transcript, where the children are asked to find a missing
number, Hannah relates it to her experience of playing hide-and-seek in
the garden to understand how to find missing numbers on a number line.
This prior exposure to other ideas has an impact upon the methods that
children can draw on to approach their tasks. These transcripts show that
children are drawing upon these past experiences in different contexts to
enhance their understanding of the present situation.
Extract from transcript of second lesson by Mrs Jennie Brooks at
Draycott First School
Teacher: Nooo … Holly now what’s the matter? … can you move she
can’t see? … Holly what’s the matter with my line now? …
shall we count it?
Child: One
Teacher: Nooo … zero
Child: Zero one two three four five six seven eight
Teacher: What
Child: Eight is hiding
Teacher: Is that eight? … Holly where’s eight? … can you see it?
Child: It’s gone
Teacher: Oh Hugh … has it gone? … Hannah where’s eight?
Child: I like playing hide and seek yesterday in the garden we
played that and then I found Jo … (child scans through to
look for number eight) there is eight. I am good at that.
243
I argue through the data that context and its influence in modifying
children’s memory form a key facet of the overall prior knowledge that
children use to develop their understanding of mathematical concepts.
The use of context to support understanding is limited by what the
children are familiar with and exposed to in terms of context. When
considering individual prior knowledge, we must include the context that
forms this prior knowledge and furthermore understand that context
within prior knowledge is made up of many different external experiences
which have been reshaped by the individual to formulate an evolving
model or image to support new understanding. Without context, prior
knowledge would not be complete as the physical experiences that
individuals have shape the tools they bring to understanding the
mathematical tasks presented.
6.4 Acculturation
6.4.1 Theoretical Perspective
Throughout the data analysis, another recurring theme was the noticeable
reference by children to having done the task or aspect of the task before
in a formal educational setting. Also there were some interpretations
given to tasks which could be linked directly to having engaged with it in a
specific manner before with formal instruction. These events were
distinctively different to context, encompassed within which were external
non-school contexts which children were using to rationalise the task. A
category to emerge is one where all events are related to individual
244
understanding of mathematics linked to formal education settings. It is
this that I have labelled as acculturation.
Much of the research on acculturation is based within the context of
multicultural integration and how individuals cope with the cultural, social
and psychological impact. Cabassa (2003) defines acculturation as “the
social and psychological exchanges that take place when there is
continuous contact and interaction between individuals from different
cultures” (p. 127). Berry (2005) states that “acculturation is the dual
process of cultural and psychological change that takes place as a result of
contact between two or more cultural groups and their individual
members” (p. 698). The definitions above can be used to explain events
that emerge where children bring their influences based on their prior
formal educational experiences and are now becoming accustomed to the
culture of the current setting.
Every child in every society has to learn from
adults the meanings given to life by his society;
but every society possesses with a greater or
lesser degree of difference, meanings to be
learned. In short, every society has a culture to
be learned though cultures are different.
(Levitas, 1974, p. 3)
We could argue that all schools and teachers have a way of thinking which
varies and is dependent upon the cultural values and demands of the
school and challenge the notion that all teaching of mathematics is carried
out in the same way. As Nickson (1994) states, the culture of the
mathematics classroom is “the invisible and apparently shared meanings
245
that teachers and pupils bring to the mathematics classroom and that
govern their interaction in it” (p. 8).
These definitions have some bearing upon my research in that they
emphasise that within primary schools, each classroom has its own
culture, with different classrooms in the same school having different
cultures by the virtue of being constructed by individuals who are
different.
First, we argue that teachers and students
together create a classroom mathematics
tradition or microculture and this profoundly
influences students’ mathematical activity and
learning.
(Cobb, Perlwitz & Underwood-Gregg, 1998, p. 63)
When considering classroom traditions and learning about mathematics in
the classroom and in wider society, fundamental questions are raised
about how children acculturate. I use the term acculturation and not
enculturation as they are distinctly different. My data show that there are
clear efforts made by both the teacher and the child to assimilate and
change to come to a common cultural position which is defined as
acculturation, that is the working of two cultures to adjust together. On
the other hand, enculturation implies that teachers support and shape the
way in which the children fit within the classroom culture which is not
influenced by the children (so it is a one-way process from teachers to
children). The data show that both children and teachers have made
efforts to understand the influences of prior knowledge, and in this case
the aspects of prior school experience, to support understanding of
246
mathematical tasks, hence to acculturate. However, in my analysis, I have
focused on the acculturation process of children.
In the transcript extract below, we see that Caitlin is using a previously
established routine for calculating a difference that she has used
previously in mathematics to support how she attempts the current task.
Extract from transcript of seventh lesson by Miss Lora Hunter at St
Paul First School
Teacher: What is ten and one more … when you’ve found the answer
get your number fan at the ready … when you show me ten
the one comes first and then the zero … Caitlin’s got nine
what did you do Caitlin?
Child: I used my fingers and … like with Mrs Jones [Mrs Jones is the
class teaching assistant]
Teacher: You counted on your fingers … can you show me how you did
it?
Child: I had this many [holds up 10] and then I closed one…
Teacher: You had ten … then why did you close one
Child: That’s how we worked yesterday in numeracy … made the
first number then closed the other number … like this
The events show that in order to understand tasks, children refer to how
they learnt within school and how this supports them to develop methods
to approach similar tasks.
Data also show the ideas that children use to carry out mathematical
tasks and how are these shaped by their past experiences in other formal
educational settings. Furthermore children are required to adapt their
thinking or, in Piagetian terms, assimilate their cognitive process to fit
within the existing structure. Children bring their own social psychological
culture and there is a process of negotiation between teacher and child to
assimilate into classroom norms.
247
In the transcript extract below, we see that the child’s understanding of
the vocabulary to describe shapes has been shaped by their experience in
previous lessons and the rules they gathered in terms of the use of
prefixes. The teacher accepted the logic applied by the child to support
the development of understanding in sorting out shapes into different
properties.
Extract from transcript of fifth lesson by Mrs Jane Marshall at
Argyle Common First School
Teacher: Everyone I looked at managed to sort out their shapes
perfectly … so well done … I was just sad that one table
couldn’t share glue … so blue table are going to lose a star …
(long pause) … right let’s see red not thick red … instead of
using the words not thick what other word could I have
used?
Child: Unfat …
Teacher: Could do … is that a word?...
Child: Last time … un is the same as not… like in literacy when we
did the opposite quiz
Teacher: Is that always the case? … unfat is not a word but ok we can
use unfat here … so now look at this shape … where will I put
it? … [teacher holds up a green thin shape]
Child: Over there [pointing to a Venn diagram on the floor made
out of hoops with the label Not red]
As Cobb et al. (1998) noted, classroom norms are full of microcultures
and routines which shape children’s way of approaching the tasks
presented. When children are in classrooms, they have to reacclimatise to
the rules and order of that classroom and the data show that within prior
knowledge, children’s understanding of mathematics is formed in part by
the cultural influences of previous formal educational settings. Therefore
children need to begin to understand the social and psychological
(Cabassa, 2003) changes that must be made in a new classroom. This
process of acculturation has an impact upon shaping prior knowledge. In
248
the transcript extract below (part of this transcript extract also appears on
page 186), we see that children are not used to number stories as they
are distracted by wider events of school routine and associate these
mathematical questions to those routines to support the calculation
required.
Extract from transcript of seventh lesson by Mrs Jill Thomas at St
Paul First School
Teacher: Umm now we’re going to see how good you are at listening
and how you can try and work out the answers to these
number stories … you could use adding … or taking away so
let’s try … ready ok? … Ok let’s think I had ten cakes and I
ate three of them … how many cakes did I have left … Lucy?
Child: From the cake sale yesterday … but you were helping at the
table miss.
Teacher: Yes at the cake sale … I had ten cakes and I ate three of
them … how many cakes did I have left …
Child: Seven
Teacher: Seven good girl … I have five pencils … if I put five more in
my tin … five pencils in my tin I put five more in my tin how
many altogether Molly
Child: That’s my job to sort the pencils..
Teacher: Ok can we just work out the answer to the number stories …
I have five pencils … if I put five more in my tin
Child: It is ten that is how many you have in there now …
Teacher: How did you work out that the answer was ten?
Child: I can see them from here … hehe … we had to tidy up
yesterday … remember?
In simplistic terms how children approach a task, e.g. adding, is
influenced to some extent by how this has been explained or taught and
understood in their previous classroom experiences. Children may have to
do 23 + 9 by putting 23 in their head first and then counting on 9 more in
their previous classroom experience, and in their new classroom they may
be taught or expected to carry out the same process as 23 + 10 – 1 which
requires a different structural understanding of the relationship between +
249
and –. This change in process and the underlying understanding can be
equated to a change in the culture and values of the classroom which they
will need to adjust to. Also it could be that the practical tools used by the
previous teacher had a constructivist pedagogical philosophy. Children
bring that to their new learning. Therefore, as Bishop (2002) states, a
child is experiencing a “cultural conflict” and it is this conflict that requires
understanding and supporting. Wolcott (1974) further clarifies the process
of acculturation as “the modification of one culture through continuous
contact with another” (p. 136). Throughout the data, we see that one of
the areas that are present prior to the task is a process that has been
developed through being in a formal setting that has to be modified in
order to assimilate into their new classroom culture where these methods
may be very different. Overall this cultural tension between child, teacher,
classrooms, past formal educational experiences and present formal
educational experiences is one which forms a part of prior knowledge.
To consider in a little more depth, the cultural conflicts which are
inevitably present between not only different settings, but also by the
virtue of children having different teachers each year and being on the
whole in different spaces complicates the transition process. The transition
process is a slow but essential process which understands that in order to
shift an individual’s thinking, they must understand the reasoning behind
the change and this reasoning depends upon children’s prior experiences.
Within schools, there is some effort to overcome these “cultural”
differences in mathematics by implementing strategies such as a
calculation policy or a whole school progression plan within mathematics.
The sound principles of these strategies are based upon recognising that
250
the way in which children develop understanding is dependent upon the
way in which their teaching and learning is structured. If we are accepting
the premise that this has key influence upon the individual’s prior
knowledge, then the change in the nature of this is a high cultural shift for
individual children. It is crucial to be explicit in that the data show not
only change in children to a new culture in their current class, but also all
the teachers developing an understanding of the way in which children are
thinking and processing and adjusting to this process. As has been
evidenced by the data, the notion that children can attempt a task without
any influence of their prior experience in a formal setting seems clearly
improbable. Though the data did not show this to be clearly the case in
every event, we cannot rule out the influence in the construction of their
prior knowledge as children’s understanding and processing of a task in
some form has been influenced by being part of a formal educational
setting, which at times is so woven into their understanding of
mathematics that it is difficult to always tease out.
6.4.2 Definition
In my prior knowledge model, I am defining acculturation as the events
related to the experiences of children within formal educational settings,
e.g. the classroom. The previous formal educational experiences that
children have had have an impact upon their understanding and
knowledge of mathematics. The cultures of schools, nurseries and pre-
school environments have a unique effect on children’s ability to
understand and attempt mathematical tasks.
251
6.4.3 Further Empirical Evidence
In the transcript extract below, children talk about having done the task
before in school. Children are bringing a wider educational culture and
what they learn from it to the task. This has an effect on how and what
they have to help support them in doing a mathematical task. In this
example, acculturation refers to the talk that teachers engage in, the rules
and routines of how mathematics is approached, and the ethos of the
teacher. The teacher is choosing to focus children into a particular method
for calculating one more and one less than. However children remember
one less than as take away, so there is a compromise made and both
approaches are used.
Extract from transcript of first lesson by Mrs Jo Fishily at
Greenville Park Community School
Teacher: And he’d be very pleased to hear that, won’t he? … now …
yesterday in number … put your hand up if you can
remember what we were doing yesterday in number? …
Hannah
Child: Ummm, counting
Teacher: Can you remember? Is it coming? … Shall I ask somebody
else? Martha
Child: Taking away and
Teacher: Taking away and? … nearly nearly there, what were you
going to say Richard?
Child: Adding one more
Teacher: Adding one more orrrr?
Child: Taking away we did this before
Teacher: Taking away one more … we were working out one more or
one less … and do you remember yesterday in numeracy, we
started off … just sit for a little bit Logan … by using our
number lines from nought to twenty, didn’t we? and we said
… oh Gemma can you point to the number one less than
nine? … one less, one less than nine … so take away one …
what should she point to?
Child: Eight
252
Some of the ideas, as can be seen in the transcript extract below, have
nothing to do with formal mathematics, but to do with the environment
and culture of the classroom created by the games that children have
recalled from previous classroom experiences.
Extract from transcript of third lesson by Mrs Jennie Brooks at
Draycott First School
Teacher: Well done are we ready? if I have got 10p and somebody
gives me another 6p how much will I have
Child: 16p … I remembered the p from before we played shops
there with Mrs Jones
Teacher: You didn’t forget the p well done … Oh this one is much too
hard … let me see if you can do this today … ten add zero
Each classroom observed had a definite set of routines and processes for
the way in which mathematics was approached. The transcript extracts
below show how, in each of the lessons, the teacher negotiated the way in
which children would approach new challenges in the tasks. For example,
a clear routine can be seen which consists of children regularly counting to
start each lesson.
Extract from transcript of third lesson by Mrs Sally Crane at Hatton
First School
Teacher: Good well done … we had the twenty instead of the twelve …
good you are getting the hang of that really well … Ruth try
and I know you don’t feel very well but see if you can join in
with us ok remember to move your hands helps you
remember how many you are counting … have a little look at
our hundred square ok … Nicholas would you like to stand up
and point to number ten for me … ok Nicholas is going to be
in charge then … can you count down the numbers with
Nicholas in ten’s as he points to them for me … off you go …
ten
Most children: Twenty thirty forty fifty sixty seventy eighty ninety a
hundred
Teacher: Wonderful … thank you sit down then … Richard would you
like to stand up and do you think you could point to them as
we count backwards
253
Child: Ohhh
Teacher: Ah yes
Child: Easy
Teacher: Oh easy jolly good right … let’s see if everybody can do it
with Nic Richard then ready … a hundred
Most children: Ninety eighty seventy sixty fifty forty thirty twelve
Teacher: Ah I caught somebody saying twelve
Child: Ten
Extract from transcript of fourth lesson by Mrs Sally Crane at
Hatton First School
Teacher: And thirty will stop there … well done ok let’s see if we can
remember our counting in tens we tried last half term …
ready with your hands
Most children and teacher: Zero ten twenty thirty forty fifty sixty
seventy eighty ninety a hundred
Teacher: Well done … let’s see if we can go backwards … ready
Teacher: Right let’s start going backwards from fifty
Most children: Fifty forty thirty twenty ten zero
Teacher: Now who can remember the robot from last time
Most children: Yeah yeah
Teacher: Now we are going to be doing some robot maths today
Extract from transcript of seventh lesson by Mrs Sally Crane at
Hatton First School
Teacher: Oh dear I think we’ve had all sorts of numbers
Child: He did eight
Teacher: What number is it?
Child: Ten
Child: Eight
Teacher: It’s number eight … let’s do it together ready
[Children start clapping]
Teacher: No we are all going to start together Richard … ready and
[Teacher and children clapping]
Few children: One two three
The interaction between teacher and children, and between children
themselves forms part of the culture of the classroom, and influences the
254
way in which children are able to approach mathematical tasks and what
they are drawing upon to support their understanding of the tasks. The
data further show that children bring to the task ideas they have gathered
in different settings. In the transcript extract below, the child recalls a
mathematical technique that they considered in their previous school
year.
Extract from transcript of eighth lesson by Mrs Sally Crane at
Hatton First School
Teacher: Eight all right … can anyone tell me let’s write it down what
the … difference
Child: What the difference
Teacher: Between is … what the difference between those two
numbers (8 and 6) … Josh
Child: Six you write 6 like this we practiced this with Mrs Wilson in
handwriting.
Teacher: You think it’s six … would you like to come and try and see if
you were right … can you remember how we did it
Child: Oh I know
Teacher: Let him see
Child: It’s the same two numbers as yesterday
Child: Jumps like we used to do with Mr Ellis
[After discussion with Mrs Crane, it was noted that Mr Ellis
was their reception teacher last year]
Teacher: Jump we did jump yes see if you’ve got the right jump …
count as you do it … out loud
Teacher and child: One two three four five
Teacher: Ah how many jumps?
Conversations which occur in a classroom about mathematics shape what
is available to children in supporting them with a task. Different classroom
cultures not only cultivate different ideas, but also shape the memory for
children to continue their mathematical development. In the transcript
extract below (part of this transcript extract appears earlier on page 203),
255
the teacher is keen and has reinforced many times for children to draw
upon facts and methods (counting on their fingers) they may already
know to support with the new task. This changes the way in which prior
knowledge is shaped and brought to bear upon this task. There is greater
emphasis on what has been learnt in this classroom and limited
acknowledgement of other ideas that children may have.
Extract from transcript of fifth lesson by Mrs Jane Marshall at
Argyle Common First School
Teacher: Let’s try this one? … fourteen add what makes twenty? so
you put fourteen in your head shhh … put it down you can’t
have number fans in your hands cause you need them for
counting … fourteen in your heads and count on till you get
to twenty … (long pause)
Child: Mrs Marshall it is easy it’s
Teacher: I’ll come to you in a moment I know what you’re going to say
… Liam?
Child: Umm six
Teacher: You are well on the ball now you’ve got it haven’t you Emily
… it is six Devon what are you going say?
Child: It’s changing the fourteen over to a four and then it’s easy
Teacher: You mean like that … put that there … well done remember
when we did some work with families it’s the thing I’ve got it
up here bargain basement if you know one thing you get a
lot of other things free … if you know that sixteen and four is
twenty all you’ve got to do is swap it around … ok if you
know that six and four is ten you should be able to work that
six and fourteen is twenty … and six and twenty-four is thirty
… one more
Child: We had that in our speedy maths
Teacher: We have done it in our speedy maths … Emily what were you
going to say?
Child: Thirteen add seven is twenty you get the seven and take
away one and add one you get four
Teacher: Right last one … (long pause) … twelve add I can see James
and Jay can’t listen this morning twelve add what makes
twenty … (long pause) … think about what number’s going to
go in your head … I am going to ask someone I haven’t
heard from today … twelve in your head and count on to
twenty … (long pause) … Ashley?
256
There is a great deal of variation in the acculturation experiences children
have had and this leads to variation in the shapes of individual memories,
and thus prior knowledge. Children bring what they understand and
remember based on their formal educational experiences of how they
were taught to approach mathematical tasks. This shapes memory and
also is a key element of prior knowledge. The data do not indicate which
classroom culture is better for mathematical development, merely that
part of what is used to address mathematical tasks by children is this
notion of acculturation and specifically, data point to acculturation of
formal educational settings.
To summarise, the data show that memory is shaped by the nature and
culture of the formal educational experiences that children have had,
shaping what children may be using in terms of addressing the
mathematics they are engaged in. There is great influence on other
elements of prior knowledge as a result of acculturation.
