Morphological Knowledge
Morphological Knowledge
rules and is represented in the human mind and how it is used in language processing
( Booij , 2005 , P. 231 ) . The paper searches for the above concept, mind and
morphological knowledge is a fertile ground for many different theories about the
nature of linguistic rules. When someone wants to use the word books, he must do
this either by retrieving the plural form of the word book from his lexical memory or
creating a new word immediately by adding the plural suffix -s to the stem book ( Ib
linguistic behavior and the psychological tactics thought to underlie that behavior.
There are two viable directions of study. One may additionally use language as a
language as it impacts memory, perception, attention, learning, etc.), and for this the
time period psychological linguistics is now and again used. Alternatively, one may
language (e.g. how reminiscence barriers have an effect on speech production and
1
comprehension). It is the latter which has provided the main center of attention of
interest in linguistics, where the difficulty is essentially considered as the learn about
between linguistic and cognitive universals, the learn about of reading, language
Mental knowledge of words is crystallized by the idea that lexical knowledge in the
native speaker is quite different from the form in the dictionary. This concept can be
those found in words in the mind. There are many words that many people do not
know. The native adult speakers of English , who got a higher education may know a
little more than 50,000 word types and certainly, words that one understands without
2
being spoken are more than words that are actually used in language ( Booij , 2005 ,
P . 232 ) .
Speakers of each language, then, draw on a common set of forms with agreed-on
meanings when they talk. In general, words provide the building blocks for larger
units. They may be combined as idioms, where the meanings of the parts do not add
up to the meaning of the whole, as in to belt up, to be in a flap, to hit the sack, to keep
tabs on, or to blow one's own trumpet. Idioms like these are typically restricted in
syntax, so some, for instance, may not appear in the passive (compare He blew his
own trumpet vs. * His own trumpet was blown by himself). Words may be combined
in short phrases that act as if they were single words, as in by and large, in short,
So the cognitive linguistic construction are quite clear that it is radically different
from that found in the dictionaries as a mental chain as Clark explained above. A
that are printed at another time contain new words and delete other words. The mental
process is quite different, man always has the ability to use words not yet placed in a
(a new word ) . Certainly there are differences between the dictionary and the mental
3
about the words of his language. It contains information about pronunciation,
category (part of speech) and meaning. It also includes information about grammatical
or what lexicographers call (range of application) , any specific terms in which the
Words in the mental lexicon carry a number of relationships to each other. Words that
mental lexicon, this is inferred from speech errors ( Booij , 2005 , P. 233 ) . The
morphological relationships. In a dictionary, on the other hand, words have one type
dictionary and a mental lexicon is that the letter also stores information about the
frequency with which you come across a word , for instance the word nice is used far
It is quite likely that in our mental lexicons we have entries that are occurred only
partial. One may know the pronunciation of a word, but not its meaning (e.g., One
4
knows how to pronounce amortize, but he is not sure what it means). Or the opposite:
for example, he knows what the word hegemony means, but he does not know if it’s
pronounced with the stress on the first or second syllable. One may also have only
distributor is part of a car and that if he has to replace it, it’s a relatively expensive
job, but he does not know what a distributor looks like or what it does Each person’s
mental lexicon is sure to contain things that are different from other people’s mental
lexicons. One person may know lots of words for types of birds or flowers, another
might know all the specialized vocabulary of sailing, and so on. Auto mechanics
surely know more details of the meaning of the word distributor than I do. But the
individual mental lexicons overlap enough that one can speak the same language
5
Acquisition of Morphology
Morphological rules are discovered on the basis of words formed according to these
laws. Children in the first phase of acquisition acquire complex single words. The
children then learn how to add (s) of the plural to the individual nouns to make plural
nouns , and then make plural nouns that they have never heard of before (Booij , 2005
, P. 236 – 237 ) .
This stage is certainly not without overgeneralization . At the lexical level, children
over generalize words to referents for which they would not be appropriate for adults
at the morphological level, children over generalize derivational suffixes at the level
of verb argument structure (syntax), children over generalize verbs into syntactic
In childhood, children acquire higher frequency morphological forms (which are used
more than other forms ). Children have the ability to formulate new words at an early
age. They can also acquire the compound . Compounding is acquired relatively early
because it complies with two principles , those of transparency and simplicity . The
meaning of compounds can be related very easily to those of its constituent words and
hence their meaning is transparent . Morever the form of the constituent elements is
not really changed where they are part of compounds and thus conforms to the
6
requirement of simplicity (Booij , 2005 , P . 238-239). Although simplicity concept in
linguistics may be confused , Bochner ( 1993 , P.1) states perhaps confusion about
this matter can be traced to the use of the term "Simplicity Measure" for particular
can connect between the verb and the stem noun . They also use the noun as a verb or
to refer to the noun's activities. Children at an early age are able to compare and
These words and their morphological forms are the main component of Syntax ,
Aitchison ( 1987 , P. 97 ) states that , gradually words are integrated into the network
and the links are built between co-ordinates , perhaps partly as a consequence of
acquiring syntax and partly to allow for fast word finding as the overall vocabulary
increases.
Psycholinguists have made experiments to try to learn how children and adults are
able to acquire words so easily. One might think that the learning of new words is a
simple matter of association: someone points at something and says “flurge” and you
learn that that something is called a flurge. This may be the way that we learn some
.words, but surely not the way we learn the majority of words in our mental lexicons
7
For one thing, not everything for which we have a word can be pointed at
actively instructing us about the meanings of words; although parents may point to
things in a picture book and name them for a child, or school children may be asked to
memorize a list of vocabulary words, we learn most words without explicit instruction
and seemingly with very little exposure. Although we do not know nearly enough
about this subject, there are several things that we do know about how word learning
occurs ( Ib Id : P.17 ) . It is believed that both children and adults are able to do what
the psycholinguist Susan Carey has called fast mapping . Fast mapping is the ability
to pick up new words on the basis of a few random exposures to them. In one
experiment, Carey showed that children who were casually exposed to a new color
of various colors) were able to absorb the word and recall it even six weeks later.
