Sub Guide
Sub Guide
Pia Pichler
EN3117
2013
Contents
i
Language and Gender
ii
Course learning outcomes and assessment criteria
Learning outcomes
By the end of the course you should:
• have become familiar with different theoretical and methodological
frameworks used in the field of language and gender
• have developed a critical awareness of different conceptualisations
of gender and identity
• be able to contrast folklinguistic and stereotypical notions of gender
with empirical findings generated in sociolinguistics and discourse
analysis
• be equipped to investigate the role that language plays in
constructing gender and other identities
• have acquired the knowledge and skills to carry out independent
empirical investigations in the field of language and gender.
Mode of assessment
One three-hour unseen examination.
Assessment criteria
You will be assessed according to your ability to:
• demonstrate your in-depth understanding of different theoretical
and methodological approaches to language and gender
1
Language and Gender
2
Chapter 1: Introduction
Chapter 1: Introduction
Reading list
Essential reading
All of your essential reading will be based on two books:
Coates, J. and P. Pichler (eds) Language and Gender: A Reader. (Oxford:
Blackwell, 2011) second edition [ISBN 9781405191272].
3
Language and Gender
Further reading
Further reading suggestions will be included in each chapter. These
readings will be taken from your two set texts as well as from many
other books. Some of the books that you may want to consult for
further reading include:
Bucholtz, M., A.C. Liang and L.A. Sutton (eds) Reinventing Identities: The
Gendered Self in Discourse. (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1999)
[ISBN 9780195126303]. Once you have worked through this subject
guide and done all your preparatory reading, you should read this
collection, which offers a range of exciting papers to deepen and
broaden your areas of interest.
Cameron, D. (ed.) The Feminist Critique of Language: A Reader. (London:
Routledge, 1998) second edition [ISBN 9780415164009]. This reader
contains several extracts from language and gender classics. However,
not all chapters are relevant, as some deal with how women and men
are represented in the media and other written language.
Eckert, P. and S. McConnell-Ginet Language and Gender. (Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press, 2003) [ISBN 9780521654265]. This textbook
elaborates on many of the issues raised in this subject guide: it is
particularly valuable on performative notions of gender, which are
introduced in Chapter 5 of this subject guide.
Holmes, J. and M. Meyerhoff (eds) The Handbook of Language and Gender.
(Oxford: Blackwell, 2003) [ISBN 9780631225034]. This offers an
extensive and valuable resource of papers and research on language
and gender. Some of the chapters are very accessible and are suitable
as introductory reading; others require a previous knowledge on the
subject.
Mills, S. and L. Mullany Language, Gender and Feminism: Theory,
Methodology and Practice. (London: Routledge, 2011)
[ISBN 9780415485968]. This book presents a range of theoretical
perspectives and methodological and analytical approaches, linking
language and gender studies firmly to their feminist roots. It provides
many examples of analysis which show how theory can be applied to a
critical discussion of data.
Talbot, M. Language and Gender. (Cambridge: Polity Press, 2010) second
edition [ISBN 9780745646053]. This is an accessible introduction to all
the topics in this subject guide. You may wish to read this book from
cover to cover.
4
Chapter 1: Introduction
Journals
Remember also that many linguistic journals will provide you with
valuable sources for further reading. Frequently these journals contain
the most up-to-date research. You may find that you can get access
to one or other of these journals electronically, through an academic
library close to you.
• Applied Linguistics
• Discourse Studies
• Discourse and Society
• Journal of Sociolinguistics
• Language in Society
• Gender and Language.
The journal Gender and Language is obviously one that is dedicated
exclusively to the subject area of this course. However, all the other
journals also publish articles on the area of language and gender. You
will, however, need to search through them to find relevant papers.
Websites
Moreover, you may also find some useful information and further links
on the website of the International Gender and Language Association
(IGALA): www.lancs.ac.uk/fass/organisations/igala/Index.html
Online resources
Please note that additional study resources may be available to you
for this course. A particularly important resource is the virtual learning
environment (VLE) for the English programme, which you can access
via the Student Portal – see the Student handbook for details of how to
log in. Do check the VLE regularly as additional material may be added
throughout the year.
5
Language and Gender
There are six chapters in this subject guide. After the general
information on how to use this guide contained in this chapter,
Chapters 2–6 cover a wide range of concepts, issues and questions in
the field of language and gender studies. These include:
Chapter 2: Stereotypes vs empirical research on language and gender;
methodological approaches to the study of language and gender.
Chapter 3: Patterns of conversational dominance in mixed-sex talk.
Chapter 4: All-female and all-male talk; the notions of ‘collaborative’ vs
‘competitive’ conversational styles.
Chapter 5: The relationship between gender, ethnicity and social class
and performative notions of gender and sexuality.
Chapter 6: Language and gender in public and institutional contexts.
We recommend that you approach each chapter in the following way:
• Read the brief chapter introduction, which will focus you on some of
the key issues that you need to explore as part of your reading.
• Carry out the Essential reading suggested for this chapter.
• Work through the chapter in the subject guide and engage with the
activities.
• Check your understanding of the main issues addressed in the
chapter by working through the list of Learning outcomes towards
end of each chapter.
• Consolidate your ideas by attempting some Sample examination
questions at the end of the chapter.
After a general introduction you are provided with a list of Essential
readings and suggestions for Further reading. After you have carried
out your essential reading, made notes and reflected on some of
the main issues raised, you can begin to work through the chapter.