6.5 Metacognition
6.5.1 Theoretical Perspective
My data show that children bring some sense of their own prior
understanding – a level of self-awareness and an understanding of what
knowledge is already there and the connections that they have already
made (metacognition) – to mathematical tasks. It is not important
whether this self-awareness is erroneous or limited. However it is vital to
understand that as part of the tools that are employed to approach
mathematical tasks, children have an internal vocabulary that they refer
257
to in order to think through approaches they have that can be used or
knowledge that is familiar and understood. The data also show that there
is some element of individual thinking occurring before, during and after
performing a task. Children have some understanding of their thoughts.
They may not be able to verbalise these thoughts, but they do have a
sense of their thinking.
In the transcript extract below, we notice that Mary is aware that she does
not know how to count in sequence of two’s and offers a strategy that will
support her in carrying out the task. Also we can see that Scott has
established that the task is well below his competence and requests
further challenge by insisting on moving to higher numbers, thus
demonstrating a clear awareness of his own thinking.
Extract from transcript of eighth lesson by Mrs Jennie Brooks at
Draycott First School
Teacher: Three … right well done right stand still and let’s keep going
… the next one is … remember you’ve got to miss one and
say the next one
Child: Four … umm no it’s … umm five …
Teacher: Ok good let’s keep going next one Mary?…
Child: Are we allowed to count out to work it out … five … six [child
talks quietly] … seven
Teacher: Next?
Child: This is easy I can do them all … three five seven … … can we
go … high … I can count really high
Teacher: Well you will have to wait … now I know you can count …
you’ve got to listen … Harry Harry listening … right you’ve got
to miss one and say one … now there are … Scott … you’ve
got to listen …
Child: Mrs Brooks this is boring can we do bigger numbers …
Teacher: You’ve got to listen … you think you’ve got to miss one … and
say one and listen … this is going to be harder … Zeno won’t
know what to do cause he’s not listening … Hannah’s going to
start and she’s got to miss one … so she can’t say number?
258
There are aspects of the data that demonstrate knowledge of having
carried out similar tasks before, the level of understanding that was
established the last time it was carried out, and the impact this may have
on the task being presented, as can be seen in the transcript extract
below.
Extract from transcript of ninth lesson by Mrs Jo Fishily at
Greenville Park Community School
Teacher: Can anyone think of another word that means add we have
had plus add
Child: Adding
Teacher: That is add another word …
[Then the teacher goes on to explore some more properties
of addition … adding more than two numbers, the number
gets bigger … then she moves on to what is meant by taking
away]
Teacher: What do we mean when we take away?
Child: I know…. Oh oh… make it smaller…
Teacher: Can you see a word on the board that also means take away
Child: I know the word… but I can’t read it … what does subtract
look like
Teacher: It’s on the board it begins with a s s
Child: No what does it look like … I remember the word but can’t
work out the one it is …
Metacognition is defined by Schoenfeld (1992), on a simplistic level, as
knowledge about one’s thought process and self-regulation. Flavell (1979)
defined metacognition as “thinking about thinking” (p. 906).
‘Metacognition’ refers to all processes about
cognition, such as sensing something about
one’s own thinking, thinking about one’s
thinking and responding to one’s own thinking
by monitoring and regulating it.
(Papaleontiou-Louca, 2003, p. 12)
259
Metacognition needs to be deconstructed in order for me to understand
and apply it correctly in ideas that emerge through the data. There are a
limited number of studies considering the meaning of metacognition.
However educational psychologists have understood the value of
metacognition to support pupils’ development.
Kuhn and Dean (2004) define metacognition as “awareness and
management of one’s own thought” (p. 270). This aspect is visible
through the data. While children consider the tasks they are presented
with, they evaluate the abilities they had to tackle the task as can be seen
in the transcript extract below.
Extract from transcript of seventh lesson by Mrs Jo Fishily at
Greenville Park Community School
Teacher: She’s taken away a cuboid … right Hannah have a seat …
umm Josh can you come and take away a cube a 3D shape
you’re so smart this morning … a 3D shape can you take
away a 3D shape please … good boy you’ve taken away?
Child: Peasy a cuboid … let’s cover our eyes so we don’t look … so it
is harder
Teacher: Umm looks like you are finding all these shapes easy ok … let
try this one …
Child: With our eyes closed … pleeeeeease …
Teacher: Ummm … ok fine with your eyes closed there you go pick one
Child: Ooh it feels like a … tricky … umm is it a pyramid?
Teacher: Why do you think it is a pyramid?
Child: I can feel the sides … there is one which feels like a square
and I poked myself on the pointy bit …
Teacher: Ok open your eyes and check
Child: Yes (fist in air) I knew it … easy
Using these definitions, I can see two aspects of metacognition – one
which is likened to knowing how to do something, and other which is the
ability to choose the best strategy to achieve a task (Carr, Alexander &
260
Folds-Bennett, 1994). Researchers have examined and considered the
strategies that children use in mathematics and this “has indicated that
children possess and use metacognition to their advantage” (Carr et al.,
1994, p. 584).
Further research states that children possessing metacognition know
about mathematical strategies (Garofalo & Lester, 1985). It is the
reflective nature of this aspect of prior knowledge that has emerged
through the data. There is much connection between cognition and
metacognition and between metacognition and the impact it has on
individual motivation. The ability to reflect, select and act upon one’s own
engagement in a mathematical task is an intrinsic part of the prior
knowledge that children bring to the task, as evident in my data. Carr et
al. (1994) have suggested that the influence of metacognition upon
mathematical tasks is “instrumental when the task demands challenge the
child but do not overtax cognitive capacity and existing skills” (p. 584).
Schraw (1998) further states that “metacognition differs from cognition, is
multi-dimensional, and domain-general in nature” (p. 118).
My data show that one of the factors which children are bringing to
resolving and understanding mathematical tasks is this multi-dimensional
thinking and connecting of ideas and experiences. The ability to evaluate
and internalise how they will approach a task is clearly shaped by an
individual’s thinking and understanding of themselves – the ability to be
self-aware.
The idea of metacognition being domain-general implies in terms of prior
knowledge that the metacognition used to address mathematical tasks is
261
not specifically mathematical metacognition and is developed through the
whole of a child’s experience. The data show us that a child’s thinking
about their own knowledge and thinking cannot be partitioned into their
understanding of a particular aspect of mathematics. Children are thinking
through all areas of their knowledge in order to support and decipher the
mathematical tasks they are presented. The mere fact that the data have
demonstrated the children are thinking through what they already know,
understand and have experienced and are using it as a tool to develop
new understanding means that, by definition based on research by others,
metacognition is a part of prior knowledge and is there before the task is
attempted.
To summarise, metacognition is an element of prior knowledge as it is
developed from individual experiences and is present before the task. Also
throughout engaging in the task, children are using their strategies to
reflect upon their approaches to the task.
6.5.2 Definition
In my prior knowledge model, metacognition refers to children’s ability to
reflect and think about mathematical tasks and the methods they are
using. The data showed how children, while engaged in mathematical
tasks, were thinking of the following:
thoughts about the mathematical concepts needed and what they
mean;
how they would approach the task;
how well they were doing the task;
262
the ease or difficulty of the task;
the outcome of the task.
All of these processes, as evident in the data, are aligned with the
established theoretical base presented earlier. When looking at the
transcripts, it emerged that there were many events within which children
were being introspective and this introspection would affect the approach
taken by the children to complete the task. Therefore within my prior
knowledge model, metacognition is the introspection and self-evaluation
that children engaged in while attempting a mathematical task. This is
further supported by the data when I look at the transcripts.
Through the analysis of the data, there was a recurrence of events which
indicated reflection and construction of understanding based on self-
questioning by children. Though children did not always verbalise this
thinking and filtering through their ideas and thoughts before attempting
mathematical tasks, anecdotally there were many occurrences when
children would pause to evaluate how they should proceed forward.
Though there is no concrete evidence to support this as being
metacognition, it raises the question about what process was being
employed to result in the choices they made. We could speculate that in
order to descend upon a path forward, children must be thinking about
what they know and how they would be able to carry out the task,
therefore thinking about their own knowledge and understanding. There is
further support for this in concrete data gathered as we will see in the
transcripts to follow. The data support the theoretical perspective which is
integrated into this definition of metacognition.
263
6.5.3 Further Empirical Evidence
Having considered the theoretical base which was supported by what was
emerging in my data, the definition developed above is a shorthand
overview to understanding the element of prior knowledge which is
metacognition. In order to develop and clarify this definition, it is of value
to look at the evidence upon which it is based. When listening to children,
while they were using many aspects of prior knowledge as evident in this
whole dataset, metacognition was one aspect which was not apparent at
first viewing and one which needed some teasing out.
There were many events where children expressed their inability to
attempt a task, as we have seen in the transcript extract in Section 5.5.4
on page 181. This raised the following question – how do children make
the decision that they cannot do a task? What factors are they taking into
consideration? I can argue this in many ways – they have never seen such
a task before; when they tried it previously, they were unsuccessful; they
are not familiar with all aspects of the task e.g. they may not understand
how to start it or know all the steps to develop the outcome needed. We
could hypothesise and conjecture the many different reasons why a child
states their inability to perform a task or indeed the choices they make to
perform the task. However that would not resolve the simple fact that the
child has made a choice. The child has, through their ability to think about
their skills, knowledge and thinking, come to conclude the choices they
had made, or put simply they have metacognated.
264
It is this that is of importance. Without this ability to think about their
thinking (metacognition), as emerged from the data, children would not
be able to make choices in relation to approaches in the way they perform
mathematical tasks. Metacognition forms a crucial facet of prior
knowledge. The extent of a child’s ability to metacognate is very much
dependent on other areas of their prior knowledge and interlinked to other
facets. However we could extrapolate that the extent to which individuals
can assess their thinking has an impact upon their approach to the task.
In the transcript extract below, the child has considered what would be
easier for him and what they had already engaged in i.e. 9 + 2 and how
this links to the question being asked i.e. 9 + 3. Furthermore when asked
about the process, he explains what he felt he could do or not do. The
child has understood the needs of the questions through some process of
evaluation and thinking about their own knowledge base which has
enabled them to address the questions. The question is considered and
processed with some thoughts about what one’s own capacity is to answer
and address the questions, and an evaluation through metacognition of
what may be the correct direction is made for the individual. The child is
struggling to vocalise their thinking process clearly. However there is
some choice made to start from their previous answer of 9 + 2 = 11 and
then build on this to adding 1 more. Though not expressed, some internal
thinking has led to the choice of adding 1 more to 11 and not counting
from 9 in 1’s to get to the answer. It is this internal process which is
manifested in the way in which the child is expressing the puzzle that they
face.
265
Extract from transcript of third lesson by Mrs Jill Thomas at St
Paul First School
Teacher: Eleven … right I am sorry that was my fault I forgot come on
I’ll change that then … right you should be able to get this
really quickly then … nine add three … nine add three … nine
add three ready … steady … show … nine add three is … ?
Few children: Twelve
Teacher: Twelve right Greg how did you work that out?
Child: Umm I know that I am good at this
Teacher: Oh shhh will you be quiet I cannot hear what Greg is saying
so neither will anyone else be able to … sorry Greg
Child: I started from nine and counted on three
Teacher: Counted on three did anyone else work it out in a different
way? Chris?
Child: I counted in three’s
Teacher: You counted in three’s why did you count in three’s?
Child: To make it a little bit easier
Teacher: Right well did anybody when I said the one before nine add
two
Child: I added one
Teacher: Is that what you did?
Child: I put twelve because I thought I can’t put eleven so I might
as well put twelve then when you said nine and three I did
twelve again
Teacher: So you thought nine add two was twelve? … no
Child: Well because I couldn’t do eleven I thought I might as well
do twelve but then
Teacher: What do you mean you might as well?
Child: Umm
Teacher: Why did you say why did you think twelve why did you know
it was twelve?
Child: I didn’t think it was twelve but there wasn’t any eleven so
Teacher: No but then I changed it didn’t I? I didn’t say nine plus two …
I said nine plus three
Child: I counted on one from eleven because it was 2 and now it is
number 3
In the next transcript, Liam has considered the task carefully and is able
to answer the numerical question, and also further clarified independently
266
about the knowledge he does not have in the question relating to pounds
and pence. This indicates that there are some thoughts about what
children know in terms of what is being presented. Here Liam is able to
control his own choices through his metacognitive process.
Extract from transcript of seventh lesson by Miss Lora Hunter at St
Paul First School
[The class are asked to tell each other what they were
looking at in the lesson yesterday … they were looking at
money]
Teacher: What were we doing … what were we thinking about when
doing money … (long pause)
Child: Will you give us some money? … wow
[The class did not know … after a long pause one child was
able to read the target on the board … finding many ways to
make different amounts of money … the teacher was able to
then ask individual children what they did in relation to the
target … the answers were slow in coming … the class was
told that they were going to do something similar … she
shows them how she is going to do this … on the board she
has created a shop front and the children are asked to go
shopping with her … she asks questions about how much
each item in the shop costs]
Teacher: How much does the guitar cost me
Child: Umm 5p
Teacher: I asked Liam
Child: 5p
[The teacher goes on to ask about each of the items in the
shop … first about the cost of each item on their own then
she goes on to combining items and calculating the cost]
Child: When you know it’s money how do you know what is pounds
and what is pence?
Liam was able to think and link to other parts of his understanding to take
control of how the task is resolved, or in the case of the next transcript,
what does not make sense to the individual and ask further questions as
he does here in terms of really understanding the clock face. The
engagement with the teacher in the transcript has an impact upon the
267
shape that memory will take, and therefore is essential to prior
knowledge.
Extract from transcript of seventh lesson by Miss Lora Hunter at St
Paul First School
[The lesson starts with clocks and the teacher asking
different times to be shown by the children on their clocks]
Teacher: How can I check that that’s right … is there any way of
knowing that that’s right? does anybody know what time it
actually is now … what is the real time … ?
Child: Half past nine
Teacher: If you look at the clock and check and see you’ll see it is
actually half past nine … so we can check that we’ve got the
small hands pointing to nine and
Child: It’s not half past see
Teacher: It is half past nine right now on the clock …
Child: Well it isn’t on the nine or the six
Teacher: Well it’s just past the nine because it’s gone past the hour it
would be … (long pause) … when it comes to ten o’clock we’ll
look at the clock we’ll see what ten o’clock looks like
Child: It is near ten I am not sure why?
[What we see here is a child who is trying to establish what
the task demands in relation to his understanding and has a
clear idea that his knowledge is limited. Furthermore he is
able to express which parts of this question he needs to
understand. There is self-awareness and thinking about the
way in which personal knowledge is constructed and what
needs to be the next step in understanding these
mathematical ideas. A clear sense of self-awareness.]
Teacher: It's just gone past the nine cause in the past the hour it
would be when we come to ten o’clock we’ll have a look at it
and we will have a look at half past ten as well
Child: What do you mean by past the hour?
There is some element of metacognition in all tasks approached in the
next transcript. Ellie is actively thinking about what she has been asked,
going through the process of trying alternates and rejecting them through
some form of evaluation before picking an answer.
268
Extract from transcript of eighth lesson by Mrs Sally Crane at
Hatton First School
Teacher: Now do you remember when we looked at our number line
yesterday … we only had it going up to ten didn’t we … and
can anyone remember what we were doing with the number
line yesterday … what were we actually doing? we were doing
some number work and it was slightly different we hadn’t
done it before
Child: We were like rolling a dice
Teacher: No we’re thinking about the number line Richard that’s the
other part of the lesson good boy but which what did we do
with the number line Rowena?
Child: Umm we put dots by how many umm … it was away from it
Teacher: Yes that’s a very good try … Ellie can you remember the
words that we used?
Child: Is it … no … wait let me think I know … it is a bit like … I can
work it out … umm its difference between
[This interchange between not knowing and having some
notion of knowing the answer is more than just the skill of
recollection. It is a sense in Ellie and her thinking that the
answer is something she is aware of, but cannot recall. There
is thinking about what knowledge she has and how this links
to what is needed.]
Teacher: The difference between that’s right we put two spots didn’t
we and we chose those numbers and then we worked out
Charlotte I’ll have that please … what the difference between
the two numbers was … right let’s stick to the line that’s
between one to ten to start with and see if you can
remember how to do it from yesterday … right I am going to
put my stops by that number which is which number?
Few children: Three
Metacognition is the active process of introspection and self-evaluation of
what is being asked and what is required to complete the task. Children
are questioning within themselves and using this to support the
completion of mathematical tasks. This notion of metacognition is a facet
of prior knowledge as it considers the cognition and will also be linked to
the other elements in order to shape these ideas and meaning. Within the
data, children bring this need to evaluate and consider how best to
approach a mathematical task.
269
6.6 Other Emerging Categories
My data revealed the emergence of further categories – individual
motivation, perception, cognition, social group and abstraction – which
also form prior knowledge of children. In the subsections to follow, I will
explore these categories through a similar structure as done for the three
categories discussed so far – theoretical perspective, definition and
empirical evidence.
The data do not make it possible to conclude that the three categories
considered earlier and the further emerging five categories being
considered in this section are a finite list of components that constitute
prior knowledge.
6.6.1 Individual Motivation
6.6.1.1 Theoretical Perspective
Despite the existence of an immense body of research in the field of
motivation, there is no agreed common definition. Research in the
psychology of motivation and what affects individual motivation states
that children are motivated by tasks that they feel are important to them,
measure their value as individuals, enable them to express their views, or
provide them with a sense of ownership (Lovell, 1973).
The author has suggested a new definition for
motivation: a potential to direct behaviour
through the mechanisms that control emotion.
(Hannula, 2006, p. 175)
270
This definition of motivation helps me to realise the value of motivation in
children’s choice in their level of engagement in mathematical tasks.
Motivation is broadly distinguished into intrinsic and extrinsic motivation.
The most basic distinction is between intrinsic
motivation, which refers to doing something
because it is inherently interesting or enjoyable,
and extrinsic motivation, which refers to doing
something because it leads to a separable
outcome.
(Ryan & Deci, 2000, p. 55)
Children bring a level of motivation to some tasks and are, to some
extent, intrinsically motivated by the task they see. What is it that drives
this intrinsic motivation? Children enjoy the task for its own sake as they
feel they can be successful or are externally motivated by the experience
of rewards (Middleton & Spanias, 1999). Whatever the cause of individual
motivation, researchers widely agree that there are many knock-on
effects of children being motivated.
When individuals engage in tasks in which they
are motivated intrinsically, they tend to exhibit a
number of pedagogically desirable behaviours
including increased time on task, persistence in
the face of failure, more elaborative processing
and monitoring of comprehension, selection of
more difficult tasks.
(Middleton & Spanias, 1999, p. 66)
These effects upon a task are crucial to my research as individual
motivation impacts on the choices that children make and how they
engage in mathematical tasks, and hence shape prior knowledge.
