Assignment 2
Assignment 2
A work example is a step-by-step solution on how to carry out a task, usually accompanied
by explanations for each step (Renkl, Atkinson, & Große, 2004). There is much debate about
the effectiveness as well as the limitations of worked examples (Logan & Ho, 2013). This
paper was designed in an attempt to answer the question: Is learning from worked
examples a good classroom strategy?. This paper highlights some of the benefits in
conjunction with some limitations of worked examples; including the worked example
effect, different variants of worked examples, how worked examples do/do not promote
learning. By the end of this paper, there will be a personal point of view to answer this
question.
In a traditional lesson, after the teacher explains the new topic and solves a few
examples on the board, then students will be given a worksheet with similar problems to
solve themselves. Instead of having students practice solving those problems, it would be
better for the students to study worked out examples. A vast majority of studies has found
that learning via worked examples in the early stages of cognitive skill acquisitions is more
effective, compared to the traditional method of problem-solving (Zhu & Simon, 1987).
According to Merriënboer and Sweller (2005), this is known as “worked example effect”.
Sweller, & Clark, 2006). This problem-solving search overburdens limited working memory
extraneous cognitive load by freeing the limited working memory to allow novice learners
build on the underlying schema and learn the procedure (Kalyuga, Renkl, & Paas, 2010). In
the case of problem-solving, a novice learner often has to employ general problem-solving
strategies in order to solve a problem. Such strategies impose a high intrinsic cognitive load
which leads to cognitive overload that does not contribute to productive learning processes
(Renkl, Hilbert, & Schworm, 2009). To expand on this, learners need to know the concepts
or procedures in order to solve a problem, without those, they will learn almost nothing. By
providing learners with a worked example, it not only helps to reduce extraneous cognitive
load but also allows learners to focus their attention on learning the steps in problem-
solving. Additionally, one of the benefits of learning from worked example is that students
do not have to work out the answer but rather focusing in studying the applied principles in
the presented solution, constructing and automating a cognitive schema (Renkl at al., 2009).
(2015) argued that the benefit of studying from worked examples does not apply equally to
all learners. The superior of using worked examples instead of problem-solving on less
experienced learners has been demonstrated in many research over the years. However,
there are occasions where the effect of worked examples does not foster learning or, may
even impair later learning (Kirschner et al., 2006). Many studies have shown that worked
examples with step-by-step solutions work well for novice learners, who have not yet
developed proper schema but may inhibit learning for those who are more knowledgeable.
This reversal effectiveness of worked examples is also known as the expertise reversal effect
(Van Gog, Paas, & Van Merriënboer, 2008). Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler, & Sweller (2003)
explained that for more experienced learners, the available schemata make the worked-out
solution steps redundant. In this case, Furthermore, the solution steps may no longer
contribute to effective learning, or at worst start to hamper learning for high prior
knowledge students. That is to say, the expertise-reversal effect may actually cause an
enormous extraneous load for a more advanced learner (Van Gog et al., 2008).
Bokosmaty et al. (2015) suggested teachers could use different variants of worked
examples to counter the expertise reversal effect and also to improve students' learning.
There are four types of worked examples; including faded worked examples, worked
examples. The four listed worked examples can be used for different educational purposes
The first type of worked example - faded example - can be used to address the
expertise reversal effect (Atkinson, Renkl, & Merril, 2003). When using faded worked
examples, the first one or more examples have complete working out, but for subsequent
examples, the amount of “worked-out” will successively fade out, and learners have to
complete the missing step(s). Renk et al. (2004) explained such scaffolding, or fading away
facilitates the shift from studying worked examples to solving problems in the later stage;
once students develop the necessary knowledge and procedure to carry out the task, the
solving. Research has shown that by learning from faded examples, students not only
perform better in solving harder problems but also can achieve the same level of
performance in a shorter period of times (Atkinson et al., 2003). Renkl, Atkinson, Maier, and
Staley (2002) found that it is better to fade the steps from the end rather than the beginning
of the problem.
