Working Memory: Literature Review An Introduction
Working Memory: Literature Review An Introduction
Literature Review
An Introduction
Working memory has fascinated scholars since its inception in the 1960’s Indeed,
more than a century of scientific studies revolving around memory in the fields of
psychology, biology, or neuroscience have not completely agreed upon a unified
categorization of memory, especially in terms of its functions and mechanisms .
Recent research is bringing to light the importance of working memory in everyone's
daily life in academic, professional, and social settings.This document seeks to
present an up-to-date introductory overview of the realm of working memory by
outlining several working memory studies from the psychological and neuro-sciences
perspectives in an effort to refine and unite the scientific knowledge concerning
working memory.
It was not until 2000 that another component termed “episodic buffer” was
introduced into this working memory model. Episodic buffer was regarded as a
temporary storage system that modulates and integrates different sensory
information. In short, the central executive functions as the “control center” that
oversees manipulation, recall, and processing of information (non-verbal or verbal)
for meaningful functions such as decision-making, problem-solving or even
manuscript writing.
● Sentence Completion and Recall-- This task requires the subject to listen to
a series of sentences, fill in the missing last word in each sentence, and
repeat the words of each of the sentences presented.
William James (1890) stated that, unlike the virtually unlimited amount of
knowledge that can be stored in a person’s “secondary memory,” only a small
amount of information can be kept conscious at any one time in one’s “primary
memory.” The best-known account of working memory limitations is George Miller’s
(1956) proposal of capacity limits, in which he argued that people are able to keep
track of a “magic number 7 plus or minus 2” chunks of information. Following are
some of the experimental studies of working memory capacity.
On the basis of the well-known serial position effects (i.e., the primacy and recency
effects in free recall) and some neuropsychological dissociations (as demonstrated
by such patients as HM), early information-processing models of memory assumed a
structural distinction between STM (or working memory) and LTM. Moreover, these
models considered STM a gateway or stepping stone to a more permanent LTM,5
proposing that information that is either rehearsed, attended to, or organized
properly in STS is transferred to an LTS. In contrast to this structural view of human
memory, an alternative view emphasized the continuity between working memory
and LTM and proposed that working memory can be considered an activated portion
of LTM representations (Norman, 1968). This more continuous view gained some
popularity in cognitive psychology as empirical data that challenged the
interpretations of the hallmark empirical findings for the structural view – the serial
position effects and the neuropsychological dissociations – started to accumulate .
The first insights into the neuronal and neurotransmitter basis of working memory
came from animal research. The work of Jacobsen and Fulton in the 1930s first
showed that lesions to the PFC impaired spatial working memory performance in
monkeys. The later work of Joaquin Fuster recorded the electrical activity of neurons
in the PFC of monkeys while they were doing a delayed matching task. In that task,
the monkey sees how the experimenter places a bit of food under one of two
identical-looking cups. A shutter is then lowered for a variable delay period,
screening off the cups from the monkey's view. After the delay, the shutter opens
and the monkey is allowed to retrieve the food from under the cups. Successful
retrieval in the first attempt – something the animal can achieve after some training
on the task – requires holding the location of the food in memory over the delay
period. Fuster found neurons in the PFC that fired mostly during the delay period,
suggesting that they were involved in representing the food location while it was
invisible. Later research has shown similar delay-active neurons also in the posterior
parietal cortex, the thalamus, the caudate, and the globus pallidus. The work of
Goldman-Rakic and others showed that principal sulcal, dorsolateral PFC
interconnects with all of these brain regions, and that neuronal microcircuits within
PFC are able to maintain information in working memory through recurrent excitatory
glutamate networks of pyramidal cells that continue to fire throughout the delay
period. These circuits are tuned by lateral inhibition from GABAergic
interneurons.The neuromodulatory arousal systems markedly alter PFC working
memory function; for example, either too little or too much dopamine or
norepinephrine impairs PFC network firing and working memory performance.
Conclusion
The research done so for in working memory and as summarized in this
literature review suggest that researchers have been quite successful in answering
the following questions about Working memory which are :
1. Basic model and representations in working memory.
2. The control and regulation of working memory which includes questions like
how the memory is developed and when does it decay or is it improvable.
3. The nature of working memory limitations i.e the capacity of working memory.
4. Role of working memory in day to day cognitive activities.
5. How is Long term memory and Knowledge related to working memory.
6. The biological implementation or the hardware of working memory.
7. Relation between working memory and attention & consciousness.