Constitutive Silences
Constitutive Silences
by
Bachelor of Music
University of Houston, 2006
Master of Music
Brigham Young University, 2010
Composition
School of Music
2015
Accepted by:
ii
DEDICATION
To Kristin, with all my heart. We did it.
iii
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
To God, who was with me throughout the creation of this document; to my wife,
who kept our life running smoothly and loved me with great patience; to Dr. Reginald
Bain, for his help in the refinement of this document; to Dr. J. Daniel Jenkins, for
John Fitz Rogers, Dr. Jesse Jones, and Dr. Fang Man, for their support and time; to
Schott Music International, for granting permission to use excerpts from the score in this
iv
ABSTRACT
This document is a study of how Gestalt principles of organization are at work in
“Désordre” (1985), the first etude in the first book of piano etudes by György Ligeti
(1923–2006). After explaining how Gestalt principles can be applied to the analysis of
music, the study presents an analysis of the etude in four main parts. The first part
identifies elements of the composition that help the listener define boundaries between
phrases, phrase groups, and sections. The second part discusses how foreground and
background layers are articulated. The third part discusses the polytempo illusion. The
fourth and final part identifies elements that contribute to large-scale unity in the
composition. Finally, some pedagogical applications for teaching composition are briefly
addressed.
v
TABLE OF CONTENTS
DEDICATION ....................................................................................................................... iii
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS........................................................................................................ iv
ABSTRACT ............................................................................................................................v
vi
CHAPTER 3: A GESTALT-BASED ANALYSIS OF “DÉSORDRE” ..............................................21
BIBLIOGRAPHY ...................................................................................................................48
vii
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure 2.1 Preliminary example of perceptual grouping .....................................................6
Figure 2.4 Comparison of resultant groupings from proximity and similarity ....................9
viii
LIST OF EXAMPLES
Example 2.1 Grouping by proximity to demonstrate the trill threshold ..............................9
Example 2.5 Continuity causes disjunct bass notes to group as a line ..............................20
Example 3.6 Perception of accents as syncopations against a traditional 4/4 meter .........37
Example 3.7 Perception of tempo phasing with elementary pulses eliminated .................37
ix
CHAPTER 1
INTRODUCTION
György Sándor Ligeti was born on May 28, 1923 in Romania, and died on June
12, 2006 in Germany. Between 1985 and 2001, he wrote three books of etudes for piano.
“Désordre” is the first of the six etudes in Book One. It was written in 1985.
During this time period, Ligeti’s music underwent dramatic stylistic changes.
influences, including African music, the player piano studies of Conlon Nancarrow, and
the music of American jazz artists Thelonius Monk and Bill Evans.1 Perhaps the most
influential source of inspiration for this particular etude was sub-Saharan African Banda
Linda music. Polyrhythm and a complex balance between order and disorder characterize
both Ligeti’s etude and this African music. In the foreword to Simha Arom’s book
Undoubtedly my interest in the music Arom has recorded stems also from
the proximity I feel exists between it and my own way of thinking with
regards to composition: that is, the creation of structures which are both
remarkably simple and highly complex. The formal simplicity of sub-
Saharan African music with its unchanging repetition of periods of equal
length, like the uniform pearls of a necklace, is in sharp contrast to the
inner structure of these periods which, because of simultaneous
superpositioning of different rhythmic patterns, possesses an extraordinary
degree of complexity… What we can witness in this music is a wonderful
combination of order and disorder, which in turn merges together
producing a sense of order on a higher level.2
1
György Ligeti, liner notes to György Ligeti, Edition 3: Works for Piano (Etudes and Musica
Ricercata), Sony Classical (DDD 62309), 1997, Pierre-Laurent Aimard, piano.
2
Simha Arom, African Polyphony and Polyrhythm: Music Structure and Methodology (Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press, 1991), foreword by Ligeti, xvii.
1
The analysis of this etude is based upon principles of perception as found in
Gestalt theory. Chapter 2 introduces the reader to these principles and recounts their early
understand the principles at work in the author’s analysis. As such, Chapter 2 introduces
common region, and continuity. Visual and musical examples demonstrate how these
The Chapter 3 analysis identifies which Gestalt principles are involved in the
“Désordre.” After a brief discussion of the form, the analysis is presented in four main
parts. The first part identifies elements of the composition that help the listener perceive
boundaries between phrases, phrase groups, and sections. The second discusses which
elements articulate foreground and background layers. The third shows which elements
make the perception of polytempo possible. The fourth addresses elements that contribute
Meyer’s Emotion and Meaning in Music3 and Eugene Narmour’s The Analysis and
Cognition of Basic Melodic Structures.4 This study is unique, however, in its application
of a Gestalt-based approach to the etude as a whole. John Sloboda confirms the need for
3
Leonard B. Meyer, Emotion and Meaning in Music (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1956).
4
Eugene Narmour, The Analysis and Cognition of Basic Melodic Structures: The Implication-
realization Model (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1990).
2
Cognitive psychology has...achieved its greatest successes in advancing
the understanding of processes that span seconds rather than minutes or
hours. For instance, a great deal is known about how human beings
process words and a lot about how they process sentences, but almost
nothing about how they process extended discourse, as found in books or
plays. Similarly, music research has yielded immense dividends at the
level of notes, chords and phrases, but very little at the level of complete
works.5
It is also important to mention that the author has benefited from a large number of
(1992), Tsong (2001), and especially Haapamaki (2012) and Uranker (1998). It is the
author’s hope that the perceptual approach taken here will contribute to the understanding
of Ligeti’s etude.
5
In Diana Deutsch, et al., “Psychology of Music,” Grove Music Online, Oxford Music Online, Oxford
University Press, accessed February 4, 2015,
http://www.oxfordmusiconline.com/subscriber/article/grove/music/42574pg1.
3
CHAPTER 2
articulation, common region, and continuity. (Other Gestalt principles are introduced in
Chapter 3.) Part of this introduction is dedicated to explaining the role of subliminal
Gestalt theory began in the early 20th century as a formal branch of psychology.