The chapters do not offer comprehensive coverage of their subjects,
but seek to raise some fundamental questions and provide you with
ideas about how you can develop your own reading, thinking and
understanding on the topics that interest you.
You will encounter many activities, including questions related to your
Essential reading and extracts from transcribed conversations, that
encourage you to carry out your own analysis. You will make most
progress if you attempt to engage with each of these activities as you
come across them in the text. Many of them will require you to refer
back to the reading and then write your answers down or discuss them
with someone else.
6
Chapter 1: Introduction
Weeks 1–4
Introductory readings on language and gender; concepts of sex and
gender, stereotypes of women’s and men’s talk; early linguistic thinking
on gender; gender differences in conversational practice; gender and
politeness; transcribing spoken interaction; analysing gender in spoken
interaction
Weeks 5–8
Mixed-sex talk and the dominance approach; conversational
dominance in mixed-sex interaction; interruptions and conversational
dominance; the turn-taking model; co-constructing conversational
dominance; critiquing the ‘dominance approach’
Weeks 9–12
Same-sex talk and the difference approach; women’s talk and
conversational collaboration; men’s talk and conversational
competition; the cultural difference approach and cross-sex
miscommunication; deconstructing the ‘competitive’/‘collaborative’
dichotomy
Weeks 13–16
Constructing gender and sexual identities; situated and sociocultural
gender identities; Communities of Practice; the social constructionist
approach; indexicality; discourses; language, gender and sexuality
7
Language and Gender
Weeks 17–20
Language and gender in public settings; institutional talk and
work/public contexts; language and gender in public settings; the
heterogeneity of gendered leadership styles at work; androcentric
norms in public institutions; attitudes towards assertive women
Weeks 21–22
Revision and review: draw up a schedule of topics to revise and study
the sample examination questions.
Examination advice
Important: the information and advice given here are based on the
examination structure used at the time this guide was written. Please
note that subject guides may be used for several years. Because
of this we strongily advise you to always check both the current
Regulations for relevant information about the examination, and the
virtual learning environment (VLE) where you should be advised of
any forthcoming changes. You should also carefully check the rubric/
instructions on the paper you actually sit and follow those instructions
The assessment for this course is via a three-hour unseen examination.
You will be expected to answer three questions out of a total of (at
least) nine. The examination paper will be quite wide-ranging, and will
contain questions on a selection of topics from the course as a whole.
You should not present substantially the same material in any
two answers, whether on this paper or in any other parts of your
examination. This shows how important it is for you to prepare to
answer questions on a range of topics: we suggest at least six. You will
find Sample examination questions attached to each chapter.
Please consult the Course learning outcomes and assessment
criteria above to check what criteria your Examiners will base their
assessment on.
Remember, it is important to check the VLE for:
• up-to-date information on examination and assessment
arrangements for this course
• where available, past examination papers and Examiners' reports for
the course which give advice on how each question might best be
answered.
9
Language and Gender
Notes
10
Chapter 2: Approaching language and gender studies
Essential reading
Coates (2004) Chapter 1 ‘Language and gender’, Chapter 2 ‘The historical
background – folklinguistics and early grammarians’ and Chapter 6
‘Gender differences in conversational practice’.
Holmes, J. ‘Complimenting – a positive politeness strategy’ in Coates and
Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.71–88.
Swann, J. ‘Yes, but is it gender?’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011)
pp.161–70.
Further reading
Brown, P. and S. Levinson Politeness: Some Universals in Language Usage.
(Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1987) [ISBN 9780521313551].
Bucholtz, M. ‘Theories of discourse as theories of gender: discourse analysis
in language and gender studies’ in Holmes, J. and M. Meyerhoff (eds)
The Handbook of Language and Gender. (Oxford: Blackwell, 2003)
[ISBN 9780631225034].
Cameron, D. Working with Spoken Discourse. (London: Sage Publications,
2001) [ISBN 9780761957737] especially Chapter 3: ‘Transcribing spoken
data’ and Chapter 12 ‘Designing your own projects’.
Cameron, D. ‘Gender and language ideologies’ in Coates and Pichler (eds)
(2011) pp.583–600.
Cameron, D., F. McAlinden and K. O´Leary ‘Lakoff in context: the social
and linguistic functions of tag questions’ in Coates, J. and D. Cameron
(eds) Women in Their Speech Communities. (London: Longman, 1989)
[ISBN 9780582009691] pp.74–93.
Coates, J. Women Talk: Conversation Between Women Friends. (Oxford:
Blackwell, 1996) [ISBN 9780631195955].
Davies, J. ‘Expressions of gender: an analysis of pupils’ gendered discourse
styles in small group classroom discussions’ in Coates and Pichler (eds)
(2011) pp.112–25.
De Francisco, V. 'The sounds of silence: how men silence women in marital
relations' in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.153–160.
Eckert, P. ‘Gender and sociolinguistic variation’ in Coates and Pichler (eds)
(2011) pp.57–66.
Eisikovits, E. ‘Girl-talk/boy-talk: sex differences in adolescent speech’ in
Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.38–48.
Fishman, P. ‘Interactional shitwork’, Heresies 2, 1980, pp 99–101.
Fishman, P. ‘Conversational insecurity’ in Cameron, D. (ed.) The Feminist
Critique of Language (London: Routledge, 1998) second edition
[ISBN 0415164001] pp.253–58.