271
6.6.1.2 Definition
In my prior knowledge model, the key features which define individual
motivation are the approach and attitude with which children tackle
mathematical tasks. These are both positive and negative attitudes and
feelings towards the task. When children first look at some mathematical
tasks, they have a response which controls the degree to which they are
willing to engage in the task presented to learn mathematics. Individual
motivation also comprises children’s desire to get the correct answer and
the consequent enjoyment which is produced. Furthermore individual
motivation includes events that allude to children’s self-confidence, both
at the beginning and during mathematical tasks.
6.6.1.3 Empirical Evidence
When I consider the data, I can see that the individual motivation that
children have towards any particular task is influenced by the prior
knowledge (by prior knowledge, I mean the emerging partial model that I
am constructing through this thesis and not the narrow common
definition) state of the child before embarking upon the task. In the
transcript extract below (this extract also appears on page 213), the child
has a positive attitude towards the game being played. This in turn
increases his desire to engage in the mathematical task and has the effect
of further shaping his memory through the experiences gained. This
desire to join in is there prior to the task being set. Therefore individual
motivation is not only there prior to the task, but has further influence on
future prior knowledge.
272
Extract from transcript of first lesson by Mrs Jo Fishily at
Greenville Park Community School
[Children are using a 100 square playing various games]
Teacher: Right who could roll the dice for me? … then we’re gonna
move the button … that many times ok … we’re going
forwards …counting … Josh would you like to roll? just stay
where you are, stay where you are and see if you can roll it
onto the floor … oh what’s it landed on?
Some children: (shout) Six
Teacher: Right, who can put their hand up and guess where I’m going
to have to move button to? … uh let me ask somebody with
their hand up … Louise
Child: Six
Teacher: Yeah, shall we see if you are right? Can you count with me?
Some children: One two three four five six
Teacher: Good girl Louise, right … (whispers) who can roll the dice this
time? … (normal) shh … let’s have Kealee can you roll it onto
the dice onto the snake, ready? … ok … oops pass it to
Kealee … ok don’t worry, you’re gonna have your own dice in
a minute if you don’t get a turn now … ooh … what’s that
landed on?
Some children: (shout) Four
Child: Easy … are we going to get to play this today?
Teacher: Yes four (child makes a fist and punches the air with a smile)
… right put your hand up if you can work out already where
my blue bead’s going to be? … let me ask somebody with
their hand up … let me ask Aiden
Child: Worked it out already its ten …
The next transcript (this transcript extract appears earlier on page 212 as
well) reveals the lack of desire which causes lack of connections to be
made with the tasks. This emotional response is a direct result of the
individual’s prior knowledge and the shape of prior knowledge to support
understanding of the task.
273
Extract from transcript of sixth lesson by Mrs Jo Fishily at
Greenville Park Community School
Teacher: Cylinder … right can you put your hand up if you notice
anything about what is left on my white board this morning …
Jack … what do you notice about what is left on my white
board [the question put on the board was 9+3 =11 children
were asked to consider the question] this morning cause
you’re talking … (long pause) … what do you notice Jack?
Child: Umm I don’t know…it’s too hard… I don’t know…
Teacher: Make a guess
Child: I don’t know
Teacher: Right anyone help … Jack right Hannah what do you notice
Data seem to show that the degree of motivation that individuals have
influences the extent to which memory is drawn upon as can be seen in
the transcript below. Where there is little desire to draw upon any
previous experiences, the shape of the memory will be limited in its effect.
When I look at the example below, I can see that individual motivation
has an influence on shaping memory. The fact that the child is
unmotivated to join in the task limits the degree to which memory may be
modified. The shape of prior knowledge has led to this child perceiving the
mathematical task as being one he cannot do.
Extract from transcript of third lesson by Miss Lora Hunter at St
Paul First School
Teacher: Are we ready … when you found the answer Harry hold it
here … haven’t asked the question yet … going to ask the
question … don’t talk about it … it is what you know … not
what the person next to you knows … Oliver … right seven
subtract two … seven subtract two … shhh … going to ask you
how you did it Oliver not how Matthew did it … how did you
work it out … shhh … well … Abbie do it yourself please … I
think that this table is ready … nearly ready red table …
Oliver
Child: I don’t know how to do it … I can’t do it
274
Teacher: Well just wait till we have all finished … have a guess … shhh
… ha Hannah … nearly ready? show me … remember if you
get it right you just put a thumb up … ok no shouting just a
thumb up … seven subtract two is five … put your hand up if
you can tell me what you had to do? what did that word
subtract mean what was it telling you to do or asking you to
do? Jordan
Child: Oh ok that’s easy Take away
Teacher: Good boy it was a take away … so Oliver what does subtract
mean?
Child: Don’t know…I am not sure ... I can’t remember.
In the next transcript, I see that the child is very keen to consider the
mathematical task in front of him. This individual desire to consider the
mathematics being presented has a changing effect upon memory in
terms of allowing new experiences to enter. Both positive and negative
motivations have an effect upon the memory of a child and therefore the
overall shape of prior knowledge.
Extract from transcript of first lesson by Mrs Jane Marshall at
Argyle Common First School
Teacher: Well done three and that number goes here … what have I
got to put next in my sum … Henry
Child: Equals I know what the answer is I’ve counted you count the
top line. I know what to do can I show can I
Therefore individual motivation is an essential element of prior knowledge
as it has great impact upon shaping of memory, but furthermore is
interconnected with the other elements that make up prior knowledge. As
can be seen from the example in the last transcript, the confidence with
which the child wants to answer is linked to the child’s cognition and
perception of the question presented. Without prior knowledge containing
individual motivation within it, there would be no engagement with the
mathematical tasks presented and thus limiting reshaping and developing
275
of ideas. The shape of prior knowledge before tasks influences what takes
place during the task.
6.6.2 Perception
6.6.2.1 Theoretical Perspective
When looking at what research has to tell me in terms of perception and
mathematics, I am hindered by the many meanings and uses of the word
perception. There is research which has considered perception or views of
teachers and children about the subject of mathematics (Borthwick, 2011;
Burton, 2009). There is also much research about social perception of
mathematics (Malkevitch, 1997; Steele & Ambady, 2006). Perception is
considered in one of two ways – as a feeling and opinion or views of
mathematics, or as a physical aspect of self and how we use our senses to
understand the world around us. It is this latter use of the word
perception that has emerged from my data.
It is of little value to consider the established research on how our senses
perceive as that is not reflected in my data, but more so what children
perceive and how are they making sense of this information. Therefore I
am not looking at pure psychological research on perception, but
considering the applied psychological views of how we develop our
understanding of the world through our perception.
I need to consider two aspects of perception – one which looks at the
sensory modes of how we make sense of the world, and the other which
looks at the cognitive processes to use this sensory understanding and
276
formulate thought. Though I have given these as separate ideas, they are
very much linked.
Pertinent to this understanding of perception are the ideas surrounding
enrichment and differentiation theory (Gibson & Gibson, 1955; Piaget,
1954).
Piaget’s view of enrichment suggests that we
impose meaning on our sensory data, either by
making it fit in with pre-existing schemas or by
generating new ones. ... Gibson’s differentiation
theory proposed that sensory stimulation is all
we need.
(Flanagan, 1996, p. 29)
Children are able to take in the vast amount of sensory information similar
to adults, but just do not have the ability to consider these data due to
lack of experiences (Bower, 1982; Gibson, 1987). Developing this
argument further means that the level of experience of the world is key to
the way in which children make sense of mathematics. That is, children
learn to perceive mathematics (enrichment theory) as opposed to just
considering mathematics as it appears to them without any link to
anything they have experienced before (differentiation theory). It is of
little value to make distinctions between these two ideas as they both
have contributions to make in terms of the perceptions that children bring
to mathematical tasks. That is to say, children go through a combination
of differentiation, i.e. experiencing many repeated stimulations and
beginning to distinguish between them, and enrichment, i.e. through the
development of schemas to allow for more sophisticated understanding
and perception of mathematics.
277
Researchers into perception all agree that it is the awareness of the world
through five senses that develops, shapes and influences how we perceive
the world around us. There is further development on this understanding
of perception.
Perception is not determined simply by stimulus
patterns; rather it is a dynamic searching for the
best interpretation of the available data …
perception involves going beyond the
immediately given evidence of the senses.
(Gregory, 1978, p. 13)
This is confirmed by Coon (1989) who defines perception as “the process
of assembling sensations in a useable mental representation of the world”
(p. 137). Therefore the way in which children perceive mathematical tasks
is dependent upon how they have experienced and interpreted the world.
6.6.2.2 Definition
In my prior knowledge model, perception is the set of sensory experiences
that children bring to mathematical tasks and the interpretations that they
have already established from these experiences in line with the
theoretical understanding debated earlier.
The interpretation children make of each mathematical task they are set is
linked to what they are using to perceive these tasks. The perspective of
the child is based on the exposure they have had in the past or are
recalling from memory. Transcripts show that it is not just exposure to
mathematics that has influenced the perception of children, but more so
their exposure to many different things, some being sensory and some
being ideas they have explored. It seems that on approaching a task,
278
children look at it from a particular viewpoint and this is linked to what
they are recalling that they feel helps to understand what they are
presented with.
The transcripts show children perceiving the mathematical tasks from
many vantage points:
the physical patterns on the page;
numbers written and what they mean to a child;
having seen this before and the form it took;
perception of the challenge of the task;
the inference a child has made from the task set.
6.6.2.3 Empirical Evidence
The transcript below shows that children have the need to put unknown
ideas into a format that is supported by something they have experienced
before. The perception of one child is very different to that of another.
Extract from transcript of first lesson by Mrs Jane Marshall at
Argyle Common First School
Teacher: If Sharna said none if Sharna said none that’s the answer
what
Child: It isn’t a shape
Teacher: What would the question be?
Child: Umm
Teacher: The answer none the answer is nothing so what would the
question be Devon?
Child: (whispers) zero?
Child: What is it?
Teacher: No … the answer isn’t zero cause you know we are thinking
about shapes … Sharna gave the answer none … what would
the question be Lauren?
Child: What have I got in my hand with none in it?
279
Perception influences the shaping of memory to support the
understanding and achievement of the mathematical task presented. As
can be seen in the transcript below, one child perceives diagonal lines
folded on a square as making a diamond while another child perceives the
same as two triangles. This in itself does not change the outcome of the
task, but is a factor in how children will approach the task. The approach
taken shapes prior knowledge and hence memory, which in the long term
affects the approaches and methods children use in their understanding of
shapes.
Extract from transcript of first lesson by Mrs Jane Marshall at
Argyle Common First School
Teacher: Brilliant excellent that’s it there … that’s it and Ashley said
it’s got a point we are not sure whether it’s a point or a
corner … right … right everybody looking this way … James
shh we’ve got to look and listen … I’ve a piece of paper shh
Few children: Oh
Teacher: And I’ve folded my
Child: In half
Teacher: Piece of paper in half
Child: Like a square
Teacher: Yes it does look a bit like a square now … and this … is the
fold ok … so if I open it out can you see the crease down the
middle? … .now … shh excuse me … on my piece of paper I
am going to draw two lines … from the fold
Child: One two … a triangle
Teacher: What shape have I drawn Lauren?
Few children: Triangle
Teacher: Triangle ok right
Child Child Child (three children speaking simultaneously): She is
going to cut it out … she’ll end up two triangles … she’ll end
up with a diamond
[The three girls in this conversation are sitting and talking
quite actively as to what is going to be the outcome of what
they are seeing. There is a sense that they all want to be
right and perceive vehemently that their estimation of the
outcome will be correct.]
280
Teacher: Shh … excuse me … why will I end up with two Lauren?
Child: Because … because … if you … she’ll end up with a diamonds
Teacher: Shh Lauren’s talking
Child: Because if you folded it up it would make a diamond and if
you chop it at the bottom you’ll have triangles … because
when you folded it over it had two pieces so when you cut it’ll
still have two pieces
Teacher: Ok … I understood ok … if I open it out what shape have I
got Damian?
Child: Two triangles
The function of perception within the partial prior knowledge structure is
to support memory modification. But furthermore there are influences
between how tasks are perceived and how this perception affects the
other elements of the prior knowledge model such as individual motivation
and the context that they draw upon.
The transcript below illustrates how the phrasing of a task is perceived so
differently compared to the intention of the task. The child has perceived
the task as one and four, and has concluded fourteen and not five. This
was a common occurrence in my data in terms of the different perceptions
of language used and implemented by children. The difference in
perception of the vocabulary used by teachers to describe the task and
how children perceive these words is shown here.
Extract from transcript of second lesson by Mrs Sally Crane at
Hatton First School
Teacher: One add four makes … good boy Richard you’re working it
out really well I want everybody … what answer do you think
it is Elly?
Child: 14 (child is holding up 1 and 4)
There is a constant change in perception of children as time passes and
this change in perception has both influence upon memory and memory
281
has an influence upon perception. This can be seen through the transcript
extract below.
Extract from transcript of sixth lesson by Mrs Rebecca Rice at St
Paul First School
Teacher: 9 and what make 10 … write it on your white board
Child: (using his fingers) umm ... 9, 10 … ooh its 1
Teacher: Show me … good … Now … ready … ok ... 8 and what make
10
Child: (again using his fingers) 8, 9, 10…..
Teacher: Good 2 … now let see if you can do 7 and what make…10
Child: This is going 1 and 2 then 3… (child just writes the answer
without the use of his fingers)
Teacher: Ok then ... let’s look at 6 and what make 10
Child: (shouts) 4 it’s going 1 2 3 4 …
In the transcript above, the child starts the task looking at ideas of
number bonds using his fingers to support a solution and perceiving it as
a problem to solve using this method. However by the end there is a
change in this perception to doing these questions as a pattern that has a
logical order to it. Along with this visual change in perception, the
inference made by the child of how best to achieve the task has also
changed. In the transcript below, another child has approached a similar
task in a contrasting manner. Here the child has perceived this as a
problem which requires a number line to support a solution and one where
counting back from 10 is required as opposed to the previous example
where the child was counting on from 10.
Extract from transcript of eighth lesson by Miss Lora Hunter at St
Paul First School
[The class is working on the carpet and they are looking at
number bonds to 10]
Teacher: The next one ... are we ready? … what do I have to add to 4
to make ten?
282
Child: (points to the number line on the wall) 10, 9, 8, 7, 6 … its 6
Teacher: Well done 6
My data have shown the subjective and individual nature of perception
that children bring to each task. In part therefore, the perception of
children has an impact on their ability to achieve the task.
In terms of my prior knowledge model, perception forms an element
which shapes memory. As the data have demonstrated, the perception of
children influences their approach to the given task. Also it is the flexible
nature of perception that supports children’s approach to a mathematical
task and functions as a facet of prior knowledge.
6.6.3 Cognition
6.6.3.1 Theoretical Perspective
There are a plethora of perspectives in relation to how individuals cognate
and what this process entails. Throughout history there have been many
themes about this seemingly unique ability that individuals have to learn
and comprehend. It is of little value for me to consider in any great detail
all the various views and opinions which have been put forward by many
eminent researchers on cognition. I feel that in terms of understanding
what role cognition has as an element of prior knowledge, it is important
to consider the key concepts and ideas in terms of the role they have to
play in the development of prior knowledge. Cognition, in very simplistic
terms, is a way to understand the world.
283
Tait-McCutcheon (2008) states that “cognition refers to the process of
coming to know and understand; the process of storing, processing, and
retrieving information” (p. 507). This idea of cognition and what its
functions are must not be confused with theories provided by many such
as Thorndike, Schoenfeld, Piaget, Anderson, and Bruner which consider
how individuals cognate. This distinction is critical in my model as the role
of cognition within my prior knowledge model is as an element of prior
knowledge which depicts that knowledge which is already known.
Ashcraft (1982) states that there are many methodologies used by
children for retrieval, e.g. number facts from memory, which have been
conceptualised as being automatic skills which do not require any
reflection. Theories of cognition have demonstrated that after processing
understanding of mathematical tasks through various processes such as
Dienes’ (1971) perspective of cognition through practical tasks or Piaget’s
(1954) view that we construct knowledge through our experiences, the
ultimate outcome is that within mathematics, there are concepts that we
eventually realise into our memory and they remain there unchallenged
and unchanged and become intuitive.
For example, if we consider how we calculate 12+1 is 13, adults may find
it very difficult to explain the mental stages involved in arriving at the
answer. However in the past, prior to knowing that 12+1 is 13, there
would have been a series of experiences to allow you time to revisit the
question and explore the ideas and concepts which allow an
understanding of why 12+1 is 13. Thus over time it becomes intuitive. It
is this ability to access answers without any prompts that manifests itself
284
in the transcripts. In prior knowledge, cognition is knowledge which has
become tacit through experience. Children bring an element of tacit
knowledge as part of their prior knowledge.
6.6.3.2 Definition
In my prior knowledge model, cognition is the efficiency and level of
accuracy with which children complete mathematical tasks. It is not to be
mistaken for the process used to complete the task. Throughout the
observations, there were some tasks or parts of mathematical tasks that
the children seemed to be able to do with little or no reference to
anything. It appears as if there are some things that children intuitively
know. These events have been classified in my prior knowledge model as
cognition. Cognition is the ability to carry out a task or series of tasks in
as few steps as possible giving the appearance of intuition or being tacit.
6.6.3.3 Empirical Evidence
Looking at the first transcript below, it can be seen that children are able
to attempt with clarity, accuracy and little link or acknowledgement of any
other idea to support the formulation of an answer. In the second
transcript, I observed a child who is not only very able to solve the
mathematical problem, but also able to explain his answer with clarity.
Extract from transcript of second lesson by Mrs Jane Marshall at
Argyle Common First School
Teacher: Eighty and two … how many tens in eighty Lauren?
Child: Eight
Teacher: How many ten pences would I need?
Child: Eight
Teacher: And how many pennies would I need?
285
Child: Two
Extract from transcript of third lesson by Mrs Jennie Brooks at
Draycott First School
Teacher: Three … ten take away something makes seven … are you
ready think what Matthew has just done …let us try another
… ten take something makes eight … Matthew
Child: Two
Teacher: How did you know that because I didn’t see you doing it?
Child: You know that eight add two make ten so ten take away two
is eight
In the next transcript, children are also able to identify with speed,
accuracy and without any prompting errors that they make.
Extract from transcript of eighth lesson by Mrs Jo Fishily at
Greenville Park Community School
Teacher: Give me a take away sum where the answer is six
Child: 25–16 no I mean umm nine
Teacher: Ok we need a take away sum where the answer is six?
Child: Umm twenty-six take away twenty
In the following transcripts, children are able to provide answers without
any hesitation or further steps.
Extract from transcript of third lesson by Mrs Jill Thomas at St
Paul First School
Teacher: Right let’s see if we can remember the different words we
used for … addition and subtraction … remember all those
different words so you have got to listen very carefully …
remember you are not going show until I say ready steady
show … right three plus four … three plus four … ready steady
show … three plus four is … ?
Most children: Seven … (hold up their white boards)
Teacher: Seven … let’s do another one nine add two … nine add two …
nine add two
Child: Eleven …… (holds up his white board)
286
Extract from transcript of ninth lesson by Mrs Sally Crane at
Hatton First School
Teacher: What is one less than eleven?