Its origins, however, may be traced back to the work of 19th-century psychologists Carl
Stumpf and Christian von Ehrenfels.6 In 1883, Carl Stumpf published a theory about the
relationship between the parts of a whole and the whole in his book Tonpsychologie.7
into a single impression….”8 He noticed that there was something intrinsically different
about the perception of a dyad. Ehrenfels took Stumpf’s theory a step further with his
investigations into why the mind was still able to recognize a melody after it was
6
Stephanie Sabar, “What’s a Gestalt?” Gestalt Review 17, no. 1 (2013): 7, accessed March 11, 2015,
http://www.gisc.org/gestaltreview/documents/whatsagestalt.pdf.
7
Carl Stumpf, Tonpsychologie, 2 vols. (Leipzig: Hierzel, 1883–90).
8
Deutsch, et al., “Psychology of Music.”
4
transposed to a new key.9 In 1938, Ehrenfels’s student Max Wertheimer, described the
initial problem:
Ehrenfels concluded that something about the relationships between the tones of a
melody remained the same upon transposition. Because of these relationships (i.e., the
intervals between the pitches), the melody has a complex identity, which the mind can
remember even after transposition. Conversely, the tones of the same melody presented
in a different order would take on a new identity because of the altered relationships
between the pitches. In the first few decades of the 20th century, Ehrenfels’s students
Wolfgang Köhler and Kurt Koffka developed his theories further. Their work eventually
led to the formal acceptance of Gestalt theories into the discipline of psychology.
organization” that seem to describe the ways that objects are perceived.11 The principles,
or laws as the founders called them,12 correlate to “rules of the organization of perceptual
senses.”13 Stephanie Sabar describes how these principles affect our perception of the
visual world. Sabar explains that the founders of Gestalt theory learned that:
9
Christian von Ehrenfels, Über Gestaltqualitäten [On Gestalt Qualities] (Leipzig: Reisland, 1890),
trans. Barry Smith in Foundations of Gestalt Theory (Munich: Philosophia, 1988), 83–85.
10
Max Wertheimer, “Gestalt Theory,” in Source Book of Gestalt Psychology, ed. Willis D. Ellis (New
York: Harcourt, Brace, 1938), 4.
11
Max Wertheimer, “Principles of Perceptual Organization,” in Readings in Perception, ed. David
Beardslee and Michael Wertheimer (Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand, 1958), 115–35.
12
See Meyer, Emotion and Meaning in Music; Narmour, Basic Melodic Structures; and Narmour, The
Analysis and Cognition of Melodic Complexity: The Implication-realization Model (Chicago: University of
Chicago Press, 1992).
13
Dejan Todorovic, “Gestalt Principles,” Scholarpedia.org, 2008, accessed March 11, 2015,
doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.5345.
5
…our eyes are not like a camera or a window. We do not see the world
objectively. Rather, what we see is interpreted and given meaning by the
observer….”14
The founders discovered that “perception involved more than just the stimuli that entered
the eye.”15 When perceived, the stimuli become something more, or different as Köhler
famously worded it: “The whole is different than the sum of the parts.”16 According to
Gestalt theory, the mind groups stimuli into what Ehrenfels called Gestalts (Gestalten),17
or percepts of higher significance than the parts that caused them to form. The German
word Gestalt means form or shape. When writing about Gestalt theory, English writers
typically substitute the word “group” for Gestalt as the author does in this study.
2.1, letters of like case (upper vs. lower) tend to be grouped together. This is grouping by
G X a Q S B V e L F C
Other letters in the figure tend to group because of their proximity. The lowercase letters
seem to act as spatial dividers between the uppercase letters, causing the following three
14
Sabar, “What’s a Gestalt?”, 8.
15
Russell A. Dewey, “Psychology: An Introduction,” chap. 4, part 1, in Psych Web (psywww.com),
accessed March 11, 2015, http://www.intropsych.com/.
16
Wolfgang Köhler, The Task of Gestalt Psychology, intro. Carroll C. Pratt (Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press, 1969) 9–10. This quote is commonly mistranslated as “The whole is greater than the sum
of the parts.” According to Pratt, Köhler disliked this translation.
17
Ehrenfels, Gestaltqualitäten, various pages. Note that Gestalten is the German plural form of Gestalt.
6
Similarity and proximity are two important principles of perception. Many other
principles have been discovered and named. Others have yet to be discovered.18
Groupings can be explained using the principles of similarity and proximity. These
principles are similarly applicable to the perception of music. For example, Alf
pertain to vision, and then as they pertain to musical experience. These are the main
2.2 Proximity
The principle of proximity states that elements tend to be grouped if they are near
to each other.20 In Figure 2.2, the dots nearest to one another tend to group, while others,
In general, the first group the mind forms upon seeing stimuli is what Wertheimer calls
the “natural grouping.”22 Different groupings may be forced into perception, but:
18
See Stephen E. Palmer, “Common Region: A New Principle of Perceptual Grouping,” Cognitive
Psychology 24 (July 1992): 436–47.
19
In Deutsch, et al., “Psychology of Music.”
20
See Todorovic, “Gestalt Principles,” and Palmer, Vision Science, 257–58.
21
The visual examples in this chapter are often based on the examples in Wertheimer’s “Laws of
Organization.”
22
Wertheimer, “Laws of Organization in Perceptual Forms,” In A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology,
ed. Willis D. Ellis, 71–88 (New York: Harcourt, Brace, 1938), accessed on March 10, 2015,
http://psychclassics.asu.edu/Wertheimer/Forms/forms.htm.
7
…this cannot be maintained for long. The natural grouping soon returns as
an overpowering “upset” of the artificial arrangement.23
For example, in Figure 2.3, the natural grouping tends to yield a perception of four
columns of dots, but four rows of dots can be forced into focus temporarily.
principle of proximity. For example, a note of a melody that follows another by stepwise
motion tends to group with it. As documented in the work of George Miller and George
Heise,24 the greater the distance between the tones, the greater the tendency to hear more
than one line. Scott Lipscomb reports on their findings: “When two pitches are far
enough apart, fission (a perceptual splitting apart) occurs. Likewise, when two pitches are
close together, they tend to fuse.”25 The author-composed melody in Example 2.1
demonstrates the effect of the so-called trill threshold.26 The melody begins with conjunct
motion. Starting in m. 5, with the leap of a P4, it widens into two separate strands. By the
end of m. 10, when the intervals are again a third or smaller, its strands have merged back
together.