Goodwin, M. ‘Cooperation and competition across girls’ play activities’ in
Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.89–111.
Harrington, K., L. Litosseliti, H. Sauntson and J. Sunderland (eds) Gender and
Language Research Methodologies. (Houndmills: Palgrave Macmillan,
2008) [ISBN 9780230550698] especially Chapter 1 ‘Current research
11
Language and Gender
References
De Beauvoir, S. The Second Sex. (London: Vintage, 1997) [ISBN
9780099744214].
Butler, J. Gender Trouble: Feminism and the Subversion of Identity. (London:
Routledge, 1990) [ISBN 9780415900430].
Cameron, D. Verbal Hygiene. (London: Routledge, 1995) [ISBN
9780415103558].
12
Chapter 2: Approaching language and gender studies
Introduction
The aim of this chapter is to introduce you to language and gender.
Your Essential reading for this chapter covers a wide range of historical
and topical issues and debates. We will begin by examining the
difference between the concepts ‘gender’ and ‘sex’. We will then
consider a range of stereotypes about how women and men talk, as
well as early linguistic writing on the topic of gendered differences
in language use. We will focus in particular on the work of Robin
Lakoff (see, for example, 1975, 1998, 2004), pioneer of language and
gender studies , critically engaging with her view that there is such a
thing as ‘women’s language’ and that this ‘women’s language’ is very
unassertive. We will then begin to engage with empirical research on
gender differences in conversational practices, and this engagement
will continue throughout this subject guide and the course. You will find
a list of linguistic features and practices here that have been examined
by language and gender scholars to find out if women and men do
indeed use language in different ways. You will be able to begin to
understand how stereotypes about women’s and men’s use of language
compare with the findings of empirical research in this vibrant area of
sociolinguistics. We will also consider the relationship between these
stereotypes and the ways in which women and men really do use
language. Finally, this chapter will consider some important questions,
asking how the analysis of language and gender can be approached
in real, empirical studies undertaken by scholars and students. We will
particularly consider warrants of analysis, that is, the ‘proof’ that scholars
usually offer on the basis of their research to support their claims that
language use is indeed gendered.
Activity
Let us start our journey into the exploration of the relationship between language
use and gender with the help of the following questions:
• How do we ‘do’ gender in our lives? Which props do we use?
• Do you view your own way of talking as gendered? Why (not)?
• Think of family and friends, etc.: which verbal and non-verbal props do they
use to ‘do' gender, that is, to present themselves as feminine or masculine?
• Are there any gender norms? When/how do people ‘do’ gender inappropriately?
13
Language and Gender
Activity
Try to answer as many of the above questions as possible. Then return to the
questions whenever you can as you work through this guide and reflect on how
your answers may change as a result of your reading.
so far that people speak about the gender rather than the sex of their
pets (Cameron, 1995, p.127)! With regard to humans, the conflation
of the two terms in many ways captures the persistent belief that
women and men are determined by their biological sex, that the way
they think, talk and behave is a mere consequence of their innate
differences. Although it is probably the case that many sociolinguists
would, if pressed, not deny that genetic differences between women
and men do affect individuals’ behaviour to some extent, most would
argue that the differences between the way women and men behave
both in linguistic and non-linguistic ways is very much due to cultural
norms about masculinity and femininity. Whereas the term ‘sex’ is
used to refer to men's and women’s biological difference, that is, their
sex chromosomes and their outer bodily appearance including their
genitalia, gender is seen as a ‘cultural construct’. This notion of gender
as a ‘cultural construct’ reflects the view that women's and men’s
behaviour is influenced by and orients to social and actual norms
about how women and men ought to behave.
Language and gender scholars have long resisted the notion of women
and men as being predetermined by their biological sex. They refer to
Simone de Beauvoir’s famous saying ‘One is not born, but becomes a
woman’(1997, p.295) to emphasise that there is a difference between
sex and gender. More recently, it has been argued that gender
identities are not learned once and for all in early childhood, but
that instead gender is continuously constructed or performed (see
Butler, 1990, p.33) throughout our lives. You will hear more about this
performative or social constructionist approach to gender in Chapter
5, but throughout this guide we will continue to regard gender as a
social and cultural construct, rather than as a reflection of biological
differences between men and women. Of course, one of the most
significant ways in which masculine and feminine gender identities are
reflected and enacted is through language.
15
Language and Gender
Activity
Think about the ways in which your friends and family use language. Do they use
language in a stereotypically feminine or masculine way or not? What type of
gender identity are they constructing for themselves? For example, if a woman
finds it very inappropriate for women to swear and subsequently moderates her
language behaviour, this will go some way towards the construction of a more
traditional femininity (although this construction can be challenged in other ways).
17
Language and Gender
18
Chapter 2: Approaching language and gender studies
Activity
Do you agree with Lakoff? Why/why not? How would you critique the ‘deficit’
approach?
Lakoff ’s ‘method’ has since been criticised heavily: the list of features
which is at the core of her claims was not obtained from empirical
research but was instead derived from introspection, that is, Lakoff ’s
own views and informal observations. A further problem was that she
did not fully acknowledge that many of the linguistic forms on the
list above can function in different ways, at times signalling lack of
assertiveness, at other times fulfilling other very important functions.