Child: Ten
Teacher: Brilliant how did you do it?
Child: I just put one finger and took it away and I knew it was ten
Teacher: You just did it in your head did you ... you just did it?
Child: Yes
The structure of prior knowledge contains elements of pre-understood
cognition. In common definitions of prior knowledge, it is this ability to
cognate that is mistakenly called prior knowledge. Looking at the data,
children appear to already have the relevant subject knowledge without
any indication showing the need to learn this subject knowledge. There is
a sense that this knowledge has always been part of the children, waiting
for the correct moment for it to be part of an individual’s approach to a
task. Where has this knowledge come from? The transcripts reveal that
the knowledge has been built up as a result of past experiences which
have changed or modified past memory. When considering the overall
range of data, a theme to come through is a notion of practice and
repeated exposure to mathematical ideas. This repetitious exposure to the
same mathematical concept explains the eventual efficiency, fluency and
accuracy with no further changes to that aspect of memory which I have
labelled as cognition. From other sections of this chapter, I have seen that
this repetitious engagement involves children using other elements of
prior knowledge in order to comprehend and make sense of mathematical
tasks and eventually gaining fluency. The process of repetitious practice
removes knowledge from its original context as it becomes tacit.
287
When engaged in some familiar tasks, cognition appears to be the
dominant element of prior knowledge. The development of cognition is
very much influenced by all other elements of prior knowledge. However
in some mathematical tasks, it is the one which has been brought to bear
upon the task. Cognition is not about measuring ability, it represents the
aspect of prior knowledge where children have become efficient. Prior
knowledge has, for any given aspect, been built up through different
elements playing a lead role, and in the case of cognition this has been
manifested through the lack of need to reference to any other external
framework to understand the task. The children’s prior knowledge has
been shaped so that it can be applied to the task with efficiency. Cognition
must not be confused with intelligence, but more so with the structure of
prior knowledge at that moment. Each time children engage in
mathematical tasks, they bring uniqueness in how each element of prior
knowledge influences their understanding and approach to the
mathematical tasks.
6.6.4 Social Group
6.6.4.1 Theoretical Perspective
The element of prior knowledge which has caused the greatest complexity
in defining, though we all understand when we read the words, is social
groups. What is roughly meant by this expression? We all have a very
different interpretation of who constitute our social groups. However we
can agree that there is a common understanding.
288
A social group can be defined as two or more
individuals who share a common social
identification of themselves.
(Turner, 2010, p. 15)
Furthermore researchers offer ideas of how to classify social groups in
terms of common characteristics, though these do not support my thesis
(Cooley, 1909; Sumner, 1907). Ellwood’s (1919) classification of social
groups offers some clarity in terms of the groups that manifested
themselves in my research – permanent/temporary. Permanent groups
consist of parents and siblings, and this can be extended to any
individuals that form longer relationships such as grandparents, and in
terms of the modern family unit, extended family such as half-siblings and
so on. In Ellwood’s terms, temporary groups, in contrast to permanent
groups, are individuals who have limited relations in terms of length of
time such as friends, friends of siblings, and so on. Over time some
members may move from temporary to permanent. However in terms of
my data, this classification suffices.
Of greater importance are the functions of social groups as stated by Park
and Burgess (1921).
The individual is influenced in differing degrees
and in a specific manner, by the different types
of group of which he is a member.
(Park & Burgess, 1921, p. 52)
This influence of groups upon children as members of a social group
shapes what children bring to their mathematical tasks and influences the
way in which they are able to engage with mathematical tasks.
289
6.6.4.2 Definition
In my prior knowledge model, the experiences that children have with
other people and how those experiences have shaped their ability to
understand and approach mathematical tasks are what I am calling social
groups. The transcripts below provide some examples which help to define
social groups in terms of my model. These transcripts show how children
use the ideas that they have established through their interactions with
other people to support understanding and achievement of tasks.
The data show that the people that children have had experience of fall
into two groups – family and friends. All of the analysis which was marked
as belonging to social group belongs to these two groups. Not included in
this area of social group are teachers as these would form part of
acculturation (Section6.4).
6.6.4.3 Empirical Evidence
In the transcript below, this child’s understanding and familiarity with
money is linked to the experiences and conversations she has already had
with her mother. Prior knowledge has been shaped by the ideas explored
in the past with her mother.
Extract from transcript of third lesson by Mrs Jennie Brooks at
Draycott First School
Teacher: Money sums that’s right … right sit in a circle please … umm
you two are talking to much … don’t … up the top James well
spotted come on Holly … over by Scott … right let’s see what
I’ve got
Child: Looks like money … lots of money… I have 100 pennies in my
piggy bank. Mummy says I can spend it when I am big.
290
In the transcript below, the impact on this child’s tackling of this question
is influenced by how he has experienced the reduction of numbers in
terms of balloons being burst. In this case, there is an overlap between
the element of social group and the element of context represented by
balloons.
Extract from transcript of sixth lesson by Miss Lora Hunter at St
Paul First School
[Children are looking at a card with balloons on it and
crossing out balloons to do a subtraction sum]
Teacher: It was it was a take away … how do we know? how do you
know it was a take away? … Kieran?
Child: Balloons burst and went… my brother does that … he jumps
on balloons and they pop. I don’t like the noise and I lose my
balloons (child looks down and is sad)
Teacher: That’s it the balloons burst so they went away
In the next transcript, it can be seen that ideas developed about addition
are associated with siblings and their ages. This child has used a
chronological number line in order to complete the question asked.
Extract from transcript of sixth lesson by Miss Lora Hunter at St
Paul First School
Teacher: And four more how many do six and four more make … count
them
Child: Nine
Teacher: Count it
Child: One two three four five six seven eight nine
Teacher: How many does it make?
Child: Umm
Teacher: She’s not sure let’s ask somebody else to see if we can have
a clue how they worked it out … Abbie what did you do?
Child: I got six on the number line like me then I jumped on four
more like my sister who is a baby… so six first cos I am
bigger then my baby sister four
291
Another factor derived from the transcripts which links to social groups
and affects how prior knowledge is structured are the ideas and concepts
children have about attitudes and approaches of other members of the
social group towards mathematics. As can be seen in the transcript below,
in this case the child has linked his understanding and ability to complete
this task to the notion that this is as a result of his father’s abilities in
mathematics. I can extrapolate from this example that attitudes of
individual members of the social group have an influence on the approach
and outcome children achieve on mathematical tasks.
Extract from transcript of fifth lesson by Mrs Jennie Brooks at
Draycott First School
Teacher: Well done … how did you do it … Harry if I see bits all over
the floor I shall be cross … ready listen to what I am saying …
four p Harry four p add three p add … five p four p add three
p add five p
Child: I can’t do it on here
Teacher: Shhh shhh yes you can
Child: How do you do it?
Teacher: Have a go Bethany … hold it up … shhh who is going to be
first four p add three p add five p
Child: That is one two three
Teacher: Well done Nathan well done Bethany … no Matthew well done
Chloe
Child: I know four five
Teacher: Shhh … all right add it up Scott … well done Jack … no
Matthew … shhh shhh
Child: It’s
Teacher: No I haven’t asked a number yet
Child: It’s twelve
Teacher: Well done Zeno you are good at these
Child: My daddy is good at these
The next transcript illustrates another aspect of how social groups have an
impact upon children’s ability and methods used to address mathematical
292
tasks. In the transcript below, the child has recalled counting in four’s
through experiencing his friend’s singing.
Extract from transcript of sixth lesson by Mrs Jane Marshall at
Argyle Common First School
[The children build up the pattern of fours to ten]
Child: I know it ends at forty
Teacher: Why
Child: I counted up in four’s … you know Joe (child whispers to his
friend) Poppy sings her counting in four’s …
Children’s level of confidence and security is dependent upon the
encouragement children have experienced from members of their social
group. In the transcript below, the child is comfortable in making further
attempts to answer a question as she has had positive experiences from
her mother even when she has made an error. In this case, this ability
and emotion to attempt the question again impacts upon prior knowledge
and memory. Conversely the impact still exists even when there are
negative experiences from social groups. In terms of shaping prior
knowledge, all experiences have some form of impact upon it.
Extract from transcript of fourth lesson by Mrs Rebecca Rice at St
Paul First School
[The teacher is talking about a worksheet children did at
home and brought into school. The task was to identify odd
and even numbers by colouring them red and blue.]
Teacher: Number fifteen Sam is
Child: Even…
Teacher: Are you sure?
Child: Umm… its 5 and 0…so…can I have another go….?
Teacher: Not fifty … fifteen … yes…
Child: I did this with mummy and said I need to keep trying… it
is…odd?
Teacher: Well done…
293
Children often formulate the question in terms of a scenario they have
already experienced as in the second transcript above where the
disappearance or bursting of balloons and his big brother left the child
with fewer balloons. Furthermore the link to taking away or simple
understanding of numbers is connected to personal factors such as age
and size onto the order in which numbers should be added. Children’s
understanding of numbers is in part linked to actual people. This use of
social groups to formulate understanding is not limited to just numbers,
but also to how concepts in mathematics are formulated.
The social groups that children belong to are all unique and different and,
as seen in the transcripts above, have a huge impact upon how children
approach mathematical tasks. Therefore the influence of social groups
upon children’s prior knowledge was determined by the factors below:
Using their social group as a frame of reference to understand and
conceptualise the mathematical tasks they are set;
Time that members of the social group spend with the child
engaged in mathematical activities and conversations;
The attitudes and approaches of individual members of the social
group towards mathematics;
The level of security that children feel within this social group to
make mistakes.
The other feature of social groups is the interaction between children
while working on mathematical tasks. The co-construction of
understanding has an impact on the effect social groups have on
development of understanding and shaping of memory. These ideas are
294
reflected through elements of Vygotsky’s (1978) work on social
construction of learning and development.
Every function in the child’s cultural
development appears twice: first, on the social
level, and later, on the individual level; first,
between people..., and then inside the child.
This applies equally to voluntary attention, to
logical memory, and to the formation of
concepts. All the higher [mental] functions
originate as actual relations between human
individuals.
(Vygotsky, 1978, p. 57)
6.6.5 Abstraction
6.6.5.1 Theoretical Perspective
Ferrari (2003) states that “abstraction is a fundamental process in
mathematics ... abstraction is a basic step in the creation of new
concepts” (p. 1225). Mitchelmore and White (2007) provide further
information by stating that “abstraction has been a frequent discussion
topic since the days of Aristotle and Plato” (p. 1).
Therefore it is vital that I consider what this term means, so that there
can be development in understanding the structure of prior knowledge.
Mathematics, by its very nature, demands abstraction as “mathematics
uses everyday words, but their meaning is defined precisely in relation to
other mathematical terms and not by their everyday meaning”
(Mitchelmore & White, 2004, p. 329).
Within the primary classroom, this duality of language is ever-present and
the children make many interpretations. The process of abstraction is
295
important to consider as within mathematics children go through a
transition from something they find quite complex and very specific to a
generalised idea, e.g. from counting and only being able to count objects
by touching them to being able to count anything without needing to have
it even physically present.
This notion that an element of abstraction is about being able to generate
and formulate rules is important in terms of what I have seen in my data.
This idea does have a link to the cognitive processes that children have
acquired and a concept that has been abstracted may appear as cognition
in the classroom as we have seen with the counting example (being able
to count fluently).
The other process which must be considered as a way in which we
abstract is this notion of decontextualisation. Ferrari (2003) states that
“generalisation implies a certain degree of decontextualization” (p. 1226).
The German mathematician Hilbert’s idea that all mathematical tasks
must be eventually stripped of everything that is not essential causes
some difficulty in terms of young children’s acquisition of mathematical
understanding. It is necessary for them to have some degree of
generalisation and application of their understanding. Often children use
contexts to gain a form of abstraction and the memory they glean from
the task to support them in applying ideas to other situations.
Skemp (1987) offers an alternative view of abstraction being empirical
abstraction.
296
Abstraction is an activity by which we become
aware of similarities ... among our experiences.
Classifying means collecting together our
experiences on the basis of these similarities. An
abstraction is some kind of lasting mental
change, the result of abstracting, which enables
us to recognize new experiences as having the
similarities of an already formed class. ... To
distinguish between abstracting as an activity
and an abstraction as its end-product, we shall
hereafter call the latter a concept.
(Skemp, 1987, p. 21; italics in original)
To summarise therefore, the individual journey towards mathematical
abstraction is going to be very different for each child and each
mathematical task they face. The ways in which children abstract is part
of prior knowledge as the data have shown that it has an influence upon
how children approach their tasks.
6.6.5.2 Definition
In my prior knowledge model, the ideas that children use to make
meaning of mathematical tasks and support their understanding of and
engagement in the task are what I am calling abstraction. It is when
children demonstrate an understanding of similarities between two or
more ideas, and further can use these ideas to develop and understand
new concepts within mathematics.
6.6.5.3 Empirical Evidence
The transcript below shows how a child has used the definition and
characteristics of a shape given by the teacher and linked them to an ice
cream cone. This extrapolation and association of the description to
something which the child has experience of has allowed her to relate to
297
the mathematics being discussed. Further she has been able to take the
concrete example of an ice cream cone and abstract it to a generalised
definition of a geometrical shape.
Extract from transcript of third lesson by Mrs Jo Fishily at
Greenville Park Community School
Teacher: Don’t worry about anyone else just worry about your own
answer please … Shakar can you sit up nice and straight …
well what is that one?
Child: Cone
Teacher: Yes it is a slanty cone that doesn’t matter it’s got one flat
surface and a point
Child: Ice-cream cone … so that is a cone …
Abstraction is also the understanding of symbols and their operations.
Within mathematics, the ability to manipulate numbers without actually
doing the task physically is required. In the transcript below, children are
asked to change numbers to formulate new valid sums. The child shows
the ability to move numbers around in a meaningful manner. He also
understands the notions and ideas of operations as symbols having
meaning in a physical sense without really carrying out these tasks
physically. There is initially some confusion in terms of the symbol of
division and multiplication and also the understanding of numbers.
Extract from transcript of third lesson by Mrs Jo Fishily at
Greenville Park Community School
[Children are asked to make new valid sums based on these
numbers (12 ÷ 6 = 2 or 12 ÷ 2 = 6) … taking the opposite
operation of the one above asking the children to deduce
from the knowledge they have to apply it to a new fact and
create a multiplication sum]
Child: 6 ÷ 12
Teacher: Ok 6 ÷ 12 … what will the answer be
Child: Oops no it’s six times
Teacher: Hang on shh shh Andrew you have a go
298
Child: Six times two equals twelve
The data show the children used visual ideas to understand mathematical
tasks through the use of symbolic representation to understand the tasks
they are being asked to engage in. In the transcript below, children are
working on patterns. The fact that this child is able to abstract from this
pattern to one which is symmetrical and is like looking in a mirror helps
children to visualise the pattern without ever having to use an actual
mirror.
Extract from transcript of eighth lesson by Mrs Jennie Brooks at
Draycott First School
[The teacher has arranged a few children in a pattern
(without telling the children why she is arranging them in this
particular way as a symmetrical pattern) and they are now
asked to explain what they see. Children pick up on the
pattern there is much talk amongst the children.]
Child: Two boys are facing the two boys on that side and one girl is
facing a girl on that side
Child: Oh (shouts) it’s just like a mirror!!!
Teacher: Go on … only it’s not like a mirror because look at him and
look at you
Child: Cause it’s all a bent line
Teacher: What do you mean?
Child: A mirror is like this [child expresses a straight line with her
hand]
[The children are split in half by the teacher and positioned in
a different pattern and asked to look at the others and see if
they can make sure they are standing in the same way …
each child is looking closely and tries to replicates what they
see on the other side the line]
Teacher: Now Matthew said it was like a mirror … what do we call it
when it’s like a mirror what’s that big word? S SS
Child: Similar
Teacher: Nearly Jack
Child: Symmetry
Teacher: Yes that’s right … it is symmetrical
299
In the next transcript, children have understood the ideas involved in
addition and finding numbers that help reach a particular value. They
demonstrate the ability to combine two sets of numbers together. This
ability to use number facts in the task demonstrates the ability to abstract
and understand features of numbers and values.
Extract from transcript of eighth lesson by Mrs Jill Thomas at St
Paul First School
[The children all have number fans on the table they are
going to use those to carry out some questions on number
bonds of ten … the teacher has instructed that she will say
one or hold a number and the children are to find another
they think when added will make ten]
Teacher: When I hold up say umm the number two you will hold up
number
Child: Eight
Teacher: Right you will hold up number eight … well done
Child: So you’ve got to get the number to ten
Teacher: Yes … you don’t need to say the number you don’t need to
say eight or anything else … you need to keep it to yourself
till I say ready steady show … all right … I am not going to
say what my number is I think you can read the number
[The children are given a few examples first by the teacher
saying the number … then she just shows the number … the
children all are trying to work independently but there are
still some who like to see if their answer is correct in relation
to others and lack confidence in their answer … once the
children have shown their answer the number bond is said
out loud]
Teacher: Three and
Most children: Seven
Children and teacher: Seven make ten
The notion of abstraction is dependent upon other elements of prior
knowledge and later formulates the ability to abstract more widely as in
the third transcript or in Piagetian terms:
Piaget (1977) made a distinction between
abstraction on the basis of superficial
300
characteristics of physical objects (abstraction à
partir de l’objet) and abstraction on the basis of
relationships perceived when the learner
manipulates these objects (abstraction à partir
de l’action). But both are based on the child’s
physical and social experience, and in both
similarity recognition is essential. In using the
term empirical abstraction to cover both cases,
we are making the distinction between
abstraction on the basis of experience and what
we shall call theoretical abstraction.
(Mitchelmore & White, 2004, p. 332)
Abstraction based on physical objects like a mirror or abstraction based on
relationships established when the learner manipulates objects as in the
second transcript leads to abstraction of theoretical concepts as in the
fourth transcript.
6.7 Summary
Having considered the elements emerging from the data that form a
possible structure of prior knowledge and how they influence the central
category of memory, it is essential to finally look at my partial model, how
the elements all fit together and form a possible structure of prior
knowledge, and how may the model function when children are engaged
in mathematical tasks.
Before that is possible, it is essential to consider the lenses which have
been used to carry out this study or, in Glaserian terms, theoretical
sensitivity.
Theoretical sensitivity is the ability to recognize
what is important in data and to give it
meaning. It helps to formulate a theory that is
301
faithful to the reality of the phenomena under
study.
