A Powerful Way To Improve Learning and Memory PDF
A Powerful Way To Improve Learning and Memory PDF
SCIENCE BRIEF
A powerful way to improve learning and memory
Practicing retrieval enhances long-term, meaningful learning.
Jeffrey Karpicke, PhD, is the James V. Bradley associate professor of psychological sciences at Purdue University. He received a BA in
psychology from Indiana University and a PhD in psychology from Washington University in St. Louis. Karpicke's research sits at the
interface between cognitive science and education, with the goal of identifying effective strategies that promote long-term learning and
comprehension. Karpicke's research has been funded by the National Science Foundation and the Institute of Education Sciences at the
U.S. Department of Education. Karpicke was the recipient of the Janet Taylor Spence Award for Transformative Early Career
Contributions from Association for Psychological Science, a National Science Foundation CAREER award, and the Presidential Early Career Award for
Scientists and Engineers. Author website (http://learninglab.psych.purdue.edu/) .
The movie "Inside Out" (2015) takes us inside the mind of its young protagonist, an 11-year-old girl named Riley, and depicts memory in a way that is sure
to resonate with many people. In Riley's mind, her memories are objects — globes colored with emotions — that are stored in a mental space, just as
physical objects are stored in a physical space. When Riley experiences an event and creates a new memory, a new globe is produced in her mind,
rolling down a ramp like a ball returning in a bowling alley. When Riley re-experiences a past event, a globe is placed in a projector and events are
replayed, projected on a screen in her mind. Cognitive psychologists refer to the mental processes involved in the creation of new memories and the
recovery of past memories as encoding and retrieval, respectively.
The depiction of the mind in "Inside Out" follows centuries of thought on how mind and memory work. Throughout history, scholars have used a common
metaphor to talk about memory: The mind is a vast storehouse or space; memories are objects stored in that space; and retrieving a memory is akin to
searching for and finding an object in a physical space (Roediger, 1980). To learn something new, according to this view, the challenge lies in getting
knowledge "in" one's mental space. Getting it back "out" when needed is important, too, but learning is usually identified with the encoding of new
knowledge in memory. Retrieval is assumed to be neutral for learning; retrieval is needed to assess what a person has learned, but retrieval processes
themselves are not thought to produce learning.
Recent advances in the science of learning and memory have challenged common assumptions about how learning happens. Specifically, recent work
has shown that retrieval is critical for robust, durable, long-term learning. Every time a memory is retrieved, that memory becomes more accessible in the
future. Retrieval also helps people create coherent and integrated mental representations of complex concepts, the kind of deep learning necessary to
solve new problems and draw new inferences. Perhaps most surprisingly, practicing retrieval has been shown to produce more learning than engaging
in other effective encoding techniques (Karpicke & Blunt, 2011). This approach, which recognizes the central role of retrieval processes in learning and
aims to develop new learning strategies based on retrieval practice, is referred to as retrieval-based learning.
A word-learning experiment illustrates some key points about retrieval-based learning. In the experiment (Karpicke & Bauernschmidt, 2011), students
learned a list of foreign language words (e.g., Swahili vocabulary words like "mashua — boat") across cycles of study and recall trials. In study trials, the
students saw a vocabulary word and its translation on the computer screen, and in recall trials, they saw a vocabulary word and had to recall and type its
translation. The students studied a list of vocabulary words, then attempted to retrieve the whole list, studied it again, retrieved it again, and so on across
alternating study and retrieval practice blocks.
There were several different conditions in the experiment. In one condition, students simply studied the words once, without trying to recall them at all. In
a second condition, students continued studying and recalling the words until they had recalled all of them once. After a word was successfully retrieved
once, it was "dropped" from further practice — the students did not see it again in the learning session.
Other conditions in the experiment examined the effects of repeated retrieval practice. Once a word was recalled, the computer program required the
students to practice retrieving the items three more times. One repeated retrieval condition had the three recall trials happen immediately, three times in
a row. This condition, referred to as massed retrieval practice, is akin to repeating a new piece of information over and over in your head right after you
experience it. Finally, in the last condition highlighted here, the students also practiced retrieving the words three times, but the repeated retrievals were
spaced throughout the learning session. For instance, once a student correctly recalled the translation for mashua, the program moved on to other
vocabulary words, but prompts to practice retrieval of the translation for mashua would pop up later on in the program. In this way, the retrieval
opportunities were spaced throughout the learning session.