For example, using question intonation in what is supposed to be a
declarative, as in the ‘Oh... around six o’clock?’ answer above, does not
signal a lack of assertiveness but simply signals that dinner could also
be at another time. So, in a very efficient way, the question intonation
actually says, ‘Is that OK with you?’ The function then is cooperation,
rather than tentativeness. The same goes for several of the other
features highlighted above. The fact that one linguistic form can fulfil
different functions has become known as the ‘form-function problem’,
which later language and gender studies were very much aware of.
Activity
When do speakers use hedges such as ‘well’, ‘y’know’, ’kinda’, etc.? Think of specific
situations. Are these forms superfluous or do they serve important functions, and
if so, which? What would happen if we avoided using hedges in all situations?
• minimal responses
• topic choice and topic development
• use of tag questions such as ‘isn’t it?’, ‘don’t you?’
• use of hedges such as ‘you know’, ‘kind of’, ‘maybe’
• use of questions and/or question intonation in declaratives
• features of politeness such as compliments or levels of
(in)directness.
Many of these features will be discussed in detail in later chapters when
we examine language and gender research on mixed-sex, same-sex,
and private and public talk.
For example, the question of whether women and men have a different
sense of what is appropriate turn-taking is investigated in Chapters 2
and 3 (mixed and same-sex talk) and the question of whether women
or men are more talkative is addressed both in Chapter 2 (mixed-sex
talk in private contexts) and in Chapter 6 (mixed-sex talk in institutional
contexts).
There are many language and gender studies which suggest that women
make more use of minimal responses such as ‘mhm’, ‘yeah’, ‘right’ than
men to signal that they are listening and to encourage to speaker to
continue (Fishman, 1983; DeFrancisco, 2011; Preisler, 1986). This type of
conversational support can at times be inserted in between utterances
of the current speaker, but frequently is produced while the other person
is speaking. The following is an example of such minimal support, taken
from the talk of a group of 17 to18-year-old male British students with
Nigel, an ethnographer, who is interviewing them.
Example 1
Example 2
Some scholars argue that there are gender differences not only with
respect to the quantity of minimal responses used by speakers, but
also their function. Difference theorists, like Maltz and Borker (1982) or
Tannen (1992), suggest that for women a minimal response tends to
signal active listenership whereas for men it actually signals agreement
with the proposition being made. DeFrancisco’s (1991) study of
heterosexual couples shows that men use minimal responses much
more in a non-supportive way, signalling inattentive, uncooperative
behaviour, by delaying them, for example. However, Reid-Thomas
(1993, in Cameron, 2001, p.118) finds no such gender difference in
the use and interpretation of minimal responses in her own data,
suggesting that minimal responses serve different functions for both
women and men, depending on the context in which they are used.
There is also a widespread belief that women use more epistemic
modal forms, including hedges such as ‘well’, ‘you know’, ‘kind of’,
‘I think’, ‘like’; modal auxiliaries such as ‘could’, ‘may’ and ‘might’ and
other mitigating forms such as ‘perhaps’, ‘maybe’. All of these forms
are said to function as mitigation, either by reducing the force of an
utterance or by expressing the speaker’s attitude (lack of certainty)
towards their utterance. The belief that women’s language is tentative
not only featured in Robin Lakoff ’s (1975) pioneering work on
language and gender, but is still widespread in popular discourse,
fuelling assertiveness training courses and self-help books targeting
women. Several empirical studies have found a gender difference with
respect to hedges; indeed, some confirm that women’s overall use
is higher than men’s (Fishman, 1980; Preisler, 1986), but the findings
of most empirical studies are rather more complex, pointing to the
multifunctionality of these forms.
‘You know’ can have a positive interactional function, allowing speakers
to signal their sensitivity to the feelings of others (Coates, 1996) or
aiming to get another speaker’s attention (Fishman, 1980; Holmes,
1984, 1987). This latter function is exemplified in the following example
from a group of 16-year-old girls discussing contraception, where one
states ‘you know boys think condoms are passion killers’. (This example
and those that follow are from Pichler, 2009.)
Of course, ‘you know’ can signal uncertainty, as in the following
example of private school girls complaining about their socially
unaware fellow students: ‘some people just don’t think that other
people perceive you as sort of (.) you know over-privileged’.
21
Language and Gender
22
Chapter 2: Approaching language and gender studies
Activity
After this introduction on language and politeness, answer the following
questions based on your preparatory reading of Janet Holmes’s chapter,
‘Complimenting – a positive politeness strategy’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011).
• What quantitative gender differences in the use of compliments did Holmes
find?
23
Language and Gender
• What qualitative gender differences in the use of compliments did Holmes find?
• How did Holmes explain gender differences in complimenting?
• To what extent is women’s status in society relevant to explanations of gender
differences in complimenting?
• What is your view on these explanations?
• Which factors other than gender may influence how speakers use compliments?
Activity
Consider the following two transcripts which capture the talk of a group of young
British Bangladeshi girls who reminisce about skipping lessons at school (Pichler,
2009, pp.111–12). Which transcript do you prefer and why? How do the transcription
conventions reflect the differences between spoken and written language?
A: Traditional transcript
24
Chapter 2: Approaching language and gender studies
B: Stave transcript
25
Language and Gender
You should always take note of how conversational data have been
transcribed, always study explanations on transcription conventions
that are given in books, chapters and journal articles. For an in-depth
discussion of linguistic transcription, see Cameron (2001) Working with
Spoken Discourse, Chapter 3: 'Transcribing spoken data'.
style etc. should be interpreted, and specifically how we can be sure that
these choices are part of speakers’ self-presentation as women or men.