(Strauss & Corbin, 1990, p. 46)
There are many sources of alignment I have used, all of which have
shaped my understanding and inclination to consider the data in a very
particular manner. Ironically it is the process of researching that has
shaped my senses and allowed me to understand data through reading,
experiences in the classroom, and crucially having spent a long time
thinking about the question “What is prior knowledge?” and looking for an
answer. The desire to understand this phenomenon has heightened my
determination to look for ideas and use many variations to try and fit and
find solutions to the questions. The mere fact that I have spent a large
amount of time thinking about the solution to my question and looking for
an answer means that unusual and creative processes are at work to
make connections which will address the puzzle I am faced with. The
stages of research and the process of analysis itself have shaped the
thinking and evaluations I am able to carry out. Literature for example
has allowed me to consider ideas which may not have been developed in
relation to prior knowledge but could be applied to its understanding. It
allows for the individual creative nature of qualitative analysis to take
hold. It acknowledges that analysis and understanding of data requires a
degree of creativity, problem solving and imagination as the solution is
not concrete, and therefore must be visualised and then put into concrete
form. Like an architect, the researcher is only limited by lack of
imagination and creative thinking, and is also limited by the lack of
practical know how. This idea of being able to link events and concepts in
302
creative new ways to make connections that are not limited by the past is
quite exciting. Furthermore the more I look at the data and interact with
the data, the better I understand what the data are trying to tell me.
Together with reading and exploring through experiences in the
classroom, the emerging model developed has a rich layer of process and
imagination based in conceptual experiences.
As each element was being explored and defined through what the data
showed, one of the key themes to emerge was the marked influence of
each element upon the shape of memory.
An essential point to note is that the structure of prior knowledge is not
static, but one that is moving and changing shape through the interaction
between the elements. Like a snowflake made up of oxygen and hydrogen
and changing shape as it passes through the environment, prior
knowledge also functions in a similar manner. Also what has become
apparent through the process of analysis is that there may be other
categories which make up prior knowledge. It is not possible for me to
definitive in the claim that these are the only constituents of prior
knowledge. The vastness of human understanding and its ever-changing
nature make it problematic to insist that the eight categories I have
proposed are definitive and there may be no more additions to the model.
The interconnected nature of each element is key to understanding how
prior knowledge functions. With every task, children bring different
aspects of prior knowledge to help them complete or understand solving
the mathematical challenges that they face. The choice of which element
they bring to bear is dependent on the shape of their memory. The
303
shaping of one element influences that of all others. For example, when a
child is working out, as in the transcript below, what half of a circle is,
they have a perception that they bring to bear upon the task, that of a
pizza which they have linked to toys they played with in the nursery – the
acculturation experiences they have had. Also they further used their
motivation and ability to metacognate in terms of their approach when a
child states “I have done this before”, “it’s easy”. All of this occurs before
the task is even attempted or becomes possible and on attempts made by
the child.
Extract from transcript of sixth lesson by Mrs Helen Fellows at
Greenville Park Community School
Teacher: What shape do we get if we cut this circle in half … like this?
Child: A moon
Child: It’s like that pretend pizza my sister plays with in nursery
Child: Ooh yes … I have played with that when I was a baby…
Teacher: Do we know what the name of the shape is?
Child: It’s easy
Teacher: Ok any guesses
Child: Umm a chopped circle
Teacher: A good guess but no … anyone else…
Through all the data analysed, there are many different combinations of
each element being used to attempt mathematical tasks. The pattern of
prior knowledge changes in a kaleidoscopic manner. As in a kaleidoscope,
the elements are the same, but keep changing in shape. This will in turn
affect all the other elements, as there are new experiences to alter the
memory, and this in turn reshapes the elements of prior knowledge. In
this case, prior knowledge is more than just prior knowledge of fractions,
it is a series of elements which link together to form modifiers of memory
304
and thus the process of how tasks are managed. Hence each time children
engage in mathematical tasks, they bring their own unique prior
knowledge which is composed of these eight elements, and possibly
others that were not revealed through my data, but in different
proportions for different children as illustrated in Figure 6.2.
Metacognition
Context Acculturation
Richard
Cognition Abstraction Jack
Jonathan
Individual
Social groups
motivation
Perception
Figure 6.2 Composition of three children’s prior knowledge
Figure 6.2 depicts the prior knowledge of three children – Richard, Jack
and Jonathan – as they engage in mathematical tasks related to shapes.
In the transcript extract below, Richard is engaging in the task of naming
shapes. In order to achieve this task, Richard is recalling some ideas from
past experience in terms of the properties of a circle and how this related
to having seen the shape before in the context of an object he has at
home. There is some element of perception in terms of similarities with a
frame for making pompoms, but the context of having used it before at
home has allowed him to name the shape. In the process, he does reflect
305
elements of individual motivation when he is unable to name the shape
right away.
Extract from transcript of second lesson by Mrs Sally Crane at
Hatton First School
Teacher: Elly and Charlotte go and sit back please … now then let’s
have a look at these shapes … now then I am going to ask
you some questions about these shapes … I am going to
choose somebody … to start … to start us off … now then let
me see Richard … would you like to choose one shape? all
right and then see what you can tell me about it? … just
choose one shape and see what you can tell me about it? …
which one would you like to choose? … right now what can
you tell us about that shape?
Child: Umm it’s yellow
Teacher: Turn around and let’s see everybody … show everyone …
right it’s yellow … very good
Child: It’s yellow and it’sss round andddd it’s got no endss and
ummm it’s quite
Teacher: That’s a very good start … a very good start
Child: Umm
Teacher: Do you know what that shape is called?
Child: No
Teacher: Have a try what is it called?
Child: It’s a pompom… It looks like the one my hat
Teacher: No it isn’t … have another go
Child: Its squashed … but has a bit of a side… (child points to the
edge of the plastic shape)
Teacher: Is it a square, triangle or circle?
Child: Circle…. why has it got fat bits at the bottom then like the
thing I’ve got to make pompoms where you have to circle
round to make it … just no hole?
In the transcript extract below, Jack is engaging in the task of identifying
shapes. But in contrast to Richard, he has used acculturation in that he
identifies that he has done this in school with the teaching assistant. Also
there is an element of cognition in that he was able to recall the fact that
they had done 2D shapes. With this, he has also noted that he has played
306
with something similar at home with a parent (social group). All these
elements to some extent support the way in which Jack tackles the task.
Extract from transcript of second lesson by Miss Lora Hunter at St
Paul First School
Teacher: That was in the afternoon … what were we doing yesterday
morning in maths?
Child: Shapes
Teacher: Look at this … what kind of shapes Jack?
Child: 2D shapes
Teacher: Well done Jack … 2D flat shapes now I am….Right I am
thinking of a shape and I am going to describe that 2D shape
and I want you to
Child: This is cool the same as we played in pairs before and with
Mum … outside with Mrs Jones. [Mrs Jones is the TA]
Teacher: Put your hand up in the air if you have guessed what that
shape Harry Harris is holding … ok
Child: Mrs Jones had that one … triangle
In the transcript extract below, Jonathan is engaging in the task of
identifying properties of shapes. In this case, there is a large element of
metacognition which is supporting Jonathan. He acknowledges that the
answer he has given is wrong and needs to be corrected. Using his sense
of what a square and rectangle are in order to address the task, we see in
this extract there are a few occurrences where he notes his error and tries
to correct them (metacognition). He does perceive the difference between
the square and rectangle and talks about the square being pulled
(perception), and goes on to tackle the question with the support of
metacognition. His ability to identify and count corners quickly without
any errors or explanation indicates elements of cognition.
307
Extract from transcript of second lesson by Mrs Rebecca Rice at St
Paul First School
Teacher: Look at my shape Jonathan what can you tell me about
what’s special about it? … why is it different to the square?
Child: It’s got four corners
Teacher: Four corners that’s the same as a square it’s got four corners
hasn’t it? … Connor what’s different about a square and a
rectangle?
Child: It’s a bit different … I know what I mean … I remember … it
is pulled …
Teacher: What’s different about it?
Child: It hasn’t got the same shape … no that is wrong … I know
ummm (child waves hand)
Teacher: What do you mean by it hasn’t got the same shape?
Child: Cause that one’s different than that … before I said corners …
not corners its
Teacher: It is different isn’t it? sides on the top are not the same are
they? … Daniel Harvey and Thomas … what’s different
Jonathan? I know it’s different but I don’t what know you
mean by different
Child: It’s … that one hasn’t got the same corners
Teacher: Not got the same corners it’s got four corners though hasn’t
it?
Child: No not corners … … one two three four one two three four …
it the same but it’s not quite the same shape I got that word
Teacher: It still has four corners but it’s not quite the same shape …
Ellis can you tell what is the difference between these two?
Jonathan has just told us it’s got four corners but it’s not the
same shape … what’s different about it? … go on Jonathan
Child: Cause that one is a rectangle and that one’s a square
Teacher: Yes that one is a rectangle and that one a square but how do
you know what’s different about them? … what makes that
one a rectangle and that one a square?
Child: Cause they’re not the same
Teacher: You are quite right they are not the same so what’s different
about them?
Child: All the other squares are not the same … I am in a muddle
Teacher: They are not the same all the squares are not the same
Child: That one’s bigger
Teacher: Well you can get large squares and small squares can’t you
but that’s not … can you see Jonathan? can you tell me what
the difference is between these two shapes?
308
Child: Well that one’s got longer corners and shorter corners … I
know what I mean (child points to sides) … made a mistake
with the word
Teacher: Oh did you hear what Philip said? … listen Connors … Shh shh
Child: Mrs Rice I have found a big square
Teacher: Listen to what Jonathan said … he said this one’s got longer
corners … does he mean longer corners do you think James?
Child: Umm yes
Teacher: Let’s see here are the corners are they longer? then those
corners … Jonathan those are the corners where my fingers
are touching are they longer?
Child: Nope
Teacher: The corners aren’t longer something else is longer …
Child: … … oh oh I know it is … sides
Teacher: Ahh we got there … good boy well done Jonathan …
Child: Yeah
These three examples illustrate how different children bring different
elements of prior knowledge to support their tackling of similar tasks.
Though in each case there were various degrees of different elements of
prior knowledge, there were individual dominating factors.
To conclude, the structure of prior knowledge based on my research is
eight interlinked elements which impact upon the shape and structure of
memory and is present when children engage in mathematical tasks. We
must consider that my proposed partial model comprising eight elements
is a starting point in the journey to develop a complete and finite
understanding of prior knowledge, as further data may allow for the
emergence of other elements which were not detected in my data.
309
310
7 RESEARCH IMPLICATIONS
7.1 Introduction
Through this research, I have developed a partial structure for prior
knowledge. By using a combination of grounded theory and content
analysis, my research has resulted in a partial model from a range of
contributory elements defining the prior knowledge of children present
when engaging in mathematical tasks. The partial model turns rhetoric
into reality by giving a deeper understanding to a common and, to a great
extent, widely misunderstood term. There are many implications as a
result of this development for practice and pedagogy. This chapter brings
this research a full circle and considers the value and place of this partial
model in practice; and furthermore examines what the next steps should
be as a result of this deeper understanding of prior knowledge. Without
considering the question “so what is the value of this research?” there is
little point in having developed this partial model. Therefore in this
chapter, I summarise the key findings and then look at the impact,
implications and value of this research on schools, teachers, children and
curriculum.
7.2 Key Findings
The partial prior knowledge model is made up of three interlinked
elements –acculturation, context and metacognition – which shape
memory, and further five emerging elements – abstraction,
cognition, individual motivation, perception, and social group;
311
Prior knowledge may contain further elements;
Prior knowledge is not the same as subject knowledge;
Each individual has their own prior knowledge profile consisting of a
combination of a range of contributory elements of prior knowledge
present each time they consider a mathematical task;
The contributory elements of prior knowledge influence individual
memory and shape what children draw upon when approaching
mathematical tasks;
Any child’s prior knowledge profile is constantly changing due to the
continuous experiences that children have;
Prior knowledge is shaped by all of a child’s experiences and not
just by classroom experiences;
Experiences are absorbed very differently by children due to the
individual nature of their prior knowledge.
7.3 Schools
The central mission of schools has always been the activity of developing
both learning and learners. The Cambridge Primary Review proposes
“twelve core educational aims which schools might pursue through the
way they organise themselves, through the curriculum, through
pedagogy, and the relationships they daily seek to foster and enact”
(Alexander, 2010, p. 197).
The proposed aims are organised into three groups – the individual (well-
being; engagement; empowerment; autonomy); self, others and the
wider world (encouraging respect and reciprocity; promoting
312
independence and sustainability; empowering local, national and global
citizenship; celebrating culture and community); and learning, knowing
and doing (exploring, knowing, understanding and making sense;
fostering skill; exciting the imagination; enacting dialogue) (Alexander,
2010, p. 197-199). My prior knowledge model forces us to consider a new
way of developing strategies to support these aims.
If, as has emerged from this research, each individual is dependent on the
shape of their prior knowledge and the elements of this prior knowledge
are made of constituent components, maybe the way to meet the aims set
out above is not to group children in either age or ability, but to consider
their experiential base and gather evidence from the way in which they
approach tasks and the aspects of prior knowledge that are leading
thinking in a particular area.
The partial prior knowledge model offers a way to start understanding the
individual child and support their development in a mass system.
Understanding offered by this structure forces teachers to rethink the way
in which they listen to children and view their actions in the classroom and
consider the complexities of each learner, enabling a process by which
focused informed planning can take place. For example, looking at Figure
6.2, we note that the three children have different aspects of prior
knowledge that they are using to support understanding of a similar task.
Therefore consideration should be given to whether grouping them in
ability groups would be of any benefit or whether considering grouping in
a way that allows them to use areas of prior knowledge to support greater
understanding and engagement would allow them to progress more.
313
Knowing what individuals are using to understand the task helps in
presenting other tasks that will allow relational understanding to take
place. E.g. Jonathan is using his metacognition and perception to
understand the task. Therefore allowing him to consider the task in a
more challenging way which requires him to sort through a range of
outcomes may be the way forward in supporting the way he is using his
metacognition skills to decode the task.
Giving a structure to something that did not previously have any definition
(as seen in Chapter 2) in itself is useful as it empowers teachers to
verbalise and categorise the notion of prior knowledge, giving them a
language for communicating the prior knowledge of children. My partial
model offers schools a starting point for the process of thinking about how
children will be accessing the learning that takes place. Having partly
defined a structure for prior knowledge allows us to know what to look for
in children while they perform a task. Knowing the way in which children
are thinking not just allows us to determine what they are learning, but
also know how they may be using it to make meaning of each task.
Knowing this is powerful as it allows teachers to present material that
maximises the rate at which individuals make sense of the task.
The partial model forces teachers to consider, in terms of mathematical
teaching, a much wider base in children’s understanding. This research
has brought to light the diverse nature of children’s knowledge in
mathematics and how they approach tasks. The discourse for schools is
widened as the realisation that prior knowledge is not based and fixed in
the classroom has been clarified. Also knowing what some of the
314
constituents of prior knowledge are allows us to start thinking of a range
of experiences that we can expose children to in order to develop a bank
of ideas that provide greater tools to access difficult areas of
mathematics.
Understanding gained from this research and the emergence of this partial
model raises questions about the way we organise learning within schools.
There is currently, to some extent, a formulaic approach to mathematics
lessons and the knowledge that must be taught. My partial prior
knowledge model suggests that there must be a shift in thinking away
from a one-size-fits-all solution for organising classrooms to an
individualised personalised model of delivery. We need to move towards
an evidence-based teaching model – one which takes a diagnostic
approach to choice of pedagogy, planning and development of curriculum
and delivery. This is supported by the partial model developed through
this research as the emerging model offers a framework through which
evidence can be gathered.
Using this partial prior knowledge model as a way to gather information
for individualised planning would be ideal. However, as we are constrained
by the current education model, there needs to be a way to implement
this profiling and gathering of evidence. There is little value in planning
within schools without taking into account the new understanding of prior
knowledge offered in this thesis as it offers the ability to start from where
the children already are and opens the door that many learning theories
have relied upon to inculcate learning.
315
7.4 Teachers
The greatest impact of this partial model will be felt by the teachers.
Teachers are the dominant force in the improvement of a child’s ability to
relationally understand mathematics, and therefore form a bridge between
children and knowledge. Understanding of prior knowledge and its impact
upon learning is ingrained in pedagogical dialogue, and with it is the
limited definition – prior knowledge is what children already know about a
subject. This is easy to evaluate as subject knowledge can, to some
extent, be tested. However, my research has demonstrated that children
come to any mathematical task not only with prior subject knowledge, but
also a deeper relationship with the mathematics they are encountering.
My research offers a way to start understanding this deeper relationship.
Being able to listen to children while engaged in mathematical tasks with
some emerging pegs to hang ideas about the child is incredibly useful in
supporting, planning, differentiation, assessment and personalisation of
learning and thus support individual progress. The partial model offers a
way to listen to children and make meaning of how they think who they
are and how to move them forward. It opens the scope to have greater
precise fluid differentiation. Also understanding prior knowledge through
the evidence gathered from this partial model allows teachers to question
children in a more focused manner to direct their daily learning
experiences.
In order to support the development of children, teachers need to know
the real picture of prior knowledge within children. My partial model
supports teachers in gaining a truer picture of individual children.
316
Therefore teachers need to establish a new pedagogy – one which
integrates this emerging structure of prior knowledge within it. This partial
model offers teachers a template with which to begin gathering
information about children, to develop a more accurate detailed picture of
prior knowledge, and to enhance, use and develop to better effect the
critical role that prior knowledge plays in learning. This partial model
takes prior knowledge out of the dark domain of pedagogical rhetoric into
a useable meaningful map to enhance relational understanding and
learning.
Thinking about prior knowledge in terms of my model will require a huge
paradigm shift in the mindset of teachers, from the pre-assumed linear
structure of mathematics teaching to considering mathematics as a more
organic process that needs thoughtful construction of experiences to
support children’s learning. Teachers could argue that this seems like an
onerous task adding to their already heavy workload. However my partial
model offers a starting point to unlocking some of the difficulties that
teachers face on a daily basis. The 2009 House of Commons Public
Accounts Committee Report has highlighted that though, through the
implementation of the National Numeracy Strategy, there have been
improvements in planning and delivery of primary mathematics, there has
been little improvement in attainment (House of Commons Public
Accounts Committee, 2009). This report further highlights a fall in
mathematical knowledge and skills. These outcomes could be due to
teachers’ lack of understanding of the prior knowledge of the children they
are teaching. As already considered in Chapter 2, there is a link between
prior knowledge and learning, and my partial model fills this gap in
317
understanding. Through the use of the partial model, teachers will be able
to gain a better understanding of the prior knowledge of the children they
are teaching and put their strong pedagogical knowledge to better use to
enrich and enhance development of children in mathematics. As most
teachers carry out many activities which will support them in developing
this understanding of the child, it is not a question of implementing a new
process, but using best practice in a more mindful way to understand
children. For each area of my partial model, Table 7.1 and Table 7.2
contain some practical ideas for gaining this information. Table 7.1
focuses on the first three elements of the partial prior knowledge model,
while Table 7.2 focuses on the five other emerging elements of the partial
prior knowledge model. It must be noted that these are only one of a
myriad of possible practical methodologies that can be developed by
teachers to dovetail with existing practice for the understanding of
children’s prior knowledge.