The second type of worked example is worked examples with self-explanation. This
approach requires students to answer the “what”, “how”, and “why” the steps included in
the examples are done correctly. This self-explanation promotes conceptual understanding
as well as increases declarative knowledge (Aleven & Koedinger, 2002; Hilbert, Renk,
Schworm, Kessler, & Reis, 2008). In addition, when students actively explain the steps
required in a certain problem, this will help them to identify necessary steps when it comes
to solving similar problems. Also, students’ ability to carry out these processes will improve
as they familiarise themselves with the underlying concepts inherent in the problems
The third type of worked example is incorrect worked examples. Normally the
teacher would provide students correct solution to a problem, however, for the incorrect
worked example, the teacher demonstrates a common mistake that students typically make
when solving a particular problem. Also, the teacher can provide correct examples in
conjunction with incorrect examples, so students can learn from these errors. Durkin and
Rittle-Johnson (2011) found that when students explain an error in the incorrect worked
example, they actually learn it themselves and avoid not to use the wrong
procedure/strategy again. In line with this, there is research suggested that learning from
incorrect work examples, reduces students misconception about the concept, which can
lead to greater procedural and conceptual understanding (Adams, McLaren, Durkin, Mayer,
Rittle-Johnson, Isotani, & van Velsen, 2014; Booth, Lange, Koedinger, & Newton, 2013).
Studying incorrect worked examples have shown to be beneficial for all students regardless
In contrast, often students make the same mistakes over and over again because of
their misunderstandings not being properly addressed (Renkl, 2017). It is important that the
teacher explicitly highlight or point out the part where the errors occur. If the errors are
explicitly stated, students with less experienced can avoid making these errors. However,
most of the time, worked examples only provide the correct solutions to mathematical
problems. These worked examples do not address and correct student errors (Renkl, 2017).
The effects of worked examples can be enhanced by including a mix of correct and incorrect
worked out solutions. For example, students will be given a mix of correct and incorrect
working out solutions for a mathematical problem. Students will then have the opportunity
to sort these solutions into two categories: “correct working out solution” and “incorrect
working out solution”. Next, students identify the errors which are found in the incorrect
working out solution and then fix the errors. This technique of students identifying incorrect
working out a solution and fixing the errors in the incorrect solution has now been used in
Showing students two or multiple examples and then letting students compare and
contrast them is the idea of the last variant - comparing worked examples (Rittle-Johnson &
Star, 2007). Rittle-Johnson and Star (2007) also found that students will gain both
procedural and conceptual understanding when they are given opportunities to learn
students to “move beyond rigid adherence to a single solution method to the more adaptive
On the other hand, the potential of worked examples can be hindered if they are not
used appropriately. Many teachers believe that worked examples only promote knowledge
about procedures but not conceptual understanding. These worked examples just teach
students the knowledge about algorithms (Renkl, 2017). For example, the students
worked examples. They then apply the algorithms in the worked examples to solve similar
problems that just differ with respect to the used numbers or objects. As a result, one
support way for worked examples to effectively lead to conceptual understanding is that the
students are asked prompting, open-ended questions so that they can self-explain the
worked examples (Boaler & Foster, 2007). Asking prompting questions will develop
students’ higher-order thinking, cater for mixed ability students, and hence encourage them
of a mathematical concept such as diagrams, equations, symbols and many more separately
or in isolations. The students may fail to see the relationship between different
representations and hence limiting their conceptual understanding. Teachers should help
same concept. Teachers can achieve this by explicitly explain to their students the technical
meanings of mathematical terminologies, notations and symbols which their students have
never seen before (Schleppegrell, 2007). Teachers can also use “color coding” to help
Teachers also carefully set out the structure of worked examples. For example, in Stage 4
topic “Properties of geometrical figures”, the equation and a geometrical figure should be
put near the angles and lines which it is related to rather than printing them separately
(Renkl, 2017).
Based on all of the above findings of the benefits as well as the limitations of worked
examples. I personally think learning from worked examples is a good classroom strategy.
The challenge here is to overcome all the problems such as how and when worked examples
should be used or which group of learners will benefit from what sort of worked examples.
Approaches that can be used to counter these problems are teachers explicitly explain the
meaning of symbols, terminologies, and diagrams, teachers asking prompting and open-
ended questions, and teachers must take into consideration the experience/knowledge of
learners. With efficient use of these strategies will result in the effective use of worked
Year 12
Topic: Calculus Subtopics: MA-C2 Differential Calculus
C2.2: Rules of differentiation
The teacher explicitly explain the product, quotient and chain rules of differentiation.