Activity
On the basis of your preparatory reading and the following list based on Swann
(2011): ‘Yes, but is it gender?’, discuss the advantages and drawbacks of a range of
such ‘warrants’, including:
• quantitative and/or general patterns
• indirect reliance on quantitative/general patterns
• participants’ orientations as evident in the text
• speakers’/participants’ solicited interpretations
• analysts’ theoretical positions
• analysts’ intuitions
• speakers/participants are female, male.
Learning outcomes
By the end of this chapter, having studied the essential reading and
some of the recommended critical texts, you should:
• have developed an understanding of the wide range of
conversational strategies and linguistic features that have been
examined by language and gender scholars as well as of the
multiple functions that have been attributed to these features
• have considered stereotypical beliefs of women’s and men’s way
of talking and their relationship to actual research on gendered
language
• be able to discuss the deficit approach to language and gender
critically, contrasting it with other explanatory frameworks
• have read and reflected critically on Robin Lakoff ’s work on
language and gender
• have examined a range of different transcripts and transcription
conventions employed by language and gender scholars
• have developed an ability to critically discuss different ‘warrants of
analysis’ employed by language and gender scholars.
27
Language and Gender
28
Chapter 3: Mixed-sex talk and the dominance approach
Essential reading
Coates (2004) Chapter 7 ‘Conversational dominance in mixed talk’,
pp.111–24.
DeFrancisco, V. ‘The sounds of silence: how men silence women in marital
relations’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.143–52.
O’Barr, W. and K. Bowman Atkins, ‘Women´s language’ or ‘powerless
language’?’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.451–60.
Swann, J. ‘Talk control: an illustration from the classroom of problems in
analysing male dominance in conversation’ in Coates and Pichler (eds)
(2011) pp.161–70.
West, C. and D. Zimmerman ‘Women’s place in everyday talk: reflections on
parent-child interaction’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.139–42.
Further reading
Cameron, D. Working with Spoken Discourse. (London: Sage, 2001)
[ISBN 0761957737] especially Chapter 7, ‘Sequence and Structure:
Conversation Analysis’.
Edelsky, C. ‘Who’s got the floor?’ in Tannen, D. (ed.) Gender and
Conversational Interaction. (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1993)
[ISBN 0195081943] pp.189–226.
Fishman, P. ‘Conversational insecurity’ in Cameron, D. (ed.) The Feminist
Critique of Language. (London: Routledge, 1983) second edition
[ISBN 0415164001] pp.253–58.
Greenwood, A. ‘Floor management and power strategies in adolescent
conversation’ in Bergvall, V., J. Bing and A. Freed (eds) Rethinking
Language and Gender Research: Theory and Practice. (Harlow: Longman,
1996) [ISBN 9780582265738] pp.77–97.
Herring, S. et al. ‘Participation in electronic discourse in a ‘feminist’ field’ in
Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.171–82.
Jie Yang Zuiqian ‘“Deficient mouth”: discourse, gender and domestic
violence in urban China’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.183–92.
Lakoff, R. Language and Woman´s Place. (New York: Harper & Row,
1975) [ISBN 0060903899].
Mills, S. ‘Rethinking politeness, impoliteness and gender identity’
in Litosseliti, L. and J. Sunderland (eds) Gender Identity and
Discourse Analysis. (Amsterdam: John Benjamins Pub Co., 2002)
[ISBN 9781588112132] pp.69–90.
West, C. ‘When the doctor is a lady: power, status and gender in physician-
patient encounters’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.468–82.
Wetzel, P. J. ‘Are ‘powerless’ communication strategies the Japanese norm?'
in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.461–67.
29
Language and Gender
References
Beattie, G. ‘Interruptions in conversational interaction, and its relation to
the sex and status of the interactants’, Linguistics 19, 1981.
Beattie, G. Talk: Analysis of Speech and Non-verbal Behaviour in Conversation.
(Milton Keynes: Open University Press, 1983) [ISBN 9780335104147].
Cameron, D. ‘Rethinking language and gender studies: some issues for the
1990s’ in Mills, S. (ed.) Language and Gender: Interdisciplinary Perspectives.
(London: Longman, 1995) [ISBN 9780582226319] pp.31–44.
Coates, J. Women Talk. Conversation Between Women Friends. (Oxford:
Blackwell, 1996) [ISBN 9780631195955].
Coates, J. Men Talk. (Oxford: Blackwell, 2003) [ISBN 9780631220466].
Fishman, P. ‘Interactional shitwork’, Heresies 2, 1980, pp.99–101.
Hutchby, I. and R. Wooffitt Conversation Analysis. Principles, Practices and
Applications. (Cambridge: Polity Press, 1998) [ISBN 9780745615493].
James, D. and S. Clarke ‘Women, men and interruption’ in Tannen, D. (ed.)
Gender and Conversational Interaction. (Oxford: Oxford University Press,
1993) [ISBN 9780195081947].
Kollock, P. et al. ‘Sex and power in interaction: conversational privileges and
duties’, American Sociological Review 50, 1985.
Menz, F. and Al-Roubaie, A. ‘Interruptions, status and gender in medical
interviews: the harder you brake, the longer it takes’, Discourse & Society
19(5), 2008.
Pichler, P. Talking Young Femininities. (London: Palgrave, 2009) [ISBN
9780230013285].