23
Wertheimer, “Laws of Organization.”
24
George A. Miller and George A. Heise, “The Trill Threshold,” Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America 22 (September 1950), 637–38.
25
Scott D. Lipscomb, “The Cognitive Organization of Musical Sound,” in Handbook of Music
Psychology, ed. Donald A. Hodges (San Antonio, TX: IMR Press, 1996), 148.
26
The musical examples in this chapter were composed by the author for this paper.
8
Example 2.1 Grouping by proximity to demonstrate the trill threshold.
2.4 Similarity
The principle of similarity states that elements tend to be grouped if they are
similar to each other.27 The dots in Figure 2.4a28 are homogenously colored and spaced
a b c d e
In 2.4b where the spacing remains the same, the similar coloring between certain dots
yields a grouping into columns. In 2.4c, proximity yields a natural grouping into three
rows.29 This same spacing in 2.4d, however, is less forceful. Here, the principles of
similarity and proximity compete against one another, yielding two possible natural
groupings between which the mind is able to switch back and forth with relative ease. In
2.4e, the principles work together to produce a natural grouping into rows. The figure
27
See Todorovic, “Gestalt Principles,” and Palmer, Vision Science, 258.
28
The identifying letters a, b, c, d, and e are purposefully off center to avoid affecting the grouping.
29
When the eye traces from 2.2b to 2.2c, the grouping from the first may temporarily affect the
grouping in the second.
9
demonstrates how certain groupings can be made to dominate when two or more
In A Generative Theory of Tonal Music (1983), Fred Lerdahl and Ray Jackendoff further
explain that similarity groupings made over the course of an entire piece form
Initially, local patterns (e.g., motives and themes) are identified, then they
are placed hierarchically within the context of the entire composition.32
Motives are perceived as gaining complexity as the composer develops them. Example
a a a'
a a a'''
10
Segments marked a indicate the motive that undergoes development in the example.
Because of transpositional and durational differences, motive a' shares close but inexact
similarity with motive a. Motive a'' adds another degree of variation through interval
expansion. Motive a''' varies the pitch content for yet another degree of variation.
Motives a', a'', and a''' all relate to motive a through close but inexact similarities. These
slight variations enable grouping processes to form complex relationships between the
motives. Each new variation of the original adds a level of complexity to the group.
Figure 2.5 represents the process (as it happens in time) by which motive a gains
complexity.
Tracing from left to right in the figure shows the progression from motive a through a'''.
The arrows reflect how grouping processes continually compare new material with
material heard earlier in order to determine the level of similarity they share. When
variations are similar enough to the original, they can be grouped with it. In the melody
of Example 2.2, each variation of the motive is compared with the original. As Figure 2.5
shows, the variations are also compared with each other. Each variation adds a level of
complexity to the preceding motive. By the time the melody ends, three levels of
complexity have been added to motive a, two to a', and one to a''.
11
2.6 Subliminal Perceptions and Large-scale Grouping
The melodic analysis of Example 2.2 demonstrates how complex groupings can
form over large spans of time. Some of these groupings may even occur subliminally
without a listener being able to pinpoint exactly which structural elements are responsible
for the perception. Stephen Palmer, in his book Vision Science,33 addresses the issue of
Palmer supports this theory by documenting scientific research that provides strong
subliminal grouping.
33
Stephen E. Palmer, Vision Science: Photons to Phenomenology (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1999).
34
Ibid., 639. Palmer clarified the difference between the words subliminal and subconscious with the
following explanation: “The term ‘subliminal’ is derived from the prefix sub, meaning ‘below,’ and the
root limen, meaning ‘threshold’.”
35
Ibid., 639–44.
12
In this logo, grouping immediately occurs between highly similar elements (e.g., triangles
with triangles and circles with circles). Grouping also occurs because of two other factors
that may be less obvious, and are perhaps only noticeable after a few seconds of scrutiny.
Cohesion of the whole is strengthened by the arrangement of sun symbols into a circle.
This larger circle groups by similarity with each smaller circle in the sun symbols. The
second somewhat hidden ingredient of cohesion has to do with the central octagon in the
word Son. While the surrounding sun symbols form a circle, the interior angles created by
their connecting triangles form a large octagon. The smaller octagon in the word groups
by similarity with the larger, slightly hidden octagon, giving the name of the company a
second sense of cohesion to the figures surrounding it (the first being the triangles that
form the letters S and n). The observer may not consciously notice the connection
between the octagon and the surrounding symbols, and yet, because of the grouping
influence of repetitious elements, the observer’s mind may notice it anyway and the small
1982 article “Pattern, Poetry, and Power in the Music of Frederic Chopin,” Douglas
36
Douglas R. Hofstadter, Metamagical Themas: Questing for the Essence of Mind and Pattern (New
York: Basic Books, 1985): 177–78. The article “Pattern, Poetry, and Power” is reprinted in the book.
13
The development of motivic complexity described in the discussion of Example 2.2, or
the groupings discussed in the Figure 2.6 logo, may not all be conscious perceptions. Yet,
the ease with which listeners and observers form groups when presented with some
degree of repetition suggests that they do not need to consciously notice each degree of
detect every motivic variation in the first movement of Beethoven’s Symphony no. 5 to
be able to walk away from a performance of it having felt that its structure had a strong
sense of unity.
element is large and homogenous enough to be perceived as distinct from the smaller
element.38 This is demonstrated in Figure 2.7, where three objects (figures) seem to lie
upon a ground (the gray rectangular area). Dejan Todorovic describes the phenomenal
37
Wertheimer, “Laws of Organization.” Brackets in original.
38
Palmer names three additional requirements for the perception of figures upon grounds, but they do
not appear to be immediately applicable to this analysis. See Palmer, Vision Science, 281–83.
14
Figure 2.7. Grouping by figure/ground articulation.
The areas of the figure and the ground usually do not appear juxtaposed in
a common plane, as in a mosaic, but rather as stratified in depth: there is a
tendency to see the figure as positioned in front, and the ground at a
further depth plane and continuing to extend behind the figure, as if
occluded by it. Furthermore, the border separating the two segments is
perceived as belonging to the figure rather than to the ground, and as
delineating the figure’s shape as its contour, whereas it is irrelevant to the
shape of the ground.39
In other words, the natural grouping suggests the figures are sitting on top of the gray
area rather than suggesting they are holes or cutouts exposing a darker background.