The key question in this research was, how well would students remember the vocabulary word translations in the long term? Figure 1 shows the
proportion of translations that students remembered one week after the initial learning session. Merely studying the words once without ever recalling
them produced extremely poor performance (average recall was 1 percent, barely visible on the figure). Practicing until each translation was recalled
once was much better. But what about the effects of repeated retrieval practice? Massed retrieval — repeating the translations three times immediately
https://www.apa.org/science/about/psa/2016/06/learning-memory 1/6
18/03/2019 A powerful way to improve learning and memory
— produced no additional gain in learning. Repeated retrieval enhanced learning only when the repetitions were spaced, and indeed, the effects of
repeated spaced retrieval were very large. In a single experiment, simple changes that incorporated spaced retrieval practice took performance from
nearly total forgetting to extremely good retention (about 80 percent correct) one week after an initial learning experience (see also Karpicke &
Roediger, 2008; Pyc & Rawson, 2010).
An emphasis on getting knowledge in memory shows up on surveys of students' learning strategies. In one survey (Karpicke, Butler & Roediger, 2009),
college students were asked to list the strategies they use while studying and to rank-order the strategies. The results, shown in Figure 2, indicate that
students' most frequent study strategy, by far, is repetitive reading of notes or textbooks. Active retrieval practice lagged far behind repetitive reading
and other strategies (for a review of several learning strategies, see Dunlosky, Rawson, Marsh, Nathan & Willingham, 2013). A wealth of research has
shown that passive repetitive reading produces little or no benefit for learning (Callender & McDaniel, 2009). Yet not only was repetitive reading the
most frequently listed strategy, it was also the strategy most often listed as students' number one choice, by a large margin.
Figure 2. Student study strategy usage. Survey data from Karpicke, Butler, & Roediger (2009).
Why don't learners use repeated retrieval practice more frequently? Many students view retrieval as a "knowledge check"; they test themselves to see if
they know something, rather than out of the belief that practicing retrieval itself will help them learn. This means that many students will use a "one-and-
done" strategy: If they can recall something once, they believe they have learned it, so they remove it from further practice. Many students study this way
when they regulate their own learning (Karpicke, 2009), even though their long-term learning will not benefit from repeated retrieval practice. Instead, a
one-and-done strategy will produce long-term performance similar to the recall-once condition in Figure 1. There is some evidence that instructing
learners about the benefits of retrieval leads students to report using retrieval practice more frequently when they study on their own (Einstein, Mullet &
Harrison, 2012), but the best ways to influence students to practice retrieval remain to be discovered.
https://www.apa.org/science/about/psa/2016/06/learning-memory 2/6
18/03/2019 A powerful way to improve learning and memory
the same amount of time studying in the two conditions; the difference was whether they created concept maps or practiced actively retrieving while
learning.
Figure 3 shows the results of two different final assessments given one week after the learning session. On one assessment, students answered two
types of short-answer questions aimed at measuring meaningful learning: verbatim questions, which assessed concepts stated directly in the texts, and
inference questions, which required students to make new connections across concepts. On another assessment, the final assessment involved creating
a concept map, because concept mapping is often used as an assessment of the coherence and integration of students' knowledge. On the final
verbatim and inference questions and on the final concept map assessment, practicing retrieval during learning produced the best performance, even
better than studying the material by making concept maps.
Figure 3. Data adapted from Karpicke & Blunt (2011) (from Figure 2, panels A and C).
Several additional studies have established that retrieval practice promotes meaningful learning. Retrieval practice enhances the learning of
educationally relevant materials, including educational texts, multimedia presentations, material explained in classroom lectures and a variety of other
complex concepts (Jensen, McDaniel, Woodard & Kummer, 2014; Johnson & Mayer, 2009; Larsen, Butler, Lawson & Roediger, 2013; Lyle & Crawford,
2011; Roediger, Agarwal, McDaniel & McDermott, 2011). Retrieval practice also supports students in making inferences, solving new problems and
transferring knowledge (Butler, 2010; Chan, 2009; Hinze & Wiley, 2011; McDaniel, Howard & Einstein, 2009; Smith & Karpicke, 2014). Retrieval-based
learning is an effective method for improving meaningful learning.