Sacks, H., E.A. Schegloff and G. Jefferson ‘A simplest systematics for the
organisation of turn-taking for conversation’, Language 50, 1974,
pp.696–735.
Talbot, M. ‘‘I wish you’d stop interrupting me’: interruptions and
asymmetries in speaker-rights in equal encounters’, Journal of
Pragmatics 18, 1992, pp.451–66.
West, C. and D.H. Zimmermann ‘Small insults: a study of interruptions in
cross-sex conversations between unacquainted persons’ in Thorne, B.,
C. Kramarae and N. Henley (eds) Language, Gender and Society. (Rowley,
MT: Newbury House, 1983) [ISBN 9780838429372] pp.103–18.
Zimmerman, D.H. and C. West ‘Sex roles, interruptions and silences in
conversation’ in Thorne, B. and N. Henley (eds) Language and Sex:
Difference and Dominance. (Rowley, MT: Newbury House, 1975) [ISBN
9780883770436] pp.105–29.
Introduction
In this chapter we will focus on how women and men speak to each
other, or, in other words, on mixed-sex talk. We will take a closer look
at some of the conversational features that have been introduced in
the previous chapters, with a particular focus on features that can
be associated with dominant conversational behaviour. The term
‘conversational dominance’ is particularly relevant with respect to
mixed-sex talk, as much of the seminal research on the talk between
men and women suggests that men dominate conversations with
30
Chapter 3: Mixed-sex talk and the dominance approach
Example 1
31
Language and Gender
32
Chapter 3: Mixed-sex talk and the dominance approach
33
Language and Gender
Example 2
Activity
Having read West and Zimmerman (2011) try to answer the following questions
about turn-taking.
• What is the turn-taking model?
• What are ‘unit-types’?
• What, according to West and Zimmerman, is the difference between
interruptions and overlaps?
• What is a ‘transition (relevance) place’? How is it relevant to your analysis?
• What is ‘latching’?
• What do speakers display or’ accomplish’ when they interrupt one another?
(Can we answer this question categorically, independent of the context?)
35
Language and Gender
Example 3
36
Chapter 3: Mixed-sex talk and the dominance approach
Example 4
Activity
Compare the two examples above from the same group of speakers. Try to decide
whether the simultaneous speech in Example 3 and Example 4 (particularly in
stave 2) represent instances of supportive simultaneous speech or interruptions.
You will probably have decided that Example 3 does not contain an
interruption. Jane starts talking very near a completion point or, as
CA would term it, a Transition Relevance Point (TRP). Elizabeth may
well have completed her utterance after saying ‘twelve’, and Jane
simply predicts this possible point of completion very well. This in fact
shows speakers who are very well tuned in with each other. Example
4 is more difficult to interpret. If we only take syntactic criteria into
consideration then we have to say that in stave 2 Roberta starts talking
half way through Nicky’s turn without Nicky being anywhere near a
completion point. On the other hand we could say that Roberta may
have anticipated how Nicky was going to complete her utterance (for
example, by saying ‘... with many different women’). These types of
predictions and also incomplete utterances (such as Nicky’s) are very
common in groups of female friends (see Coates, 1996). So Roberta’s
intention may not have been to interrupt Nicky and take over from her.
37
Language and Gender
38
Chapter 3: Mixed-sex talk and the dominance approach
Learning outcomes
After working through this chapter and having done a substantial
amount of reading on the topic you should:
• have an understanding of the conversational strategies that can be
examined in an analysis of conversational dominance, including
interruptions, topic control, or absent or delayed minimal responses,
silence and verbosity
• be able to discuss the dominance approach to language and gender
critically, contrasting it with other explanatory frameworks
• have read and reflected on a range of empirical studies on mixed-
sex interaction
• have developed a critical understanding of the relationship
between language, gender and power
• have developed an insight into the turn-taking model used in
Conversation Analysis.
39
Language and Gender
Notes
40
Chapter 4: Same-sex talk and the difference approach
Essential reading
Coates, J. ‘Gossip revisited: language in all-female groups’ in Coates and
Pichler (eds) (2011).
Coates, J. (2004) Chapter 8 ‘Same-sex talk’.
Maltz, D. and R. Borker ‘A cultural approach to male-female communication’
in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.487–502.
Tannen, D. ‘Asymmetries: women and men talking at cross purposes’ in
Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.503–17.
Troemel-Ploetz, S. ‘Selling the apolitical’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011)
pp.518–28.
Further reading
Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) Part IV ‘Same-sex talk’, pp.193–286.
Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) Part VIII ‘Theoretical debates 2: difference or
dominance’, pp.483–528.
Cameron, D. ‘Performing gender identity: young men’s talk and the
construction of heterosexual masculinity’ in Coates and Pichler (eds)
(2011) pp.250–62.
Cameron, D. The Myth of Mars and Venus: Do Men and Women Really
Speak Different Languages? (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2007)
[ISBN 9780199550999].
Coates, J. Women Talk: Conversation between Women Friends. (Oxford:
Blackwell, 1996) [ISBN 9780631195955].
Coates, J. ‘One-at-a-time: the organisation of men’s talk’ in Meinhof, U. and
S. Johnson (eds) Language and Masculinity. (London: Blackwell, 1997)
[ISBN 9780631197683] pp.107–29.