Example 2.3 demonstrates how, if a melody takes on enough of the attributes of its
39
Todorovic, “Gestalt Principles.”
40
Lipscomb, “Cognitive Organization,” 145–46.
15
Example 2.3 Melodic assimilation into the accompaniment.
This brief analysis of Example 2.3 will make it apparent that the principle of
figures.
In the example above, the part played by the right hand (RH) begins by presenting
a melody (figure) over a drone (ground) in the left hand (LH). To make this distinction,
durational and registral differences work together to distinguish the separate layers. Two
layers are perceived whose respective elements have been grouped by similarity.
melody progresses. Measures 4 and 10 present situations in which the melody takes on
the characteristics of its accompaniment, thereby fusing with it. First, the melody
16
proximity. Then, it sheds its rhythmic distinctions and adopts those of the
melody, in effect, transforms into the accompaniment and becomes part of it.
and fully assimilates as part of the accompaniment by the second beat of m. 5. At the
transition from mm. 5–6, the LH accompanimental material eases into the character of a
^ ^
melody through a stepwise 7 to 1 resolution. From mm. 6–9, the LH material takes on all
of the characteristics of the previous RH melody. At this point, the roles have reversed.
discovered by Palmer in 1992,41 states that elements within a closed region of space tend
41
See Palmer, “Common Region,” 436–47.
17
to be grouped together.42 In Figure 2.8a, the dots that are set closest together tend to
group (proximity), i.e., the second with the third and the fourth with the fifth, leaving the
a b
In 2.8b, because of common regions delineated by rectangular boundaries, the dots group
into three sets of two, even though proximity relationships remain the same from 2.8a
to 2.8b.
perceive boundaries. One way to create a boundary in music is to replace the pattern a
listener is experiencing with a new one. When this happens, the listener perceives a
boundary at the seam. According to the principle of common region, musical elements
preceding the boundary tend to be perceived as belonging together. These elements group
by similarity because they share the same pattern and because they fall within the same
boundary. Earlier in this chapter, Sloboda was quoted as having said: “Without the ability
to recognize similarity between elements within a piece, the apprehension of form would
be impossible.”43 It should be added that the perception of musical boundaries would not
be possible without the ability to recognize dissimilarity between elements. The ability to
notice dissimilarity makes the perception of boundaries possible. This concept has
42
Palmer, Vision Science, 260.
43
In Deutsch, et al., “Psychology of Music.”
18
significant applications in the next chapter’s analysis when it will be shown that Ligeti
disrupts these grouping processes by blurring the edges of a significant boundary between
sections.
2.11 Continuity
The principle of continuity, or good continuation,44 states that elements that can
example, Figure 2.9a tends to group as two intersecting lines. The natural grouping is not
likely to be seen as two right angles whose vertices meet, as shown in 2.9b.
a b
grouping by continuity that creates the perception of a single line. In reality, the notes are
not connected at all. They are simply articulated moments in time. In fact, melodies, or
musical lines, are often disjunct arpeggiations, moving across intervals of 3rds and 4ths,
and as is often the case in bass lines, across 5ths or even 8ves. And yet, their individual
notes are typically perceived as continuations of each other. Example 2.5 demonstrates a
44
See Wertheimer, “Laws of Organization.”
45
Palmer, Vision Science, 259.
19
Example 2.5 Continuity causes disjunct bass notes to group as a line.
In his book Explaining Music, Leonard B. Meyer uses the word criticism
synonymously with analysis, writing that “criticism attempts to understand and explain
introduced in this chapter. The Chapter 3 analysis will apply these Gestalt principles to
Ligeti’s “Désordre.”
46
Leonard B. Meyer, Explaining Music: Essays and Explorations (Berkeley: University of California
Press), 1973: 16–17.
20
CHAPTER 3
four parts. The first part identifies elements of the composition that help the listener
define boundaries between phrases, phrase groups, and sections. The second discusses
how foreground and background layers are articulated. The third discusses issues of
Ligeti said:
In the analysis that follows, the author uses the term “perceived tempo” to refer to these
individual layers of different tempos. This term will be used extensively in the formal
47
Ligeti, Erato.
21
5
13
13
17
17
*) Use the pedal sparingly throughout. Play the melody legato in both hands.
22
Circles in the example indicate beginnings of phrases. Three phrases in a particular hand
constitute a phrase group. The process of repeating and transposing the two (LH and RH)
important to notice that the original patterns shown in Example 3.1 consist of RH
material that is presented exclusively on the white keys against LH material that is
throughout the etude (except for the last note) and is one of the many special features of
“Désordre.” Figure 3.1 provides a diagram of the etude’s large-scale form to serve as a
Analysts seem to generally agree that the etude is divided into three main
sections.48 For example, Mayron K. Tsong writes that the etude “follows a basic ternary
structural pattern of A B A'.”49 Thus Figure 3.1 divides into three sections labeled: I, II,
and III, respectively. Because of Ligeti’s misalignment of barlines in the score (e.g., see
Example 3.1, RH m. 4), the vertical pitch axis divides into RH (top) and LH (bottom)
measures. The range of the pitch axis is C1–C8, where C4 is middle C. A horizontal
dotted-line grid marks the location of each C: i.e., C1, C2, C3, etc. The dots in the figure
indicate the beginnings of phrases. Line segments extending from these dots indicate the
approximate phrase lengths. Phrase groups are indicated using rectangular boxes and
each phrase group is numbered. Because the phrase-group lengths differ in each hand,
48
See Sampo Elias Haapamaki, “Order in ‘Désordre’: Rhythmic and Melodic Structure in György
Ligeti’s Piano Etude No. 1,” DMA diss. (Columbia University, 2012), 5; Mayron K. Tsong, “Études pour
piano, premier livre of György Ligeti: Studies in Composition and Pianism,” MM thesis (Rice University,
2001), 24; and Mark Uranker, “Ligeti’s ‘Désordre’ from Études pour piano—premier livre: An Analysis
and Performance Guide,” MM thesis (California State University, Long Beach, 1998), 27.
49
Tsong, “Études pour piano,” 24.