Experiments have shown that adults exhibit this fast mapping ability as well; while
the ability to learn linguistic rules (say, of syntax or phonology) is thought to decline
8
The Evidences on the Mental Source Side of Morphological
Information
priming experiments: two stimuli are presented to subjects and it is tested whether the
first stimulus (word) has influence on the recognition of the second .Priming is an
dog). The stimulus to which responses are made (e.g., dog) is the target and the
preceding stimulus (e.g., cat or table) is the prime. The classical task for investigating
semantic priming is the lexical decision task. The stimuli consist of correctly spelled
words and meaningless strings of letters called “non words” (e.g., blit). On each trial
9
of the experiment, a prime and a target are displayed on a computer screen.
Participants are instructed to read the prime silently and then to decide whether the
target is a word or a non word. The standard finding is that lexical decision responses
are faster and more accurate when the target is semantically related to the prime (e.g.,
cat-dog) than when the target is semantically unrelated to the prime (e.g., table-dog).
Another commonly used task is naming or pronunciation. In this task, people are
asked to read the target word aloud as rapidly as possible (non words are typically not
presented). Again, the common finding is that people can name the target word faster
semantically unrelated word ( Mcnamara , 2005 , P. 4) . Priming means that the prior
presentation of another word. This means that the subject who has to make a lexical
of English, the response latency will be reduced if, before this task is performed, the
same word calculation is presented to the eye or the ear of the subject, a case of
identity priming. This effect suggests that the word calculation receives a higher level
of activation by previous access to the same word. Other ways of priming are
phonological priming (the prime word is phonologically similar to the target word),
and semantic priming (with a semantically similar word as the prime). In the case of
10
the target word calculation, the words calcium and computation may function as
phonological and semantic primes. If these primes reduce response latencies for the
target word, one can conclude that words in the mental lexicon are connected to
priming, a word that is morphologically related to the target word is used as a prime.
For instance, calculate will function as a prime for calculation. As this example
2005 , P.241) . Naturalistic data are the 2nd source of proof for theories of
morphology in the mind. These are statistics regarding the true conduct of language
users. When we study that teenagers coin new words or word types that they have no
longer come throughout before, this varieties naturalistic information that might also
be interpreted as evidence for the children’s having obtained morphological rules. The
same applies to the language use of adults. We can conclude that a morphological
sample has the popularity of productive rule if new instantiations of that sample are
determined in the true language use of native speakers. Speech mistakes (which can
also be elicited in experiments) are any other occasion of naturalistic data ( Ib Id) .
The language behaviour of people with a language deficit, such as aphasia, may throw
11
light on the mental representation of morphology. Aphasia is a language impairment
resulting from damage to the brain, in most cases due to a cardiovascular accident.
Aphasics that suffer from agrammatism (the inability to use rules), usually referred to
as Broca aphasics, may not be able to produce correct plural forms of nouns, except
for nouns with a high frequency plural form such as the word eyes(Ib Id ) . The
Mistakes like this occur repeatedly in the speech of some aphasics, and in its extreme
TOMORROW. They seem able to find names connected with the general area they
are talking about, but unable to pinpoint particular words within it, so that a ‘garden
roller’ could be called a LAWN MOWER, a ‘spade’ may be called a FORK, and a
‘rake’ may be called a HOE. A mistake like this occurred in one of the aphasic
passages quoted above, when the patient said DIVING instead of ‘swimming
( Aitchison , 2008 , P.242 ). This is evidence for the position that high-frequency
plural nouns are retrieved from lexical memory, whereas low-frequency plural forms
are computed on the spot, and may not be stored. In an investigation of three
12
German aphasic patients with agrammatism it was found that their syntactic abilities
were severely impaired, whereas their ability to use inflectional morphology was still
intact . This fact can be interpreted as evidence for the position that syntactic rules
morphology (Booij , 2005 ,P.242). The discovered capacity of native speakers to coin
new phrases or word varieties is the simple argument for assuming that morphological
knowledge encompasses greater than storage of the complicated words that language
users are exposed to. There ought to be mechanisms in the mind that allow us to
prolong the set of complex phrases in a language. For languages with a wealthy
machine of inflectional morphology such as Turkish, the place every phrase may have
lots or hundreds of forms, it would even be quite absurd to expect that all these
varieties are memorized as such. The reminiscence load for such languages can be
decreased drastically through making use of rules. A viable model is that language
13
the storage at the time of need. It is also a system that forms rules without the need to
recall storage. These facts have led a number of linguists to defend a dual system
model of morphological knowledge . The past tense and participial forms of regular
English verbs are not stored in lexical memory, but always created by rule. The
irregular forms, on the other hand, are stored in memory. These stored irregular forms
are linked to each other in an associative way, and thus the language user will be able
to discover similarity patterns such as -ing/k -ang/k -ung/k for the set of certain
verbs . This explains why the pattern may be extended incidentally to similar verbs
such as to bring .(Booij , 2005 , P.242) . The pioneer in the field of this research is
Clahsen where he reached the following results .By looking at the regular and
irregular inflection of the German past participle and the noun plural formation from
various perspectives Clahsen achieved his main goal namely to show that the
he argues for the strong position that only irregular inflected words are stored in the
lexicon, where as all regular inflected words are processed by rule some problems
14
arise. His proposed model is not fine grained enough to capture for instance all the
nuances of regularities within the German plural inflection system and it can’t deal
15
References
Library of Congress
Press
Blackwell Publishing
Press