Retrieval is a learning event. Practicing retrieval is a simple and effective way to enhance long-term, meaningful learning.
Some effective learning strategies, like retrieval practice, are underutilized. Conversely, the most popular learning strategy among college students –
repetitive reading – leads to very limited levels of learning.
When practicing retrieval, retrieve more than once and space your retrievals, rather than massing them all together. Self-testing as a knowledge check
is a good idea, but don't stop at just one successful retrieval (one-and-done). Two or three additional spaced retrievals will bolster long-term learning.
Retrieval can happen in a variety of ways, and many existing activities may be converted into retrieval-based learning activities. The key ingredient is to
spend time actively retrieving when trying to learn something new.
References
Agarwal, P.K., Karpicke, J. D., Kang, S.H.K., Roediger, H.L., & McDermott, K.B. (2008). Examining the testing effect with open- and closed-book tests. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 22(7), 861-876. doi:10.1002/acp.1391.
https://www.apa.org/science/about/psa/2016/06/learning-memory 3/6
18/03/2019 A powerful way to improve learning and memory
Balota, D.A., Duchek, J.M., Sergent-Marshall, S.D., & Roediger, H.L. (2006). Does expanded retrieval produce benefits over equal-interval spacing? Explorations of spacing
effects in healthy aging and early stage Alzheimer's disease. Psychology and Aging, 21(1), 19-31. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.19.
Blunt, J.R., & Karpicke, J.D. (2014). Learning with retrieval-based concept mapping. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(3), 849-858. doi:10.1037/a0035934.
Butler, A.C. (2010). Repeated testing produces superior transfer of learning relative to repeated studying. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Cognition, 36(5), 1118-1133. doi:10.1037/a0019902, 10.1037/a0019902.supp.
Callender, A.A., & McDaniel, M.A. (2009). The limited benefits of rereading educational texts. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34(1), 30-41.
doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2008.07.001.
Chan, J.C.K. (2009). When does retrieval induce forgetting and when does it induce facilitation? Implications for retrieval inhibition, testing effect, and text processing.
Journal of Memory and Language, 61(2), 153-170. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2009.04.004.
Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K.A., Marsh, E.J., Nathan, M.J., & Willingham, D.T. (2013). Improving students' learning with effective learning techniques: Promising directions from
cognitive and educational psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14(1), 4-58. doi:10.1177/1529100612453266.
Einstein, G.O., Mullet, H.G., & Harrison, T.L. (2012). The testing effect: Illustrating a fundamental concept and changing study strategies. Teaching of Psychology, 39(3), 190-
193. doi:10.1177/0098628312450432.
Fritz, C.O., Morris, P.E., Nolan, D., & Singleton, J. (2007). Expanding retrieval practice: An effective aid to preschool children's learning. The Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 60(7), 991-1004. doi:10.1080/17470210600823595
Grimaldi, P.J., & Karpicke, J.D. (2014). Guided retrieval practice of educational materials using automated scoring. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(1), 58-68.
doi:10.1037/a0033208.
Hinze, S.R., & Wiley, J. (2011). Testing the limits of testing effects using completion tests. Memory, 19(3), 290-304. doi:10.1080/09658211.2011.560121.
Jensen, J. L., McDaniel, M.A., Woodard, S.M., & Kummer, T.A. (2014). Teaching to the test ... or testing to teach: Exams requiring higher order thinking skills encourage
greater conceptual understanding. Educational Psychology Review, 26(2), 307-329. doi:10.1007/s10648-013-9248-9.
Johnson, C.I., & Mayer, R.E. (2009). A testing effect with multimedia learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(3), 621-629. doi:10.1037/a0015183.
Karpicke, J.D. (2009). Metacognitive control and strategy selection: Deciding to practice retrieval during learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(4),
469-486. doi:10.1037/a0017341.
Karpicke, J.D., & Bauernschmidt, A. (2011). Spaced retrieval: Absolute spacing enhances learning regardless of relative spacing. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(5), 1250-1257. doi:10.1037/a0023436.