Table 7.1 Possible methods for understanding children’s prior
knowledge
Prior knowledge Possible methods
element
Context Talking and listening to children’s stories; using
circle time to understand and listen to each child
Working with parents to develop a mathematics
diary (similar to reading records)
Homework which ensures using contextual tasks
e.g. counting in pairs for sorting shoes
Supporting mathematics through outdoor learning
Acculturation Looking at records from previous settings
Talking to past class teachers to gain an
understanding of children
A comprehensive progression map within school
which allows all teachers to establish processes
318
(e.g. calculation) which are consistent throughout
the school
Having a clear system of established vocabulary
throughout the school
Metacognition Choices in the methods in what children do while
engaging in mathematical tasks
Taking part in self-assessment e.g. traffic lighting
Choosing own targets
Lessons where children spend time “marking” their
own work and giving explanations of their own
next steps
Use of rich tasks and time for children their
approaches
Table 7.2 Possible methods for understanding children’s prior
knowledge
Prior knowledge Possible methods
element
Individual Look at how they wish to challenge themselves;
motivation consider length of time that children spend on a
task
Choice in how children record their work
Perception Problem solving and how they use their logic
Reading and writing number symbols
Cognition Through observation, noting mathematical tasks
that children achieve with great efficiency
“Fact finding” lessons looking at what children’s
subject knowledge is before starting
Social group Information from parents through pre-existing
structures such as homework diaries
Abstraction Through written work, considering children’s
understanding of mathematical symbols
Using and applying their knowledge in a range of
problem solving contexts
On a practical level, one way in which this partial model can be used is at
the start of every school year for teachers, through observations,
consultation with both parents and past teachers, and setting
319
mathematical tasks, to build a prior knowledge profile constituting
elements of the partial model and to use this in order to plan their lessons
and set targets. Furthermore constantly updating this prior knowledge
profile of children can support development. This deepened understanding
will allow teachers to use their skills to plan in a more informed manner.
For example, when we consider the three children’s prior knowledge
described in Figure 6.2, we see a detailed picture of the way in which
these children think and have developed their understanding and
knowledge of shapes. We are given many clues as to the network of ideas
that the children have used to support this understanding. Listening to the
children, as illustrated by the transcripts, we can see that Richard uses his
ideas of other physical objects to understand different shapes. Therefore
in future planning, teachers should build new learning on these existing
ideas. For example, using everyday objects to consider properties of
shapes. In the case of Jonathan, he clearly understands that he is lacking
some vocabulary to express and explain his mathematical concepts.
Though he has an accurate idea of the shape he is looking at, he needs
some guidance to identify its name. Therefore one idea could be to use
resources which link different shapes to names and sharing these with
Jonathan to help build on his existing knowledge which is a clear
understanding of the properties of a rectangle.
7.5 Children
When considering how this partial structure of prior knowledge affects
children, I need to declare that my discovery of this structure does not
320
require any action from children. They are not being asked to consider
how they behave or act when faced with a mathematical task. They will
continue to behave and respond in the ways they always have. However
the impact of my partial prior knowledge model will be felt by children
through the actions of teachers, and in turn their response to these
actions. Starting to understand the structure of prior knowledge as it has
emerged from children, teachers will be equipped to teach in a more
informed manner, and not feel the restrictions imposed upon them by the
current teaching discourse.
The emphasis in almost all cognitive
developmental theories has been on identifying
sequences of one-to-one correspondences
between ages and ways of thinking or acting,
rather than on specifying how the changes
occur.
(Siegler, 1994, p. 1)
Understanding the categories of prior knowledge which have already
emerged or may emerge through further research allows teachers to
understand how the changes they see in children occur. It is the emerging
categories within prior knowledge and the effect that being in the world
has upon each of these categories that develops and supports learning
within children. Knowing this ensures that teachers should be more
mindful of these emerging categories and should understand how to
support appropriate change which will enhance children’s engagement
with mathematical tasks.
Within the system of schools, currently there is great deal of focus on
mechanisms which are perceived to be good teaching such as targets, AfL,
321
APP, cross-curricular planning through a creative curriculum and topic
plans, but little mindful targeted focus on what individual children need.
When starting this journey, I was prompted by a simple question. What
does it mean when teachers say “I want to get to know my class” or “You
should always go from where children are and then build on this”. In
terms of supporting learning and development, both these desires make
perfect sense. However I feel that till now, I was not given any direction
to achieve this understanding which in turn meant that my support was
very mechanical. Knowing the emerging structure of prior knowledge
allows me to start to develop and use a framework to build this picture of
the child and be more targeted in my support. Also this structure forces
teachers to acknowledge that there is more to an individual than just
subject knowledge which has an impact upon their learning and
understanding of mathematics. It allows teachers to observe children as
people and listen to what they are telling teachers about themselves. It is
only by listening and observing that teachers are able to understand the
uniqueness of each child and thus teach children and not just instruct
them. The partial prior knowledge model evolved in this research supports
this process of listening by having a structure for interpreting children’s
responses and ensuing pedagogical choices. It allows the start of a
structure that supports evidence-based practice.
7.6 Curriculum
The biggest challenge facing the current system of education is how to
personalise learning and ensure individual progress. There is much debate
in current educational corridors about the need to ensure progress for
322
every child and to develop strategies which allow for this progress to be
planned. The dominant theme, for the focus of Ofsted (2012) inspections
in the new framework, is the progress of pupils and how well teachers
manage this progress and development to ensure that “tasks matched to
pupils’ learning needs” (p. 15).
In order to meet this sea of change, the focus is back on the teacher’s
ability to evaluate and understand how each child is learning and the prior
knowledge they bring to each situation. This puts the demand upon
understanding the structure of an individual’s prior knowledge at the
forefront of the dialogues developed around good teaching and learning in
schools.
Though we find ourselves in limbo in terms of the current curriculum for
primary mathematics, it provides me with an ideal opportunity to
construct a utopian ideal of the curriculum I would want to see in our
schools. The great number of changes in the curriculum has had some
effect upon how teaching is implemented within schools. I must be clear
in my debate of the practical possibilities for implementing a creative
curriculum. However if I go back to the start of this thesis and consider
the central premise that good teaching is based upon understanding
children’s prior knowledge, and that effective learning is based upon links
made to prior knowledge, then a curriculum must be designed that allows
for this process of developing and considering for each individual child’s
prior knowledge using the partial structure proposed and then developing
learning needs based on this. This new dialogue in education fills me with
hope as there is an appetite for moving away from the mechanical process
323
of consuming knowledge to a more considered organic process – one
which on the face of it has a prominent place for prior knowledge.
Having uncovered some elements that each child brings to every task and
knowing that it is these elements that may have an effect upon the way in
which learning can be organised, the greatest change that should take
place is to change from a linear curriculum to a more organic curriculum.
The curriculum needs to allow for these non-linear ideas and knowledge to
form the central premise of curriculum development.
Throughout conducting this research, I have been privileged in observing
children and learning from them what they have to tell us about what they
already know and how this could support their future learning. The partial
structure of prior knowledge developed through this research allows
teachers to structure their thoughts, to observe and listen mindfully to
children, to understand each child individually and thus to teach the child
and not children, and to have an evidence-based approach to teaching.
7.7 Moving Forward
This journey has been long and one which has taken many interesting
turns. However throughout the process, I have learnt a lot about the way
in which we should teach and the power of children to share openly their
thoughts. Children bring such a cornucopia of dimensions to their
learning, and it is the teachers’ role to listen, observe, understand,
support and guide children through the many mazes they will find in their
learning journey, so that they may eventually be able to work out their
own struggle and path to follow. For me, all theory aside, empirically
324
there is no other way forward but to continue to search for further
elements of prior knowledge and to start using this framework for my
teaching and understanding of children’s mathematics.
It will be naive of me to think that this was the end of the road. There is
much more to be done and much to learn from children. I have listed
below three areas that I think need further exploration, now that a partial
structure for prior knowledge has been developed.
Application – ask teachers to use this structure in order to see how
it works in practice and develop a toolkit;
Effects – what are the factors that affect prior knowledge?
Extension – conduct research to uncover further possible categories
of prior knowledge.
7.8 Summing Up
The goal and intention of this research process was simple – to
understand why children in schools had such varying ability in
mathematics. The journey to understand this has been fascinating and led
me to the key for learning – prior knowledge. Exploring and unpicking this
established concept has enabled me to gain an all-round view of education
and the process of learning. The result of all this exploration is an
emerging partial model for prior knowledge – a structure which exists in
us all – which has been established from a range of contributory
elements. My contribution to the understanding and teaching of children is
325
this partial structure of prior knowledge which has not been discovered
before.
326
REFERENCES
Ajello, A. M., & Belardi, C. (2002). Acquiring abilities within informal
contexts: recognition and accreditation. Paper presented at the
Learning Communities and Assessment Cultures Conference, EARLI
Special Interest Group on Assessment and Evaluation, University of
Northumbria, 28-30 August 2002. Retrieved from
http://www.leeds.ac.uk/educol/documents/00002241.htm.
Alexander, P. A., Kulikowich, J. A., & Jetton, T. L. (1994). The role of
subject-matter knowledge and interest in the processing of linear
and non-linear texts. Review of Educational Research, 64 (2), 201-
252.
Alexander, P. A., Pate, E. P., Kulikowich, J. M., Farrell, D. M., & Wright, N.
L. (1989). Domain-specific and strategic knowledge: effects of
training on students of differing ages or competence levels.
Learning and Individual Differences, 1 (3), 283-325.
Alexander, R. (Ed.). (2010). Children, their world, their education: final
report and recommendations of the Cambridge Primary Review.
London, UK: Routledge.
Anderson, J. R. (1995). Cognitive psychology and its implications (4th
ed.). New York, NY: W. H. Freeman.
Anderson, L. W., & Burns, R. B. (1989). Research in classrooms: the
study of teachers, teaching and instruction. Oxford, UK: Pergamon.
Apple, M. W. (1979). Ideology and curriculum. London, UK: Routledge.
Ashcraft, M. H. (1982). The development of mental arithmetic: a
chronometric approach. Development Review, 2 (3), 213-236.
Asimov, I. (1991). Foreword. In C. B. Boyer, & U. C. Merzbach, A history
of mathematics (2nd ed.) (pp. vii-viii). New York, NY: John Wiley.
Askew, M., Millett, A., Brown, M., Rhodes, V., & Bibby, T. (2001).
Entitlement to attainment: tensions in the National Numeracy
Strategy. The Curriculum Journal, 12 (1), 5-28.
Atkinson, S. (1992). Mathematics with reason: the emergent approach to
primary maths. London, UK: Hodder & Stoughton.
Ausubel, D. P., Novak, J. D., & Hanesian, H. (1978). Educational
psychology: a cognitive view (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston.
Baker, C., Wuest, J., & Stern, P. N. (1992). Method slurring: the grounded
theory/phenomenology example. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 17
(11), 1355-1360.
327
Baldwin, B. T., & Stecher, L. I. (1925). The psychology of the preschool
child. New York, NY: D. Appleton.
Baroody, A. J. (1987). Children's mathematical thinking: a developmental
framework for pre-school, primary and special education teachers.
New York, NY: Teachers’ College Press.
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: a study in experimental and social
psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Becker, H. S. (1990). Generalizing from case studies. In E. W. Eisner, & A.
Peshkin (Eds.), Qualitative research in education: the continuing
debate (pp. 233-242). New York, NY: Teachers’ College Press.
Berry, J. W. (2005). Acculturation: living successfully in two cultures.
International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 29 (6), 697-712.
Bishop, A. (2002). Mathematical acculturation, cultural conflicts, and
transition. In G. De Abreu, A. J. Bishop, & N. C. Presmeg (Eds.),
Transitions between contexts of mathematical practices (pp. 193-
212). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Bjorklund, D. F. (1985). The role of conceptual knowledge in the
development of organization in children's memory. In C. J.
Brainerd, & M. Pressley (Eds.), Basic processes in memory
development: progress in cognitive development research (pp. 103-
142). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
Blanck, G. (1996). Vygotsky: the man and his cause. In L. C. Moll (Ed.),
Vygotsky and education: instructional implications and applications
of sociohistorical psychology (pp. 31-58). Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Bloom, B. S. (1976). Human characteristics and school learning. New
York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Bogdan, R. C., & Biklen, S. K. (2007). Qualitative research for education:
an introduction to theory and methods (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn
& Bacon.
Borasi, R. (1986). On the nature of problems. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 17 (2), 125-141.
Borko, H., & Livingston, C. (1989). Cognition and improvisation:
differences in mathematics instruction by expert and novice
teachers. American Educational Research Journal, 26 (4), 473-498.
Borthwick, A. (2011). Children’s perceptions of, and attitudes towards,
their mathematics lessons. In C. Smith (Ed.), Proceedings of the
British Society for Research into Learning Mathematics (pp. 37-42),
31 (1), London, UK, 11 March 2011.
328
Bower, T. G. R. (1982). Development in infancy (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
W. H. Freeman.
Brightman, H. J. (1982). Teaching statistics through advance organizers.
The American Statistician, 36 (3), 217.
British Educational Research Association. (2004). Revised ethical
guidelines for educational research. London, UK: British Educational
Research Association.
Briton, D., Gereluk, W., & Spencer, B. (1998). Prior learning assessment
and recognition: issues for adult educators. In Proceedings of the
17th Annual Conference of the Canadian Association for the Study of
Adult Education (pp. 24-28), University of Ottawa, 29-31 May 1998.
Bugelski, B. R. (1962). Presentation time, total time, and mediation in
paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63
(4), 409-412.
Burrell, G., & Morgan, G. (1979). Sociological paradigms and
organisational analysis: elements of the sociology of corporate life.
London, UK: Heinemann.
Burton, M. (2009). Exploring the changing perception of mathematics
among elementary teacher candidates through drawings. In S. L.
Swars, D. W. Stinson, & S. Lemons-Smith (Eds.), Proceedings of
the 31st Annual Meeting of the North American Chapter of the
International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education
(pp. 363-370), Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA, 23-26
September 2009.
Cabassa, L. J. (2003). Measuring acculturation: where we are and where
we need to go. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 25 (2),
127-146.
Cameron, L. (2003). Metaphor in educational discourse. London, UK:
Continuum.
Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1963). Experimental and quasi-
experimental designs for research on teaching. In N. L. Gage (Ed.),
Handbook of research on teaching (pp. 171-246). Chicago, IL: Rand
McNally.
Carr, M., Alexander, J., & Folds-Bennett, T. (1994). Metacognition and
mathematics strategy use. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8 (6),
583-595.
Carspecken, P. F. (1996). Critical ethnography in educational research: a
theoretical and practical guide. London, UK: Routledge.
Charmaz, K. (2007). Constructing grounded theory: a practical guide
through qualitative analysis. London, UK: Sage Publications.
329
Chi, M. T. H., & Ceci, S. (1987). Content knowledge: its role,
representation, and restructuring in memory development. In H. W.
Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 20)
(pp. 91-142). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. J. (Eds.). (1988). The nature of
expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Clemson, D., & Clemson, W. (1994). Mathematics in the early years.
London, UK: Routledge.
Cobb, P., Perlwitz, M., & Underwood-Gregg, D. (1998). Individual
construction, mathematical acculturation, and the classroom
community. In M. Larochelle, N. Bednarz, & J. Garrison (Eds.),
Constructivism and education (pp. 63-80). Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
Cockcroft, W. H. (1982). The Cockcroft Report: Mathematics counts:
report of the Committee of Inquiry into the teaching of mathematics
in schools. London, UK: HMSO.
Cohen, L., Manion, L., & Morrison, K. (2001). Research methods in
education (5th ed.). London, UK: Routledge.
Cooley, C. H. (1909). Social organization. New York, NY: Scribner’s.
Coon, D. (1989). Introduction to psychology: exploration and application
(5th ed.). St. Paul, MN: West Publishing Company.
Cooper, B., & Dunne, M. (1988). Anyone for tennis? social class
differences in children’s responses to National Curriculum
mathematical testing. The Sociological Review, 46 (1), 115-148.
Coulby, D. (2000). Beyond the National Curriculum: curricular centralism
and cultural diversity in Europe and the USA. London, UK:
RoutledgeFalmer.
Cronbach, L. J. (1975). Beyond the two disciplines of scientific
psychology. American Psychologist, 30 (2), 116-127.
Denzin, N. K. (1989). The research act: a theoretical introduction to
sociological methods (3rd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Department for Education. (1995). Mathematics in the National
Curriculum. London, UK: HMSO.
Department for Education. (2008). DCSF: School Workforce in England
(including Local Authority level figures), January 2008 (Revised).
London, UK: Department for Education. Retrieved from
http://www.education.gov.uk/rsgateway/DB/SFR/s000813/index.sh
tml.
330
Department for Education and Employment. (1999). The National
Numeracy Strategy: framework for teaching mathematics from
Reception to Year 6. Sudbury, UK: DfEE Publications.
Department for Education and Skills. (2006). Primary framework for
literacy and mathematics. London, UK: DfES Publications.
Department of Education and Science. (1985). Better schools. London,
UK: HMSO.
Dey, I. (1993). Qualitative data analysis: a user-friendly guide for social
scientists. London, UK: Routledge.
Dickson, L., Brown, M., & Gibson, O. (1984). Children learning
mathematics: a teacher's guide to recent research. London, UK:
Cassell.
Dienes, Z. (1971). Building up mathematics (4th ed.). London, UK:
Hutchinson Educational.
Dochy, F. J. R. C. (Ed.). (1992). Assessment of prior knowledge as a
determinant for future learning: the use of prior knowledge state
tests and knowledge profiles. London, UK: Jessica Kingsley.
Dochy, F., Segers, M., & Buehl, M. M. (1999). The relation between
assessment practices and outcomes of studies: the case of research
on prior knowledge. Review of Educational Research, 69 (2), 145-
186.
Donaldson, M. (1989). Children's minds (16th ed). London, UK: Fortuna.
Draucker, C. B., Martsolf, D. S., Ross, R., & Rusk, T. B. (2007).
Theoretical sampling and category development in grounded
theory. Qualitative Health Research, 17 (8), 1137-1148.
Dudai, Y. (2007). Memory: it’s all about representations. In H. L.
Roediger, Y. Dudai, & S. M. Fitzpatrick (Eds.), Science of memory:
concepts (pp. 13-16). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving (L. S. Lees, Trans.). Psychological
Monographs, 58 (270), 1-113.
Education Reform Act 1988 (c.40). London: HMSO.
Edwards, A. (2002). Responsible research: ways of being a researcher.
British Educational Research Journal, 28 (2), 157-168.
Edwards, A. D., & Westgate, D. P. G. (1994). Investigating classroom talk
(2nd ed.). London, UK: The Falmer Press.
Ellwood, C. A. (1919). Sociology and modern social problems. New York,
NY: American Book Co.
331
Ely, M., Anzul, M., Friedman, T., Garner, D., & Steinmetz, A. M. (1991).
Doing qualitative research: circles within circles. London, UK:
RoutledgeFalmer.
Erickson, F., & Schultz, J. (1981). When is a context? some issues in the
analysis of social competence. In J. Green, & C. Wallat (Eds.),
Ethnography and language in educational settings (pp. 147-160).
Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Ernest, P. (1999). Forms of knowledge in mathematics and mathematics
education: philosophical and rhetorical perspectives. Educational
Studies in Mathematics, 38 (1), 67-83.