23
24
related to the original pattern (Example 3.1) by diatonic transposition Tn, where n is an
integer representing the transposition distance and direction, measured in steps along
respective white-key and black-key collections, from the original pattern. For example, in
the RH, T0 represents the original white-key pattern beginning on B4, T1 represents the
pattern beginning on C5, T2 represents the pattern on D5, and so on. In the LH, T0
represents the original black-key pattern beginning on D#4, T-2 represents the pattern
beginning on A#3, T-4 represents the same pattern beginning on F#3, and so on. The lines
connecting the dots in Figure 3.1 are designed to visually highlight and track the
movement of the RH and LH pitch materials over time. Figure 3.1 makes clear certain
overall trends: e.g., the large-scale pitch ascent in the RH, and the large-scale descent in
Table 3.1 provides a formal overview and a bullet-point summary of the most
important aurally perceivable characteristics in each section of the etude. The table also
with these characteristics will be helpful in the sections that follow. Like Figure 3.1, this
table is designed to serve as an overview and guide for the analytical discussions that
follow.
processes and enable the listener to distinguish between the following three components
of formal delineation: (1) phrases, (2) phrase groups, and (3) sections.
25
Table 3.1 Aurally Perceivable Characteristics in “Désordre”
The etude’s phrases are designed in a way that makes their boundaries clear to the
listener. The phrase boundaries in “Désordre” are defined in two ways: header repetitions
and ending elongations. Header repetitions, the author’s term, mark the beginnings of
phrases. Example 3.2 shows the opening eight measures of the etude with header
26
5
Rhythms that occur at the end of each phrase also stand out in the texture and help
introduce the header repetitions. In mm. 4 and 8, Ligeti lengthens the final melodic
duration of the phrase in both hands to fill the measure, as circled in the example. This
elongation helps signal the end of the phrase, and functions as a kind of musical breath
mark. When this breath precedes a header repetition, it allows the repetition to stand out
of the texture. Header repetitions and ending elongations work together to define phrase
boundaries that trigger elements within a phrase to group by common region (section
Because header repetitions and ending elongations occur in every phrase, in order to
27
make a distinction between phrases and phrase groups, additional elements must be
added to delineate a more complex group to occur every three phrases. Ligeti does this by
lengthening every third phrase, thus delaying the header repetition that follows.
Throughout the etude, every third phrase is lengthened in this way, establishing a large-
scale pattern that affects the grouping structure, if only subliminally. As shown in section
2.6, highly similar elements are easily detected by the mind’s subliminal processes.50
The sequencing ensues without any exception and, taken for itself,
together with the corresponding rhythmical procedures, is easily
perceivable….51
Just as changes are needed to distinguish phrase groups from phrases, changes are
needed to distinguish sections from phrase groups. In Figure 3.1, perceived “tempo
changes” may be seen by comparing the width of the rectangular boxes that identify the
phrase groups. In Section II, phrase groups progress at a faster rate than in the outer
sections (I and III). It is not that the phrases in Section II have fewer melodic notes;
rather, the space between them has been shortened, as it would be with standard tempo
changes. However, perceived tempo changes do not suffice as a means to delineate the
boundaries between the sections. If one takes a close look at the Section I phrase lengths
as they approach the boundary of Section II, it is apparent that the phrase lengths slowly
approach those found in Section II. Therefore, perceiving the boundary between Sections
I and II based on these tempo changes alone can be difficult. This gradual speeding up of
50
See Palmer, Vision Science, 258.
51
Hartmuth Kinzler, “György Ligeti: Decision and Automatism in ‘Désordre,’ 1re étude, premier
livre,” Interface 20 (1991): 102.
28
the texture subtly hides the section boundary in a manner not unlike camouflage.52 At this
moment in the etude, Ligeti uses the principles of similarity and continuity as cloaking
devices: that is, to deflect grouping processes, hiding much of the dissimilarity between
the sections that would otherwise suggest a boundary to a listener. Ligeti does this by
effecting a perceived accelerando. In Transitions 1 and 2 (Table 3.1), Ligeti contracts the
space between melodic notes little by little in a way that makes them appear to speed up.
In the Ending (Table 3.1), he does the opposite in the LH, creating the illusion of a
ritardando where none is marked in the score. Example 3.3 shows the perceived
Note that Ligeti has placed accents on each melodic note to bring them out of the texture.
The melody in Example 3.3b is a transposition (T3) of the original shown in 3.3a. All of
the notes of the original melody are present in the transposition. The differences between
52
See Palmer, Vision Science, 261, for an explanation of how camouflage “foils” grouping processes.
29
the melodies have to do with the number of eighth notes between the accents. In every
case, there are fewer eighths between accents in 3.3b than in 3.3a. And yet, the rhythms
in 3.3b do not seem altered, only accelerated. Ligeti achieves this effect of an apparent
accelerando by contracting the space between accents in a way that maintains the
rhythmic proportions from the original. In other words, every instance of a short-long or
long-short rhythm in 3.3a is kept in 3.3b, as shown in the example. When the melody in
3.3b finishes 8 eighth notes sooner than its original, it appears to be faster.
process described above, a listener may hear Sections I and II as a contiguous group
No such devices conceal the boundary between Sections II and III. In fact, just the
opposite is true. Four powerful compositional devices work together to ensure that this
boundary will be perceived. This moment in the music is shown in Example 3.4. The first
compositional device is an abrupt shift in perceived tempo. The fast pace of Section II
ends abruptly at the arrival of the RH’s m. 99. It then reverts to the perceived tempo that
had been present in most of Section I. The second device is an abrupt change in the LH’s
register. At the climax that overlaps the LH’s mm. 96 and 97, the LH’s descent to the
bottom of the keyboard strikes the lowest black key at the triple sforzando, then rests for
a few eighth notes as the pianist shifts the hand up four octaves to resume its normal
activity. The third device is textural. At RH m. 99 where the music reaches a second
climactic triple sforzando, Ligeti gives us a chord containing four pitches. Up until that
point, no more than two pitches (excluding octave doublings) have been heard in a single
30
93
91
99
97
RH chord. The fourth and final device is the discontinuation of the octave doubling of the
two melodies that has been present since m. 1, as shown in Example 3.4. Instead, Ligeti
employs other intervals in a coloristic manner. This change in the texture combines with
the force of the first three devices (abrupt change in the perceived tempo, registral
changes, and the introduction of chords) to suggest that a new section has arrived.