- provide students worked examples with step-by-step how to apply these rules
- let students learn these worked examples carefully before asking them to do similar or
harder problems
When students just learn the concept, the teacher should not ask students to do a long list
The teacher should show students that sometimes it is easier to use quotient rule instead of
product rule. Showing the same questions with different solutions, help students to learn to
A work example guides students the steps required to solve a problem, usually accompanied
by explanations for each step
Benefits of worked examples:
- Learning from worked examples is more effective than problem -solving methods (focus
more on learning instead of searching for strategies)
- Worked examples reduce extraneous load and allow students to concentrate on
understanding the applied principles in the presented solution, constructing and automating
a cognitive schema.
- Different worked examples can be used for different educational purposes (faded worked
examples, self-explanation worked examples, incorrect worked examples and comparing
worked examples)
- All four variants of worked examples benefit all learners
- Students achieve the same level of performance in shorter period of times.
- Promote higher order of thinking which lead to the develop of conceptual understanding.
- Worked examples are not always beneficial to all students, especially experienced
students.
- Worked examples may inhibit learning (expertise reversal effect)
- Many believe that worked examples does not foster conceptual understanding as its main focus is
on procedural demonstrations.
Conclusion
Learning from worked examples is a good classroom strategy when all the limitations are
appropriately addressed
References
Adams, Mclaren, Durkin, Mayer, Rittle-Johnson, Isotani, & Van Velsen. (2014). Using
10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.053
Aleven, V., & Koedinger, K. (2002). An effective metacognitive strategy: Learning by doing
and explaining with a computer‐based Cognitive Tutor. Cognitive Science, 26(2), 147-
Atkinson, R., Renkl, A., & Merrill, M. (2003). Transitioning from studying examples to solving
Boaler, J. & Foster, D. (2007). Raising Expectations and Achievement. The Impact of Wide
Scale Mathematics Reform Giving All Students Access to High Quality Mathematics.
content/uploads/2017/03/Raising-Expectations.pdf
Bokosmaty, S., Sweller, J., & Kalyuga, S. (2015). Learning Geometry Problem Solving by
Booth, Lange, Koedinger, & Newton. (2013). Using example problems to improve student
Durkin, & Rittle-Johnson. (2011). The effectiveness of using incorrect examples to support
learning about decimal magnitude. Learning and Instruction, 22(3), 206-214. doi:
10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.11.001
Hilbert, Renkl, Schworm, Kessler, & Reiss. (2008). Learning to teach with worked‐out
Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler, & Sweller. (2003). The Expertise Reversal Effect. Educational
Kalyuga, S., Renkl, A., & Paas, F. (2010). Facilitating Flexible Problem Solving: A Cognitive
10.1007/s10648-010-9132-9
Kirschner, P., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. (2006). Why Minimal Guidance During Instruction Does
10.1207/s15326985ep4102_1
Logan, T., & Ho, S. Y. (2013). The Classic Word Problem: The Influence of Direct Teaching. In
Steinle, V., Ball., L., & Bardini, C. (Eds.). Mathematics Education: Yesterday, Today and
Merriënboer, J., & Sweller, J. (2005). Cognitive Load Theory and Complex Learning: Recent
doi: 10.1007/s10648-005-3951-0
Renkl, A., Atkinson, R., & Große, K. (2004). How Fading Worked Solution Steps Works – A
10.1023/B:TRUC.0000021815.74806.f6
Renkl, A., Atkinson, R., Maier, U., & Staley, R. (2002). From Example Study to Problem
Renkl, A., Hilbert, T., & Schworm, S. (2009). Example-Based Learning in Heuristic Domains: A
Cognitive Load Theory Account. Educational Psychology Review, 21(1), 67-78. doi:
10.1007/s10648-008-9093-4
Rittle-Johnson, B., & Star, J. (2007). Does Comparing Solution Methods Facilitate Conceptual
Van Gog, Paas, & Van Merriënboer. (2008). Effects of studying sequences of process-
10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.03.003
Zhu, X., & Simon, H. (1987). Learning Mathematics From Examples and by Doing. Cognition