Evans, K. (2002). The challenges of 'making learning visible': problems
and issues in recognising tacit forms of key competences. In K.
Evans, P. Hodkinson, & L. Unwin (Eds.), Working to learn:
transforming learning in the workplace (pp. 79-94). London, UK:
Kogan Page.
Ferrari, P. L. (2003). Abstraction in mathematics. Philosophical
Transactions: Biological Sciences, 358 (1435), 1225-1230.
Finlay, L. (2008). Introducing phenomenological research. Retrieved from
http://www.lindafinlay.co.uk/An%20introduction%20to%20phenom
enology%202008.doc.
Flanagan, C. (1996). Applying psychology to early child development.
London, UK: Hodder & Stoughton.
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: a new area
of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34 (10),
906-911.
Gardiner, T. (2000). Reference levels in school mathematics education in
Europe: national presentation: England. Helsinki: Committee on
Mathematics Education, European Mathematical Society.
Garofalo, J., & Lester, F. K. J. (1985). Metacognition, cognitive
monitoring, and mathematical performance. Journal of Research in
Mathematics Education, 16 (3), 163-176.
Gelman, R. (1980). What young children know about numbers.
Educational Psychologist, 15 (1), 54-68.
Gibson, E. J. (1987). Introductory essay: what does infant perception tell
us about theories of perception? Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13 (4), 513-523.
Gibson, J. J., & Gibson, E. J. (1955). Perceptual learning: differentiation or
enrichment? Psychological Review, 62 (1), 32-41.
332
Gillard, D. (2004). The Plowden Report. In The encyclopaedia of informal
education. London, UK: infed. Retrieved from
http://www.infed.org/schooling/plowden_report.htm.
Ginsburg, H. P. (1989). Children's arithmetic: how they learn it and how
you teach it (2nd ed.). Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
Glaser, B. G. (2001). The grounded theory perspective: conceptualization
contrasted with description. Mill Valley, CA: Sociology Press.
Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory:
strategies for qualitative research. New York, NY: Aldine.
Glaser, R. (1984). Education and thinking: the role of knowledge.
American Psychologist, 39 (2), 93-104.
Glaser, R., & De Corte, E. (1992). Preface. In F. J. R. C. Dochy (Ed.),
Assessment of prior knowledge as a determinant for future
learning: the use of prior knowledge state tests and knowledge
profiles (pp. 1-2). London, UK: Jessica Kingsley.
Glaser, R., Lesgold, A., & Lajoie, S. (1987). Toward a cognitive theory for
the measurement of achievement. In R. R. Ronning, J. A. Glover, J.
C. Conoley, & J. C. Witt (Eds.), The influence of cognitive
psychology on testing and measurement (pp. 41-85). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Gold, R. L. (1958). Roles in sociological field observations. Social Forces,
36, 217-223.
Gregory, R. L. (1978). Eye and brain: the psychology of seeing (3rd ed.).
New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Groen, G., & Resnick, L. B. (1977). Can preschool children invent addition
algorithms? Journal of Educational Psychology, 69 (6), 645-652.
Hadow, H. (1931). The Hadow Report: the primary school: a report of the
Consultative Committee. London, UK: HMSO.
Haenggi, D., & Perfetti, C. A. (1992). Individual differences in
reprocessing of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84 (2),
182-192.
Hammersley, M., & Atkinson, P. (2007). Ethnography: principles in
practice (3rd ed.). London, UK: Taylor & Francis.
Hannula, M. S. (2006). Motivation in mathematics: goals reflected in
emotion. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 63 (2), 165-178.
333
Harris, J. (2000). Re-visioning the boundaries of learning theory in the
assessment of prior experiential learning (APEL). Paper presented
at 30th Annual Standing Conference on University Teaching and
Research in the Education of Adults (SCUTREA) Conference,
University of Nottingham, 3-5 July 2000. Retrieved from
http://www.leeds.ac.uk/educol/documents/00001448.htm.
Hatch, J. A. (2002). Doing qualitative research in education settings.
Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
Hayes, J. R. (1985). Three problems in teaching general skills. In S. F.
Chipman, J. W. Segal, & R. Glaser (Eds.), Thinking and learning
skills: research and open questions (Vol. 2) (pp. 391-406).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Haylock, D., & Cockburn, A. (1989). Understanding early years
mathematics. London, UK: Paul Chapman.
Hazel, E., Prosser, M., & Trigwell, K. (2002). Variation in learning
orchestration in university biology courses. International Journal of
Science Education, 24 (7), 737-751.
Hitchcock, G., & Hughes, D. (1995). Research and the teacher: a
qualitative introduction to school-based research (2nd ed.). London,
UK: Routledge.
Hoepfl, M. C. (1997). Choosing qualitative research: a primer for
technology education researchers. Journal of Technology Education,
9 (1), 47-63.
House of Commons Public Accounts Committee. (2009). Mathematics
performance in primary schools: getting the best results. Twenty-
third report of session 2008-09. HC 44. London, UK: TSO.
Hsieh, H. F., & Shannon, S. E. (2005). Three approaches to qualitative
content analysis. Qualitative Health Research, 15 (9), 1277-1288.
Hughes, M. (1986). Children and number: difficulties in learning
mathematics. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Hume, D. (2010). A treatise on human nature (Vol. 1). Charleston, SC:
Forgotten Books. (Original work published in 1739)
Jaworski, B. (1994). Investigating mathematics teaching: a constructivist
enquiry. London, UK: Falmer Press.
Jones, A., Todorova, N., & Vargo, J. (2000). Improving teaching
effectiveness understanding and leveraging prior knowledge for
student learning. In Proceedings of the 15th Annual Conference of
the International Academy for Information Management (pp. 205-
209), Brisbane, Australia, 6-10 December 2000.
334
Kant, I. (2010). The critique of pure reason (J. M. D. Meiklejohn, Trans.).
Charleston, SC: Forgotten Books. (Original work published in 1781)
Kaplan, A. S., & Murphy, G. L. (2000). Category learning with minimal
prior knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, 26 (4), 829-846.
Kincheloe, J. L. (2003). Teachers as researchers: qualitative inquiry as a
path to empowerment (2nd ed.). London, UK: RoutledgeFalmer.
Kirk, J., & Miller, M. L. (1986). Reliability and validity in qualitative
research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Kitcher, P. (1984). The nature of mathematical knowledge. New York, NY:
Oxford University Press.
Krathwohl, D. R. (1993). Methods of educational and social science
research: an integrated approach. New York, NY: Longman.
Kuhn, D., & Dean, D. (2004). A bridge between cognitive psychology and
educational practice. Theory into Practice, 43 (4), 268-273.
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (2003). Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: The
University of Chicago Press.
Laurillard, D. (1993). Rethinking university teaching: a framework for the
effective use of educational technology. London, UK: Routledge.
Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: mind, mathematics, and culture in
everyday life. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Lave, J., Murtaugh, M., & de la Rocha, O. (1984). The dialectic of
arithmetic in grocery shopping. In B. Rogoff, & J. Lave (Eds.),
Everyday cognition: its development in social context (pp. 67-94).
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Lavine, T. Z. (1984). From Socrates to Sartre: the philosophic quest. New
York, NY: Bantam Books.
LeCompte, M. D., & Preissle, J. (1993). Ethnography and qualitative
design in educational research (2nd ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic
Press.
Levitas, M. (1974). Marxist perspectives in the sociology of education.
London, UK: Routledge.
Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. E. (1985a). Naturalistic inquiry. Mountain Oaks,
CA: Sage Publications.
Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. E. (1985b). Research, evaluation, and policy
analysis: heuristics for disciplined inquiry. Review of Policy
Research, 5 (3), 546-565.
335
Little, D. (2008). What is hermeneutic explanation? Dearborn, MI:
University of Michigan. Retrieved from http://www-
personal.umd.umich.edu/~delittle/Encyclopedia%20entries/hermen
eutic%20explanation.htm.
Lovell, K. (1973). Educational psychology and children (11th ed.). London,
UK: University of London Press.
Malkevitch, J. (1997). Discrete mathematics and public perceptions of
mathematics. In J. G. Rosenstein, D. S. Franzblau, & F. S. Roberts
(Eds.), Discrete mathematics in the schools (pp. 89-97).
Providence, RI: American Mathematical Society.
Marshall, S. P. (1993). Assessment of rational number understanding: a
schema-based approach. In T. P. Carpenter, E. Fennema, & T. A.
Romberg (Eds.), Rational numbers: an integration of research (pp.
261-288). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Merriam, S. B. (1995). What can you tell from an n of 1?: issues of
validity and reliability in qualitative research. PAACE Journal of
Lifelong Learning, 4, 51-60.
Merttens, R., & Head, J. (2000). Series editors’ preface. In D. Coulby,
Beyond the National Curriculum: curricular centralism and cultural
diversity in Europe and the USA (p. viii-x). London, UK:
RoutledgeFalmer.
Meyer, H. (2004). Novice and expert teachers' conceptions of learners'
prior knowledge. Science Education, 88 (6), 970-983.
Middleton, J. A., & Spanias, P. A. (1999). Motivation for achievement in
mathematics: findings, generalizations, and criticisms of the
research. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30 (1),
65-88.
Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative data analysis (2nd
ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Mitchelmore, M., & White, P. (2004). Abstraction in mathematics and
mathematics learning. In M. J. Hoines, & A. B. Fuglestad (Eds.),
Proceedings of the 28th Conference of the International Group for
the Psychology of Mathematics Educations (Vol. 3) (pp. 329-336).
Bergen, Norway, 14-18 July 2004. Cape Town, South Africa:
International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education.
Mitchelmore, M., & White, P. (2007). Abstraction in mathematics learning.
Mathematics Education Research Journal, 19 (2), 1-9.
Moon, B. (2001). A guide to the National Curriculum (4th ed.). Oxford,
UK: Oxford University Press.
Mouly, G. J. (1978). Educational research: the art and science of
investigation. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
336
Nickson, M. (1994). The culture of the mathematics classroom: an
unknown quantity? In S Lerman (Ed.). Cultural perspectives on the
mathematics classroom (pp. 7-35). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer
Academic Publishers.
Novak, J. D., & Canas, A. J. (2007). Theoretical origins of concept maps,
how to construct them, and uses in education. Reflecting Education,
3 (1), 29-42.
O'Donnell, A. M., Dansereau, D. F., & Hall, R. H. (2002). Knowledge maps
as scaffolds for cognitive processing. Educational Psychology
Review, 14 (1), 71-86.
Ofsted. (2012). The framework for school inspection. Manchester, UK:
Ofsted.
Onions, P. E. W. (2006). Grounded theory applications in reviewing
knowledge management literature. Paper presented at Leeds
Metropolitan University Innovation North Research Conference,
Leeds Metropolitan University, 24 May 2006. Retrieved from
http://www.patrickonions.org/docs/academic/2006%20Grounded%
20theory%20from%20literature%20review.pdf.
Papaleontiou-Louca, E. (2003). The concept and instruction of
metacognition. Teacher Development, 7 (1), 9-30.
Park, R. E., & Burgess, E. W. (1921). Introduction to the science of
sociology. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Parkerson, J. A., Lomax, R. G., Schiller, D. P., & Walberg, H. J. (1984).
Exploring causal models of educational achievement. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 76 (4), 638-646.
Peterson, P. L., Marx, R. W., & Clark, C. M. (1978). Teacher planning,
teacher behavior, and student achievement. American Educational
Research Journal, 15 (3), 417-432.
Piaget, J. (1954). The constructing of reality in the child (M. Cook,
Trans.). New York, NY: Basic Books.
Plowden, J. P. (1967). The Plowden Report: children and their primary
schools: a report of the Central Advisory Council for Education
(England). London, UK: HMSO.
Pressley, M., & McCormick, C. B. (1995). Advanced educational
psychology for educators, researchers and policymakers. New York,
NY: HarperCollins College Publishers.
Rand, A. (1965). Who is the final authority in ethics? The Objectivist
Newsletter, 4 (2), 7.
Rand, A. (1979). Introduction to objectivist epistemology. New York, NY:
Mentor.
337
Resnick, L. B. (1981). Instructional psychology. Annual Review of
Psychology, 32, 659-704.
Resnick, L. B., & Ford, W. W. (1981). The psychology of mathematics for
instruction. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Reynolds, D. (1998a). Implementation of the National Numeracy
Strategy: the final report of the Numeracy Task Force. London, UK:
Department for Education and Employment (DfEE).
Reynolds, D. (1998b). Numeracy matters: the preliminary report of the
Numeracy Task Force. London, UK: Department for Education and
Employment (DfEE).
Roschelle, J. (1995). Learning in interactive environments: prior
knowledge and new experience. In J. H. Falk, & L. D. Dierking
(Eds.), Public institutions for personal learning: establishing a
research agenda (pp. 37-51). Washington, DC: American
Association of Museums.
Rose, J. (2009). Independent review of the primary curriculum: final
report. Nottingham, UK: DCSF Publications.
Rosengren, K. E. (1981). Advances in Scandinavian content analysis: an
introduction. In K. E. Rosengren (Ed.), Advances in content analysis
(pp. 9-19). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.
Rumbold, A. (1990). The Rumbold Report: starting with quality: the
report of the Committee of Inquiry into the quality of the
educational experience offered to 3 and 4 year olds. London, UK:
HMSO.
Ryan, A. B. (2006). Methodology: analysing qualitative data and writing
up your findings. In M. Antonesa, H. Fallon, A. B. Ryan, T. Walsh, &
L. Borys (Eds.), Researching and writing your thesis: a guide for
postgraduate students (pp. 92-108). Maynooth, Ireland: MACE
Publications.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations:
classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary Educational
Psychology, 25 (1), 54-67.
Schmidt, H. G., De Volder, M. L., De Grave, W. S., Moust, J. H. C., &
Patel, V. L. (1989). Explanatory models in the processing of science
text: the role of prior knowledge activation through small-group
discussion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81 (4), 610-619.
Schneider, W., Körkel, J., & Weinert, F. E. (1989). Domain-specific
knowledge and memory performance: a comparison of high- and
low-aptitude children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81 (3),
306-312.
338
Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (1989). Memory development between 2
and 20. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
Schofield, J. W. (1993). Increasing the generalizability of qualitative
research. In M. Hammersley (Ed.), Social research: philosophy,
politics and practice (pp. 171-204). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Publications.
Schoenfeld, A. H. (1992). Learning to think mathematically: problem
solving, metacognition, and sense-making in mathematics. In D. A.
Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and
learning (pp. 334-370). New York, NY: MacMillan.
Schraw, G. (1998). Promoting general metacognitive awareness.
Instructional Science, 26 (1-2), 113-125.
Schwandt, T. J. (2001). Dictionary of qualitative inquiry (2nd ed.).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Shrager, L., & Mayer, R. E. (1989). Note-taking fosters generative
learning strategies in novices. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81
(2), 263-264.
Siegler, R. S. (1994). Cognitive variability: a key to understanding
cognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
3 (1), 1-5.
Skemp, R. R. (1987). The psychology of learning mathematics. Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Smith, J. K., & Hodkinson, P. (2002). Fussing about the nature of
educational research: the neo-realists versus the relativists. British
Educational Research Journal, 28 (2), 291-296.
Sotto, E. (1994). When teaching becomes learning: a theory and practice
of teaching. London, UK: Cassell.
Spencer, B. (2005). Defining prior learning assessment and recognition.
In L. M. English (Ed.), International encyclopaedia of adult
education (pp. 508-512). New York, NY: Palgrave MacMillan.
Spindler, G., & Spindler, L. (1992). Cultural process and ethnography: an
anthropological perspective. In M. D. LeCompte, W. L. Millroy, & J.
Preissle (Eds.), The handbook of qualitative research in education
(pp. 53-92). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Spradley, J. P. (1979). The ethnographic interview. New York, NY: Holt,
Rinehart & Winston.
Stake, R. E. (1995). The art of case study research. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage Publications.
339
Starkey, P., & Gelman, R. (1982). The development of addition and
subtraction abilities prior to formal schooling in arithmetic. In T. P.
Carpenter, J. M. Moser, & T. A. Romberg (Eds.), Addition and
subtraction: a cognitive perspective (pp. 99-116). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Starr-Glass, D. (2002). Metaphor and totem: exploring and evaluating
prior experiential learning. Assessment & Evaluation in Higher
Education, 27 (3), 221-231.
Steele, J. R., & Ambady, N. (2006). “Math is hard!” The effect of gender
priming on women’s attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 42 (4), 428-436.
Strauss, A. L., & Corbin, J. M. (1990). Basics of qualitative research:
techniques and procedures for developing grounded theory.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Sturman, L., Ruddock, G., Burge, B., Styles, B., Lin, Y., & Vappula, H.
(2008). England’s achievement in TIMSS 2007: national report for
England. Slough, UK: National Foundation for Educational Research.
Sullivan, P., Zeverbergen, R., & Mousley, J. (2003). The contexts of
mathematics tasks and the context of the classroom: are we
including all students? Mathematics Education Research Journal, 15
(2), 107-121.
Sumner, W. G. (1907). Folkways: a study of the sociological importance
of usages, manners, customs, mores, and morals. Boston, MA:
Ginn.
Sutton, J. (2010). Memory. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford
Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Summer 2010 edition). Stanford, CA:
The Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University. Retrieved from
http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2010/entries/memory/.
Taber, K. S. (2001). The mismatch between assumed prior knowledge and
the learner's conceptions: a typology of learning impediments.
Educational Studies, 27 (2), 159-171.
Tait-McCutcheon, S. L. (2008). Self-efficacy in mathematics: affective,
cognitive, and conative domains of functioning. In M. Goos, R.
Brown, & K. Makar (Eds.), Navigating currents and charting
directions (Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the
Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia) (pp. 507-
513). The University of Queensland, Brisbane, 28 June-1 July 2008.
Brisbane: MERGA.
Tesch, R. (1990). Qualitative research: analysis types and software tools.
London, UK: RoutledgeFalmer.
340
Thomas, D. R. (2006). A general inductive approach for analyzing
qualitative evaluation data. American Journal of Evaluation, 27 (2),
237-246.
Thomas, W. I. (1923). The unadjusted girl: with cases and standpoint for
behaviour analysis. Boston, MA: Little, Brown, and Company.
Thompson, I. (1995). "Pre-number activities" and the early years number
curriculum. Mathematics in School, 24 (1), 37-39.
Tizard, B., & Hughes, M. (1984). Young children learning. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Tobias, S. (1994). Interest, prior knowledge and learning. Journal of
Education Psychology, 87 (3), 399-405.
Toffler, A. (1970). Future shock. New York, NY: Random House.
Turner, J. C. (2010). Towards a cognitive redefinition of the social group.
In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Social identity and intergroup relations (pp. 15-
40). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Turner, J. H., Beeghley, L., & Powers, C. H. (2012). The emergence of
sociological theory (7th ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Publications.
UCAS. (2011). Applications (choices), acceptances and ratios by subject
group 2011. Cheltenham, UK: UCAS. Retrieved from
http://www.ucas.com/about_us/stat_services/stats_online/data_ta
bles/subject/2011.