Coates, J. ‘Changing femininities: the talk of teenage girls’ in Bucholtz, M.,
A. C. Liang and L.A. Sutton (eds) Reinventing Identities: The
Gendered Self in Discourse. (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1999)
[ISBN 9780195126303] pp.123–44.
Coates, J. Men Talk. Stories in the Making of Masculinities. (Oxford: Blackwell,
2003) [ISBN 9780631220466].
Coates, J. ‘Pushing at the boundaries: the expression of alternative
masculinities’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.263–74.
Eckert, P. ‘Cooperative competition in adolescent “girl talk”’ in Tannen, D.
(ed.) Gender and Conversational Interaction. (Oxford: Oxford University
Press, 1993) ISBN 9780195081947].
Edelsky, C. ‘Who’s got the floor?’ in Tannen (1993) pp.189–226.
Edley, N. and M. Whetherell ‘Jockeying for position: the construction of
masculine identities’, Discourse & Society 8/2, 1997, pp.203–18.
Goodwin, M. ‘Cooperation and competition across girls’ play activities’ in
Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.89–111.
41
Language and Gender
References
Benwell, B. and E. Stokoe Discourse and Identity. (Edinburgh: Edinburgh
University Press, 2006) [ISBN 9780748617500].
Bucholtz, M. ‘Why be normal? Language and identity practices in a
community of nerd girls’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.224–35.
Cheshire, J. (ed.) English around the World. (Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press, 1991) [ISBN 0521395658] pp.200–09.
Jones, D. ‘Gossip: notes on women’s oral culture’ in Cameron, D. (ed.) The
Feminist Critique of Language. (London: Routledge, 1990) first edition
[ISBN 0415042607] pp.242– 50.
Holmes, J. ‘Social constructionism, postmodernism and feminist
sociolinguistics’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.600–10.
Kuiper, K. ‘Sporting formulae in New Zealand English: two models of male
solidarity’ in Labov, W. Language in the Inner City. (Oxford: Blackwell,
1977) [ISBN 9780631176701].
Introduction
Whereas in the previous chapter we focused on the talk between
women and men in what we referred to as ‘mixed-sex interaction’,
the current chapter will present empirical data from and theoretical
approaches to the study of same-sex talk. Early research in the field
of language and gender was dominated by interest in mixed-sex
interaction, but interest in the talk of women in all-female groups and
of men in all-male groups has been strong since the 1980s, although, at
least initially, it frequently retained a comparative perspective, that is, it
investigated both women’s and men’s talk. This comparative approach
to same-sex talk resulted in a now-famous and – for many scholars –
problematic dichotomy, describing all-female talk as collaborative and
all-male talk as competitive.
42
Chapter 4: Same-sex talk and the difference approach
Many of the scholars interested in same-sex talk in the 1980s and 1990s
started to think about the relationship between language and gender
from what we now refer to as a ‘difference perspective’, arguing that
women’s and men’s styles of conversations differ (rather than one being
better or more dominant) as they belong to different sub-cultures
with different conversational practices that we are socialised into in
our childhoods. This ‘cultural difference’ approach to language and
gender studies also offered explanations for conversational problems
or ‘miscommunication’ in mixed-sex interaction without positioning
women as victims of male dominance. It is a model that has remained
highly popular in non-academic writing, particularly for self-help
books on what is perceived as almost inevitable miscommunication
between women and men, whereas within the field of language and
gender, it has been heavily criticised. Of course, interest in same-sex
talk continues up to the present day, although the collaboration vs.
competition binary has been challenged by a number of studies (see
Chapter 5).
Activity
Read ‘Gossip revisited: language in all-female groups’ by Coates (2011), and
consider her examples of women using simultaneous speech in a collaborative
way. Why is the term ‘interruption’ not appropriate when referring to these
examples? What do these examples show us with regard to the multi-
functionality of linguistic forms or strategies such as simultaneous speech?
43
Language and Gender
44
Chapter 4: Same-sex talk and the difference approach
Example 1
Example 2
Example 3
45
Language and Gender
Activity
Now consider the following extract from Coates (1996) p.198. There are
five speakers in the group: Meg, Mary, Jen, Bea and Sally, and their topic of
conversation here turns to child abuse. Find examples of the formal features listed
above and discuss their function.
Example 4
46
Chapter 4: Same-sex talk and the difference approach
However, Goodwin is careful to point out that girls are in fact capable
of issuing bold imperatives, and they do so when this is beneficial
for the addressee (for example, when the safety of a group member
is at stake), and when acting in different roles: for example, as
‘mothers’ when playing ‘house’ (in this context girls show awareness
of hierarchical structures in the family). So although to some extent
Goodwin’s research provides supporting evidence for the claim that
boys adopt a competitive and girls a collaborative conversational
style, she concludes by highlighting that girls’ capability of producing
unmitigated directives contradicts claims that girls avoid direct
competition and negotiation. Gendered language use needs to be
considered in relation to the specific interactional context (for example,
girls playing ‘house’).
For a full discussion of these interesting data and findings on girls’ and
boys’ talk, see Goodwin (2011).
Activity
Read the chapter ‘Asymmetries: women and men talking at cross-purposes’ by
Tannen (2011) and answer the following questions:
1. In what way are Tannen’s arguments representative of (an extreme version) of
the difference approach?