The Gestalt principle at work in the delineation of the boundary in Example 3.4 is
similarity (section 2.5). However, it is important to keep in mind that while each section
has a certain degree of similarity within itself, the perception of boundaries between
31
The presence of musical boundaries helps articulate common regions and delineate which
items belong to which section. Thus, the principles of similarity and common region can
explain how a listener perceives musical elements as belonging to phrases, phrase groups,
or sections.
…[the] basic sound elements in this composition are (1) two individual
loud, accented and irregular melodic lines…in the forte foreground and (2)
soft and regular eighth-note runs in the piano background.53
Both authors seem to take for granted that the distinguishing characteristic is the
dynamics. But other issues come into play as well, and these, together with the forte-to-
piano differences, work to support a common objective. Kinzler touched upon one called
the “octaves plane.” Doubling the two melodies at the octave reinforces the dynamic
markings, which are further reinforced by the notated accents. Still more factors are at
work. The eighth-note ascents, Ligeti’s “elementary pulses,” are fast and quasi-robotic in
their ever-ascending stepwise motions.55 The two melodies are slow and singable; Tsong
says they are “reminiscent of a Hungarian folk melody.”56 The contrast between the two
53
Haapamaki, “Order in ‘Désordre’,” 6. Italics in the original.
54
Kinzler, “Decision and Automatism,” 91.
55
Ligeti, liner notes to Ligeti: Piano Etudes, Book 1, Erato (ECD 75555), 1990, Pierre-Laurent Aimard,
piano.
56
Tsong, “Études pour piano,” 24.
32
melodies and the eighth-note ascents is apparent from the outset as can be seen in
Example 3.1.
ground. The use of accents, octave doublings, dynamic contrasts, and the decision to
make the two melodies slow and singable over quick and mechanical eighth-note ascents
delineates the edges of the melodic figure and distinguish it from its accompanimental
ground. Ligeti could have made this distinction by fewer means, but doing so may have
complicated the task of perceiving these melodic edges. He chose, instead, to take
advantage of multiple methods to serve the same purpose. The tactic to employ many
techniques to achieve the same goal was something Ligeti made use of when delineating
the boundary between Sections II and III, as described earlier in this chapter.
The figure resembles Figure 2.7, which was used to explain the principle of figure/ground
articulation. However, in Figure 3.2, the objects previously surrounded by the gray area
are now so large that they almost cover it. And yet, even though the gray area does not
33
surround them, it still appears to be behind them. The principle of figure/ground
articulation, which requires one area to surround another, no longer determines the
grouping in this instance. Rather, the active principle is one not addressed in Chapter 2,
that of occlusion.57 If an object appears to occlude part of another, the first tends to be
foregrounded. During Transition 2 (Table 3.1), Ligeti causes the figures (melodic
accents) to occlude nearly all of the ground (eighth-note ascents). Example 3.5 compares
the music a few measures before Transition 1 begins (LH mm. 37–40) with the music
after the transition ends (LH mm. 54–57), and then with the tail end of Transition 2 when
layer:
An ever present foreground plane is contrasted by a background plane,
which—in changing proportion—gradually disappears and suddenly
comes back in the middle of the piece.58
At the start of Section III, the relationship between figures and their ground return to
what they had been at the start of the etude, and perception is again influenced by
figure/ground articulation.
one. Aside from the tempo specified in the score (63 whole-notes per minute—76 in
Ligeti’s original), the two most obvious active tempo layers in “Désordre” are: (1) the
perceived tempo of the RH; and (2) the perceived tempo of the LH. The following
discussion will attempt to explain why each layer may legitimately be heard as a different
tempo.
57
Palmer, Vision Science, 236.
58
Kinzler, “Decision and Automatism,” 122.
34
38
37
VS.
56
54
VS.
93
91
György Ligeti ÉTUDES POUR PIANO No. 1 “Désordre,” LH mm. 37–40, 54–57, and 91–96
Copyright © 1986 by Schott Music GmbH & Co. KG
All Rights Reserved.
Used by permission of European American Music Distributors Company,
sole U.S. and Canadian Agent for Schott Music GmbH & Co. KG
the mind can sometimes be tricked. For example, the phenomenon of apparent motion
can convince the mind that a stationary object is in motion.59 Vection, or as Palmer calls
it, “induced self-motion,”60 can convince a person that their body is in motion when it
59
Palmer, Vision Science, 468.
60
Ibid., 504–05.
35
actually is not. In order for these phenomena to take effect, the mind must receive the
same stimuli it would receive if those experiences were real. Later in this chapter, the
discussion will show that the correct stimuli for the perception of polytempo are present
in the etude.
proper performance of the etude if they focus exclusively on these stratified perceived
tempo layers. For example, Uranker explains that pianists should focus their attention on
the eighth-note background layer.61 When a pianist focuses on this layer, Uranker says
gives the opening two melodies as notated using 4/4 measures. Notice how the LH’s
rhythms in these measures all fit into a 4/4 framework, enabling the performer to keep a
steady beat. A listener, on the other hand, has the option of ignoring the background
eighth-note pulse through selective hearing. Example 3.7 shows this approach. The
numbers between the staves indicate the offset interval in eighth notes. The example
places a barline before every note in the two melodies to better visualize the offset.
Notice that a distinct phasing becomes apparent. Because the phasing takes place at such
a slow pace, a listener will have little trouble gauging the distance between offset accents.
The LH will sound as though it lags behind the RH by greater and greater durations as
61
Uranker, “Ligeti’s ‘Désordre’,” 31.
62
Ibid.
36
Example 3.6 Perception of accents as syncopations against a traditional 4/4 meter.
37
the music progresses.63 Kinzler writes:
By the time the phasing has brought the LH behind by three eighth notes, the phasing
becomes difficult to track. As the LH falls further and further behind, Ligeti explains:
Example 3.8 shows a moment in Section I when the two melodies are so far apart
21
-5 -6
-5 -5 -5
21
Arrows indicate accents that may cause a listener to lose track of the offset distance. For
example, the first accent in LH m. 22 has fallen behind the first accent in RH m. 22 by a
63
Ligeti, “On My Études,” 7.