UNESCO. (2002). Cultural diversity, common heritage, plural identities.
Paris: UNESCO.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: the development of higher
psychological processes. M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E.
Souberman (Eds.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wakeley, R. A. (2002). Early mathematical development in low birth-
weight children. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of
California, Berkeley, CA.
Walker, C. H. (1987). Relative importance of domain knowledge and
overall aptitude on acquisition of domain-related information.
Cognition and Instruction, 4 (1), 25-42.
Walkerdine, V. (1990). Difference, cognition and mathematics education.
For the Learning of Mathematics, 10 (3), 51-56.
Ward, S. (2004). Government policy on education in England. In S. Ward
(Ed.), Education studies: a student's guide (pp. 81-91). London,
UK: RoutledgeFalmer.
341
Weber, R. P. (1990). Basic content analysis (2nd ed.). Newbury Park, CA:
Sage Publications.
Weinert, F. (1989). The impact of schooling on cognitive development:
One hypothetical assumption, some empirical results, and many
theoretical implications. EARLI News, (8), 3-7.
West, L. H. T., & Fensham, P. J. (1974). Prior knowledge and the learning
of science. Studies in Science Education, 1 (1), 61-81.
Wiersma, W., & Jurs, S. G. (2005). Research methods in education: an
introduction (8th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Williams, P. (2008). Independent review of mathematics teaching in early
years settings and primary schools. Nottingham, UK: DCSF
Publications.
Windschitl, M. (2002). Framing constructivism in practice as the
negotiation of dilemmas: an analysis of the conceptual,
pedagogical, cultural, and political challenges facing teachers.
Review of Educational Research, 72 (2), 131-175.
Winter, G. (2000). A comparative discussion of the notion of 'validity' in
qualitative and quantitative research. The Qualitative Report, 4 (3 &
4). Retrieved from http://www.nova.edu/ssss/QR/QR4-
3/winter.html.
Woods, P. (1979). The divided school. London, UK: Routledge & Kegan
Paul.
Wolcott, H. F. (1974). The teacher as an enemy. In G. D. Spindler (Ed.).
Education and cultural process: towards anthropology of education
(pp. 136-150). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Yinger, R. J. (1978). A study of teacher planning: description and a model
of preactive decision making. (Research Series No. 18). East
Lansing, MI: The Institute for Research on Teaching, Michigan State
University.
Yinger, R. J. (1980). A study of teacher planning. The Elementary School
Journal, 80 (3), 107-127.
Yuret, D. (1995). A brief review of memory research in cognitive
neuroscience. (Working Paper No. 343). Cambridge, MA: Artificial
Intelligence Laboratory, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
Zeitoun, H. H. (1989). The relationship between abstract concept
achievement and prior knowledge, formal reasoning ability and
gender. International Journal of Science Education, 11 (2), 227-
234.
342
APPENDICES
Appendix A – Data Collection Schools’ Ofsted
Reports
This appendix contains Information about the School and/or Description of
the School and Main Findings and/or Overall Effectiveness of the School
sections from the recent Ofsted inspection reports for each of the five
schools used in the data collection.
Hatton First School
This information comes from an Ofsted inspection carried out in 2009.
Information about the School
This small school stands in a rural location some distance from
Hatton village. About a third of the pupils come from the village,
others travel in from a wide area. The great majority are of White
British backgrounds, and the very small number from other ethnic
backgrounds all speak English as their first language. Few pupils
are entitled to free school meals. The proportion of pupils with
special educational needs and/or disabilities is low. The Early Years
Foundation Stage comprises of a Reception class.
Several teaching staff have been appointed since the last
inspection, including the head teacher who has been in post for two
years.
The school holds an Activemark award.
Main findings
This is a good school that equips pupils with a love of learning,
happy memories and firm foundations for their future education and
life beyond. Behaviour and attendance are excellent and pupils are
very well supported by first-rate links between the school and their
parents and carers. The school is firmly at the centre of the village
community and has strong links with the church, craft centre and
Hatton Hall. Parents and carers value the school’s warm family
ethos where their children feel very safe and secure, and make
good progress in their learning and personal development.
343
The Early Years Foundation Stage gives children a good start,
enhanced by outdoor activities, especially when they learn in a local
woodland area known as the ‘Forest School’. Pupils achieve well
throughout the school and attain above average standards by the
time they leave. Good teaching and rigorous monitoring ensure that
no-one falls behind. Boys and girls progress equally well but school
assessment data indicate some differences; boys do not reach
similar standards to girls in writing, and girls do not match the boys
in mathematics.
Good teaching and well planned lessons challenge all groups of
pupils. Pupils are confident learners, know they are expected to
work hard and say lessons are interesting. They know what they
will learn in each lesson but are not always aware of their next
steps in learning, for example what they are aiming for in
mathematics or writing. Well managed provision, and skilled
support from the teaching assistants, enables pupils with special
educational needs and/or disabilities to progress well and
participate fully in all activities. Local links and partnerships provide
extra learning activities for able, gifted and talented pupils, for
example, a music day at Hatton Hall.
The school cares well for all pupils. They say bullying is not a
problem and are certain that staff will sort out any problems. Pupils
understand how to be healthy and willingly contribute to the school
and local community. They participate eagerly in all opportunities
presented by the good curriculum and exciting range of extra
activities and clubs; sports activities are recognised in an
Activemark award. Links and visits beyond the immediate locality,
for example to a city synagogue, give pupils a good awareness of
other ways of life within our society. However, this does not extend
to the range of lifestyles and cultures in the wider world.
Senior leaders, staff and governors are strongly united in their
commitment to a shared vision for school improvement. Leaders at
all levels evaluate the school’s performance accurately and
inspection findings match their judgements of the school’s
effectiveness. Improvements since the last inspection include staff
involvement in leadership, more accurate assessment and wider
professional development. Value for money is good. Consequently,
there is good capacity for sustained improvement.
St Paul First School
This information comes from an Ofsted inspection carried out in 2009.
Information about the School
This is a large school. Most pupils come from White British families
living within the local town, although a significant number travel
344
from nearby towns and villages. Few pupils do not speak English as
their first language. Many pupils have experience of other
educational settings before they begin school. The proportion of
pupils with special educational needs and/or disabilities is below
national average. There has been a period of higher than usual staff
mobility but this has stabilised. The head teacher has been in post
for two years.
Main findings
St Paul’s C of E First School is a good school that promotes high
quality care for the well-being of all pupils, together with a
stimulating learning environment that enables pupils to achieve
well. Behaviour and relationships are good and this contributes to
the calm, safe, industrious and happy atmosphere within the
school. Pupils are comfortable expressing their opinions to adults,
and this demonstrates their growing independence and confidence.
They enjoy their learning and the opportunities available to help
them find out about other places, people and cultures. Their
spiritual, moral, social and cultural development is outstanding.
This is evident in the mutual respect and understanding shown by
everyone. Pupils appreciate the wide range of creative and sports
that enrich their education, including visits to places of interest and
working closely with other schools. There are dose links with the
local church and pupils also have an understanding of other faiths
and cultures. Pupils are engaged in fundraising and charity work
locally, nationally and in Africa.
Pupils make good progress in their academic work. They enter
school with skills and understanding above the level expected for
their age and make good progress, so that by the time they
transfer to the next stage of their education their attainment is well
above average. In the Early Years Foundation Stage the outdoor
learning area is used well to provide interesting activities but the
indoor experiences are less stimulating. The headteacher and senior
management team acknowledge that staffing issues in the past
have slowed the pace of learning for some pupils but this has been
rectified. As a result, pupils are generally making good progress in
their lessons. Strategies have recently been introduced to improve
the mathematical ability of all pupils, particularly in calculation and
problem solving, but there has been insufficient time for them to
have had an impact on standards which for the more-able pupils
are not as high as they could be.
The quality of teaching is good and there are some examples of
outstanding teaching. Teachers know the pupils very well. Detailed
assessments inform lesson planning so that tasks are usually
matched to the individual needs of pupils. High quality support is
used well to support pupils in lessons. Marking is good, particularly
in writing, and guides pupils well so that they know what to do to
improve the quality of their work. Where teaching is less effective,
345
pupils have too few opportunities to engage in discussions and to
become actively involved in their learning, and in mathematics in
particular, the more-able pupils are not always fully challenged.
Pupils have a very good knowledge and understanding of factors
which contribute to their physical and emotional well-being so that
they are keen to adopt healthy lifestyles. Pupils assume
responsibilities within school, including organising the music for
assemblies and looking after younger children through a mentoring
programme. Attendance is satisfactory, but not enough has been
done to motivate the few pupils who do not attend school regularly
into doing so.
The school has shown that it has good capacity to improve. The
senior leadership team have accurately identified areas for
development aid effective action has been taken to address the
issues raised in the last inspection. Innovative strategies have been
introduced to raise attainment in writing. The curriculum has been
reviewed and revised so that pupils can consolidate their literacy,
numeracy and information and communication technology skills
across different subject areas. Work is progressing well to provide
appropriate opportunities for more-able pupils through more
accurate target setting and planning individual learning
opportunities in mathematics.
Argyle Common First School
This information comes from an Ofsted inspection carried out in 2009.
Information about the School
This is a smaller than average primary school which draws its pupils
from the local village and the surrounding area. Almost all pupils
are of White British origin and they are taught in five mixed-age
classes. The proportion of pupils with special educational needs
and/or disabilities is below average but increasing. There is Early
Years Foundation Stage provision for children from the age of four
who share their classroom with pupils in Year 1. The school has
very spacious outdoor areas.
Main findings
This is a good school. It has improved markedly since its last
inspection because the headteacher's strong leadership has
successfully encouraged the staff to have high aspirations for
themselves and pupils. Improvement in the way leaders monitor
and evaluate teaching and learning outcomes has strengthened
pupils' progress. Pupils achieve well, and standards are above
average and improving. Evidence from the standardised
346
assessments in 2009 and from the work of current pupils, shows
there is more to do to improve writing, particularly for boys of all
ages. However, a good start has been made to encourage pupils to
write by making writing activities more meaningful and relevant
and developing word banks so that the pupils can be more
independent. Good progress has been made in improving the level
of challenge provided for the more able pupils, especially at Key
Stage 1, which was a key area for improvement from the last
inspection. The curriculum has also been strengthened and is now
good. The inclusion of themes such as being healthy, recycling and
climate change ensure that it reflects a changing world. The school
is rightly proud of its recently acquired 'green flag' award for its eco
work. Success is celebrated through good quality displays across
the school which also reflect the global dimension of the curriculum.
The school is well placed to improve even further.
Most pupils behave well in and around the school and have very
positive attitudes which make a considerable contribution to their
learning. However, there is a very small minority in one class who
find it difficult to maintain their concentration in lessons and display
immature attitudes. This is a barrier to their learning. The pupils
have a good understanding of how to keep safe and lead healthy
lives. Their spiritual, social and moral development is good and is
shown in their friendly manner, cooperative working and in the way
that older pupils support and help younger ones. Their cultural
development is satisfactory. Although attendance is above average,
too much time is lost for some pupils because they are taken on
holidays in term time. The school council has a positive influence on
how the school develops. Pupils have a good understanding about
keeping themselves safe and trust the adults who look after them.
At break times, the pupils are very active in the spacious play
areas. Most express their views and opinions with great confidence
and maturity.
Teaching and learning are good because lessons are well planned to
meet the full range of pupils' needs. Teaching assistants make a
good and sometimes outstanding contribution to those with special
educational needs and/or disabilities. While lessons have clear
objectives for learning, these are occasionally too This is a good
school. It has improved markedly since its last inspection because
the headteacher's strong leadership has successfully encouraged
the staff to have high aspirations for themselves and pupils.
Improvement in the way leaders monitor and evaluate teaching and
learning outcomes has strengthened pupils' progress. Pupils achieve
well, and standards are above average and improving. Evidence
from the standardised assessments in 2009 and from the work of
current pupils, shows there is more to do to improve writing,
particularly for boys of all ages. However, a good start has been
made to encourage pupils to write by making writing activities more
meaningful and relevant and developing word banks so that the
pupils can be more independent. Good progress has been made in
improving the level of challenge provided for the more able pupils,
347
especially at Key Stage 1, which was a key area for improvement
from the last inspection. The curriculum has also been strengthened
and is now good. The inclusion of themes such as being healthy,
recycling and climate change ensure that it reflects a changing
world. The school is rightly proud of its recently acquired 'green
flag' award for its eco work. Success is celebrated through good
quality displays across the school which also reflect the global
dimension of the curriculum. The school is well placed to improve
even further.
Most pupils behave well in and around the school and have very
positive attitudes which make a considerable contribution to their
learning. However, there is a very small minority in one class who
find it difficult to maintain their concentration in lessons and display
immature attitudes. This is a barrier to their learning. The pupils
have a good understanding of how to keep safe and lead healthy
lives. Their spiritual, social and moral development is good and is
shown in their friendly manner, cooperative working and in the way
that older pupils support and help younger ones. Their cultural
development is satisfactory. Although attendance is above average,
too much time is lost for some pupils because they are taken on
holidays in term time. The school council has a positive influence on
how the school develops. Pupils have a good understanding about
keeping themselves safe and trust the adults who look after them.
At break times, the pupils are very active in the spacious play
areas. Most express their views and opinions with great confidence
and maturity.
Teaching and learning are good because lessons are well planned to
meet the full range of pupils' needs. Teaching assistants make a
good and sometimes outstanding contribution to those with special
educational needs and/or disabilities. While lessons have clear
objectives for learning, these are occasionally too general and of
limited benefit in helping pupils understand what is expected. The
marking of writing is consistently good, providing comments to
commend good work and set further challenges. Pupils have regular
opportunities to share how well they think they are doing. However,
the use of individual pupil targets during lessons is at an early stage
of development.
Provision for children in the Early Years Foundation Stage is good.
The children enjoy school and join in confidently with all the
activities offered. They respond well to the good range of
opportunities to make choices and decisions for themselves.
Leadership has continued to be well focused since the time of the
last report. Staff have an accurate view of the school's strengths
and weaknesses, which they openly debate. This is enabling them
to refine and further develop their practice. Governors have a
visible presence around the school and have helped to forge strong
links with parents. They provide a satisfactory challenge to the
headteacher and other leaders to account for the success of
changes being made.
348
Draycott First School
This information comes from an Ofsted inspection carried out in 2007.
Description of the School
Draycott is a very small rural school with 37 pupils on roll. The
school draws most of its pupils from the surrounding villages. The
school's social and economic context is relatively favourable and
very few pupils are eligible for free school meals. A small number of
pupils are registered by the school as having learning difficulties
and disabilities. There are no pupils with a statement of special
educational need. All pupils are of White British origin.
Overall Effectiveness of the School
Draycott is a good school which is distinguished by a caring, family-
centred ethos where everyone works together. Pupils enter the
Reception class with standards expected for their age. As a result of
the good provision, which is reflected in careful assessment, and a
good appreciation of the individual needs of the pupils, a higher-
than-average proportion of them gain the early learning goals.
Pupils continue to make good progress in Key Stage 1 and Year 4,
achieving above-average standards. In Year 3, the pupils' progress
in mathematics and writing is too slow and standards are average.
All the pupils are well cared for in a safe, secure and welcoming
school community which successfully promotes the school's
Christian ethos. The school has very strong links with the village
church. The pupils' personal, social, emotional development and
well-being are given high priority and are good. Secure academic
guidance supports the pupils to achieve well in most year groups.
The pupils' behaviour and attitudes to learning are good. Pupils are
enthusiastic, respectful and well mannered. They know about
healthy eating and the importance of leading a healthy lifestyle. The
school council are good ambassadors for the school. They greatly
appreciate their school and have confidence in the adults who work
with them.
Almost 80% of the parents returned inspection questionnaires and
the responses were overwhelmingly positive. One parent said: 'My
child loves this school. She has thrived in the friendly, one big
happy family atmosphere.'
The quality of teaching and learning is good. Expectations are high
and relationships are positive. On occasions, there are too many
objectives in lessons, which results in a lack of clarity to drive
forward the learning at a fast enough pace, for example, in
mathematics and writing in Year 3. Pupils with learning difficulties
349
and disabilities make satisfactory progress in light of their often
complex difficulties and low starting points. The curriculum is good
because it is carefully tailored to the needs of the pupils, and has a
wide range of enrichment activities.
Leadership and management are good. The headteacher has a
passionate commitment to improving the life chances of the pupils,
which is clearly shown through a good team spirit and a common
sense of purpose in all aspects of the school's day-to-day life.
Whole-school self-evaluation is satisfactory but is not sufficiently
evaluative. The outcomes of monitoring the quality of teaching do
not always result in the identification of clear targets for
improvement or show the links between good teaching and
effective learning.
Greenville Park Community School
This information comes from an Ofsted inspection carried out in 2008.
Description of the School
The school serves an area that has many social and economic
disadvantages. When pupils enter the Foundation Stage, their
knowledge and skills are usually well below what is expected for
their age. Basic skills in language are weak and social, emotional
and behavioural skills are underdeveloped. The proportion of pupils
who are eligible for free school meals is well above the national
average. The proportion of pupils who are on the school's register
of learning difficulties and/or disabilities well above the national and
local authority averages
Overall Effectiveness of the School
The overall effectiveness of the school and pupils' achievements are
satisfactory although standards are too low, particularly in Years 3
to 6 and in English. Over the last 18 months, there have been The
pupils' personal development and well-being, including their
behaviour, are satisfactory. Pupils' attitudes to school life and
learning are consistently satisfactory and often good. The provision
and outcomes for pupils on the school's register of learning
difficulties and/or disabilities and for those who speak English as an
additional language are good.
The quality of teaching and learning is satisfactory with an
increasing proportion that is good. Nevertheless, a few weaknesses
remain, particularly the lack of challenge in some lessons, where
pupils' work is either too easy or too difficult and not matched well
enough to their different capabilities. The curriculum is satisfactory
and there is a good range of enrichment activities, which improve
350
the pupils' self-esteem. The quality of care, guidance and support is
good. Pupils are well looked after and feel safe and secure. The
school is successful in helping them to understand their emotions
and appreciate the importance of respecting each other, themselves
and the adults who work with them.
The pupils' attendance is well below the national average for
primary schools. In addition, the attendance of just over 10% of
the pupils is poor, and below 85%. The school is working hard with
parents and external agencies, including the education welfare
officer, to raise attendance levels, but is not yet making sufficient
inroads into improving overall attendance. Most of the pupils whose
attendance is poor make slow progress and attain low standards.
Collective leadership and management are secure. The headteacher
is resilient, has successfully raised expectations and given the
school a sense of purpose and clarity about what it can achieve.
There is a good team spirit and senior teachers know there is still
much to do to raise standards and the achievement of all pupils.
For example, while monitoring is satisfactory, the guidance teachers
receive to help improve their work is not always sharp enough and
timescales for improvement are sometimes too long. The school
provides sound value for money and has a satisfactory capacity to
improve further.
351