Karpicke, J.D., & Blunt, J.R. (2011). Retrieval practice produces more learning than elaborative studying with concept mapping. Science, 331(6018), 772-775.
doi:10.1126/science.1199327.
Karpicke, J.D., Butler, A.C., & Roediger, H.L. (2009). Metacognitive strategies in student learning: Do students practise retrieval when they study on their own? Memory,
17(4), 471-479. doi:10.1080/09658210802647009.
Karpicke, J.D., & Roediger, H.L. (2008). The critical importance of retrieval for learning. Science, 319(5865), 966-968. doi:10.1126/science.1152408.
Larsen, D.P., Butler, A.C., Lawson, A.L., & Roediger, H.L., III. (2013). The importance of seeing the patient: Test-enhanced learning with standardized patients and written
tests improves clinical application of knowledge. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 18(3), 409-425. doi:10.1007/s10459-012-9379-7.
Lindsey, R.V., Shroyer, J.D., Pashler, H., & Mozer, M.C. (2014). Improving students' long-term knowledge retention through personalized review. Psychological Science,
25(3), 639-647. doi:10.1177/0956797613504302.
Lyle, K.B., & Crawford, N.A. (2011). Retrieving essential material at the end of lectures improves performance on statistics exams. Teaching of Psychology, 38(2), 94-97.
doi:10.1177/0098628311401587.
McDaniel, M.A., Howard, D.C., & Einstein, G.O. (2009). The read-recite-review study strategy: Effective and portable. Psychological Science, 20(4), 516-522.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02325.x.
Novak, J.D. (2013). Concept mapping. In J. Hattie & E.M. Anderman (Eds.), International Guide to Student Achievement (pp. 362-365). New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor &
Francis Group.
Pyc, M.A., & Rawson, K.A. (2010). Why testing improves memory: Mediator effectiveness hypothesis. Science, 330(6002), 335. doi:10.1126/science.1191465.
Roediger, H.L. (1980). Memory metaphors in cognitive psychology. Memory & Cognition, 8(3), 231-246. doi:10.3758/bf03197611.
Roediger, H.L., Agarwal, P.K., McDaniel, M.A., & McDermott, K.B. (2011). Test-enhanced learning in the classroom: Long-term improvements from quizzing. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17(4), 382-395. doi:10.1037/a0026252.
Roediger, H.L., & Karpicke, J.D. (2006a). The power of testing memory: Basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science,
1(3), 181-210. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00012.x.
Roediger, H.L., & Karpicke, J.D. (2006b). Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science, 17(3), 249-255.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01693.x.
Smith, M.A., & Karpicke, J.D. (2014). Retrieval practice with short-answer, multiple-choice, and hybrid tests. Memory, 22(7), 784-802. doi:10.1080/09658211.2013.831454.
The views expressed in this article are those of the author and do not reflect the opinions or policies of APA.
SHARE (https://twitter.com/share?
THIS (#) url=https%3a%2f%2fround-lake.dustinice.workers.dev%3a443%2fhttps%2fwww.apa.org%2fscience%2fabout%2fpsa%2f2016%2f06%2flearning- mem
memory&via=APA&text=A+powerful+way+to+improve+learning+and+memory)
https://www.apa.org/science/about/psa/2016/06/learning-memory 4/6
18/03/2019 A powerful way to improve learning and memory
SUBSCRIBE
Profile (170)
Scholarship/Grant/Award (72)
Newsletter (58)
Review (51)
Journal (35)
Book/Monograph (25)
Video (20)
Curriculum (15)
Brochure/Pamphlet (13)
Magazine (13)
Report (10)
Toolkit (7)
Bibliography (6)
Manual/Handbook (6)
Speech/Transcript/Script (6)
Blog (5)
FAQ (5)
Chart/Table/Figure (4)
Directory (4)
Events (3)
Summary (3)
Bylaws (2)
https://www.apa.org/science/about/psa/2016/06/learning-memory 5/6
18/03/2019 A powerful way to improve learning and memory
Opinion/Editorial (2)
Presentation (2)
Guidelines (1)
Letter (1)
Obituary (1)
Policy/Procedure (1)
Privacy Statement Terms of Use Accessibility Website Feedback Sitemap FOLLOW APA more
https://www.apa.org/science/about/psa/2016/06/learning-memory 6/6