64
Kinzler, “Decision and Automatism,” 110.
65
Ligeti, “On My Études,” 7.
38
distance of five eighth notes. However, because the last accent of LH m. 21 occurs after
the first accent of RH m. 22, the LH accent in m. 21 will probably appear to be the accent
that has fallen behind. The alignment of LH and RH accents at RH m. 24 may even
At the beginning of the etude, the two melodies are easy to track, even when out
of synchronization. At times, they sound as if they are in canon with each other.66 This
seems to be an intentional decision to ensure the listener has time to become accustomed
to the necessity of tracking the offset between the two melodies. At one point in
Uranker’s analysis, he turned to the perspective of the listener and commented that:
…if the melody were more disparate and less tautly constructed, the point
of the phasing might be lost on the listener.67
There are at least four Gestalt principles that contribute to the perception of
synchrony. The principle of synchrony was not introduced in Chapter 2, but will be
defined in a moment.68
First, similarity between the rhythms of the two competing melodies helps the
listener decide which notes should be heard as lagging behind others. For example, the
melody in the LH, but slightly offset in time. Similarities between the two melodies are
66
See Haapamaki, “Order in ‘Désordre’,” 21, for a supporting discussion of canon-like qualities in the
melodies of “Désordre.”
67
Uranker, “Ligeti’s ‘Désordre’,” 24.
68
Palmer, Vision Science, 259–60.
39
melodies differ from one hand to the other. This is discussed in detail by Uranker69 and
Haapamaki.70
Similarities are also present in the contours of the two melodies. Haapamaki
further comments that they appear to be virtual copies of one another, but stated in
different scalar collections, i.e., black-keys vs. white-keys. Thus, the melodies’
similarities in both contour and rhythm will most likely convince the listener that the
lines progress in canon with each other. Because of this, the mental task of tracking the
compare the perceived tempo differences. As Example 3.8 demonstrates, the task of
tracking the offset is considerably more difficult when the offset is large, especially
because intermittent accents begin to compete. When the offset is only a factor of one or
enable the perception of polytempo, it is necessary in this etude. Without it, the two
melodies would likely be heard as irregular accents in reference to the eighth-note scalar
ascents, which, according to Uranker, is how performers will probably have to perceive
them.71 Without their “edges,” the melodies could not be perceived as objects that one
could track. Rather, they would assimilate into the homogeneity of the background
accompaniment.
69
Uranker, “Ligeti’s ‘Désordre’,” 9–12.
70
Haapamaki, “Order in ‘Désordre’,” 21.
71
Uranker, “Ligeti’s ‘Désordre’,” 31.
40
Fourth and finally, the principle of synchrony states that elements occurring at the
same time tend to group. Because the two melodies begin in synchronization, the first
instance of asynchrony (m. 5 where the barlines reflect this misalignment) is unsettling.
A listener may expect (and even hope for) the synchronization to return. This desire
could be thought of as a kind of magnetism. As the two melodies draw farther and farther
apart, the magnetism will resist the direction of separation and try to hold them together,
which can help a listener keep track of the offset. After a certain amount of time, Ligeti
systematically increases the distance of separation, at which point the offset can no longer
be tracked and the magnetism dissolves. But later, the magnetism returns when the
accents grow nearer again. When this happens, a listener may experience the expectation
that the synchronization will return and a certain degree of satisfaction will probably be
In sum, in order to ensure that the polytempo illusion is heard by the listener,
Ligeti made use of at least four perceptual tools. (1) He wrote two melodies that are
nearly exact replicas of one another. The contrast between them is mainly a result of their
differing scalar collections (white-key vs. black-key). By doing this, Ligeti maintained
contour and rhythm without oversaturating the texture with too much repetition.73 (2) The
phasing moves slow enough that a listener can track the offset. (3) Ligeti doubled each
melodic note at the octave, reinforced the doublings with accents and dynamics, and
included drastic contrasts between melody and accompaniment, ensuring that the “edges”
72
This bears a certain relation to Joseph Straus’s ideas of “pattern completion” in Stravinksy’s music.
See Joseph Straus, “A Principle of Voice Leading in the Music of Stravinsky,” Music Theory Spectrum 4
(Spring 1982), 106–24.
73
See Meyer, Emotion and Meaning in Music, 135–38, for a discussion of saturation and expectation in
music.
41
could be perceived to distinguish his figures from his grounds. And finally (4), through
the principle of synchrony, he created a magnetism that holds the phasing together for as
long as possible.
elements across large spans of music and even across entire compositions. To conclude
the analytical portion of this study, three main elements will be discussed that contribute
As shown in Example 3.1, once mm. 1–20 establish the original phrase-group
pattern, this pattern is maintained throughout the etude, changing only in terms of rhythm
and transposition. The constant presence of the original pattern acts as a cohesive element
Three elements of the texture remain constant throughout the composition. First,
the eighth-note pulse never once lets up, even at the climax shown in Example 3.9.
99
97
42
When the LH rests for three eighth notes, the RH relentlessly maintains the pulse.
Second, melodic accents may be found in every measure of the etude. As shown in
Example 3.10, this is even the case at the LH’s mm. 91–96 when it appears that the
93
91
As described earlier, just the opposite is true. Here, the two melodies of the foreground
layer, which are now progressing at almost one accent per eighth, will likely be perceived
as occluding the background. Third, the scalar ascents in the background layer become
length scalar ascent present in the RH phrase groups, as shown in Figure 3.1. The figure
labels the beginning pitch of every phrase group. Those in the RH combine to form a
It should be noted that despite the general registral descent of the LH phrase-
group patterns over the course of the etude, its background scalar material consistently
ascends. Because of this, it seems safe to assign greater hierarchical significance to the
43
idea of scalar ascent in “Désordre” than to the idea of scalar descent. Over the course of
the etude, the small ascents seem to push little by little upward toward an eventual goal of
C8, the highest note on the piano. The music seems to be pulled upward by a kind of
upside-down musical gravity leading to the upper register of the piano. C8 on the piano
seems like the natural destination for the music, not A0. In a sense, it is as if the scalar
ascents point toward, or rather foreshadow, the ending. Scalar ascents, presented
gradually or quickly, seem to be one of the pillars of the structure of the composition and
It is the author’s hope that these Gestalt principles have helped to clarify how
Ligeti’s etude is organized, and that the techniques presented may also prove beneficial to
other composers. Composers like Ligeti seem to exhibit an innate understanding of how
listeners perceive their music. In 1989, James Guthrie, who appears to have been the first
to formally analyze this etude, supplied clues that “Désordre” might have been inspired
by perceptual phenomena:
One of these words (stroboscope) names a perceptual device used for creating illusions.
A stroboscope is a mechanism that was used to make something moving fast appear to be
moving slowly, in the same way people can blink their eyes quickly when staring at a
74
Ligeti, Études pour piano - premier livre, facsimile edition 7428 (Mainz: Schott, 1986), cover.
75
James Martin Guthrie, “Études pour piano—premier livre, by György Ligeti, and The Song of Glory,
an Original Opera in One Act,” DMA diss. (Louisiana State University and Agricultural and Mechanical
College, 1989).
44
slowly spinning bicycle wheel and perceive the spokes as moving backward, or even
standing still. This has parallels in “Désordre.” Because of the spacing of the accents on
top of the fast-moving eighth-note runs, the illusion is created that the irregular accents
To the author’s knowledge, Ligeti never spoke of using Gestalt principles to guide
his choices in this etude. However, his compositional techniques seem to suggest that he
knew how listeners might perceive it. Just as a person can communicate with another
without knowing how to analyze the speech in terms of Chomskian linguistics, Ligeti
does not need to be consciously aware of the Gestalt principles behind his music.
understanding of Gestalt principles? Diana Deutsch ends her chapter in The Psychology
Just as it has been shown that “Désordre” may be explained in terms of Gestalt principles,
the careful study of perceptual principles can improve a composer’s ability to structure
musical ideas.
76
Diana Deutsch, “Grouping Mechanisms in Music,” in The Psychology of Music, 2nd ed., ed. Diana
Deutsch (San Diego: Academic Press, 1999), 342. Italics added.
45
Gestalt principles can also help teachers of composition explain to students why
certain compositional choices seem to “work” and why others clearly do not. In a
hypothetical composition lesson, assuming both the teacher and the student are familiar
with Gestalt principles, Example 3.11 represents compositional material that the student
The teacher might ask if the student’s intention was to have the first measure sound as
If the student were to respond that the notes assigned to the LH were meant to be heard as
a pedal-point ostinato, the teacher could point out that, according to the principle of
proximity, the notes in m. 1 would probably fuse into a single line as shown in Example
3.12. The teacher might continue by drawing upon the principle of figure/ground
46
articulation, explaining that unless edges are properly articulated in order to distinguish
the LH notes as a separate line, an ostinato would probably not be perceived until m. 2
when the notes finally cross the trill threshold and are no longer able to be grouped by
proximity. The teacher then might suggest the revision shown in Example 3.13.
Here the teacher would explain that, according to the principle of continuity, the presence
of the ostinato prior to the entrance of the RH material would more likely cause a listener
to hear the LH content as a line continuing through the RH notes. As is clearly evident in
Example 3.13, the teacher could further suggest a uniform articulation in the LH material,
which, according to the principle of similarity, would strengthen its distinction from the
47
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Arom, Simha. African Polyphony and Polyrhythm: Musical Structure and Methodology.
Foreword by György Ligeti, xvii–xviii. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press,
1985.
Deutsch, Diana, et al. “Psychology of Music.” Grove Music Online. Oxford Music Online.
Oxford University Press, accessed February 4, 2015,
http://www.oxfordmusiconline.com/subscriber/article/grove/music/42574pg1.
Guthrie, James Martin. “Etudes pour piano—premier livre, by György Ligeti, and The
Song of Glory, an Original Opera in One Act.” DMA diss., Louisiana State University
and Agricultural and Mechanical College, 1989.
Hodges, Donald A. Handbook of Music Psychology. San Antonio, TX: IMR Press, 1996.
Hofstadter, Douglas R. Metamagical Themas: Questing for the Essence of Mind and
Pattern. New York: Basic Books, 1985.
Howell, Peter, et al., eds. Musical Structure and Cognition. London: Academic Press,
1985.
Kinzler, Hartmuth. “György Ligeti: Decision and Automatism in ‘Désordre,’ 1re étude,
premier livre.” Interface 20 (1991): 89–124.
48
Köhler, Wolfgang. Gestalt Psychology. New York: Liverlight, 1929.
Lerdahl, Fred, and Ray Jackendoff. A Generative Theory of Tonal Music. Cambridge:
MIT Press, 1983.
———. Liner notes to György Ligeti, Edition 3: Works for Piano (Etudes and Musica
Ricercata). Sony (DDD 62309), 1997. Pierre-Laurent Aimard, piano.
———. Liner notes to Ligeti: Piano Etudes, Book 1. Erato (ECD 75555), 1990. Pierre-
Laurent Aimard, piano.
Miller, George A., and George A. Heise. “The Trill Threshold.” Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America 22 (September 1950), 637–38.
Narmour, Eugene. The Analysis and Cognition of Basic Melodic Structures: The
Implication-realization Model. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1990.
———. Vision Science: Photons to Phenomenology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1999.
Sabar, Stephanie. “What’s a Gestalt?” Gestalt Review 17, no. 1 (2013): 6–34, accessed
March 11, 2015. http://www.gisc.org/gestaltreview/documents/whatsagestalt.pdf.
49
Sloboda, John A. The Musical Mind: The Cognitive Psychology of Music. Oxford:
Clarendon Press, 1985.
Tsong, Mayron K. “Études pour piano, premier livre of György Ligeti: Studies in
Composition and Pianism.” MM thesis, Rice University, 2001.
Uranker, Mark. “Ligeti’s ‘Désordre’ from Études pour piano—premier livre: An Analysis
and Performance Guide.” MM thesis, California State University, Long Beach, 1998.
Wertheimer, Max. Experimentelle Studien über das Sehen von Bewegung [Experimental
Studies on the Seeing of Motion]. Leipzig: J.A. Barth, 1912.
50
APPENDIX A
Continuity: Objects that can be seen as smooth continuations of each other tend to be
grouped.
is not
51