2. What arguments would you present in a critique of Tannen?
Activity
Read Troemel-Ploetz’s (2011) critique of Tannen’s work. Sum up her arguments
and consider your own view.
paying very close attention to what is being said. On the other hand,
there are also clear signs of a competitive verbal style, documented by
interruptions, open contradictions and competitive jokes.
Activity
You should now read Cameron’s (2011) critique of the competitive-collaborative
dichotomy in her paper on young men’s talk. We will return to this paper again
in Chapter 5 when we consider the social constructionist or performative
approach to language and gender, which, in many ways, offers a critique of more
essentialist claims about women’s and men’s language style as ‘collaborative’ vs.
‘competitive’.
This does not mean that there are not very dominant cultural norms
about masculinity and femininity that are reflected in language use.
However, some of the more recent research that we will deal with in
Chapter 5 suggests that these cultural norms are less noticeable at the
level of conversational style than on the level of discourse, that is, the
ideologies that influence the way that people speak or think about
certain topics. Thus, for example, both Deborah Cameron (2011) and
Scott Kiesling (2011) show how important displays of heterosexuality
are for male US college students. These young men tend to present
themselves as traditionally male not only by using a competitive verbal
style, but even more so by boasting about female conquests and
fiercely distancing themselves from what they perceive as 'gay' or, in
other words, inappropriate masculinity.
It is, of course, also necessary to consider that not all women and
men speak in the same way, even if they are from a similar age and
educational background. For example, Nigel Edley and Margaret
Whetherell (1997) investigate the talk of two groups of 17 to18-year old
middle-class male students. They found that one group of young men,
labelled the ‘the Common room guys’, built their identity in opposition
to the hard, ‘chauvinist’ masculinity of the ‘sporty or hard lads’. These
‘Common room guys’ identified themselves as ‘wimps’ but at the same
time praised mental strength over physical strength, thus redefining
being a ‘wimps’ as the new dominant masculinity in their group. For an
account of an alternative young feminine identity, see Mary Bucholtz’s
research on nerd girls (2011), which we will also discuss in Chapter 5.
Learning outcomes
After working through this chapter and having done a substantial
amount of reading on the topic you should:
• have an understanding of the conversational strategies and
functions that have been associated with either a collaborative or a
competitive conversational style
• have developed a critical understanding of the multi-functionality
of conversational features, for example, of the fact that
simultaneous speech can either function as an interruption, or as a
form of collaboration
50
Chapter 4: Same-sex talk and the difference approach
51
Language and Gender
Notes
52
Chapter 5: Constructing gender and sexual identities
Essential reading
Bucholtz, M. ‘Why be normal? Language and identity practices in a
community of nerd girls’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.224–35.
Cameron, D. ‘Performing gender identity: young men’s talk and the
construction of heterosexual masculinity’ in Coates and Pichler (eds)
(2011) pp.250–62.
Eckert, P. ‘Gender and Sociolinguistic variation’ in Coates and Pichler (eds)
(2011) pp.27–37.
Eckert, P. and S. McConnell-Ginet (2011) ‘Communities of Practice: where
language, gender and power all live’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011)
pp.573–82.
Pichler, P. ‘Hybrid or in between cultures: traditions of marriage in a group of
British Bangladeshi girls’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.236–49.
Further reading
Please choose some of the suggestions for further reading from this
section. We have indicated particularly relevant and/or accessible
chapters with an asterisk (*).
Barrett, R. (2011) ‘Indexing polyphonous identity in the speech of African
American drag queens’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.413–29.
Bucholtz, M. and K. Hall ‘Theorizing identity in language and sexuality
research’, Language in Society 33, 2004, pp.469–515.
Cameron, D. ‘Relativity and its discontents: language, gender, and
pragmatics’, Intercultural Pragmatics 2–3, 2005, pp.321–34.
Cameron, D. and D. Kulick Language and Sexuality. (Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press, 2003) [ISBN 9780521009690].
Cameron, D. and D. Kulick (eds) The Language and Sexuality Reader.
(London: Routledge, 2006) [ISBN 9780415363075].
Coates, J. Women, Men and Language. (Harlow: Pearson Longman, 2004)
third edition [ISBN 0582771862] Chapter 12 ‘New developments in
language and gender research’.
* Coates, J. ‘Changing femininities: the talk of teenage girls’ in Bucholtz, M.
et al. (eds) Reinventing Identities: The Gendered Self in Discourse. (Oxford:
Oxford University Press, 1999) [ISBN 9780195126303].
* Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) Part 6 ‘Language, gender and sexuality’.
*Eckert, P. and S. McConnell-Ginet ‘New generalizations and explanations in
language and gender research’, Language in Society 28, 1999, pp.185–201.
*Eckert, P. and McConnell-Ginet. Language and Gender. (Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press, 2003) [ISBN 0521654262] Chapter 1 ‘Constructing,
deconstructing and reconstructing gender’, Chapter 9 ‘Fashioning selves’.
*Eckert, P. and S. McConnell-Ginet ‘Constructing meaning, constructing
selves: Snapshots of language, gender and class from Belten High’ in
Hall, K. and M. Bucholtz (eds) Gender Articulated: Language and the
Socially Constructed Self. (New York: Routledge, 1995)
[ISBN 9780415913997] pp.469–508.
53
Language and Gender
References
Bergvall, V. ‘Toward a comprehensive theory of language and gender’,
Language in Society 28, 1999, pp.273–93.
Coates, J. Women Talk: Conversation between Women Friends. (Oxford:
Blackwell, 1996) [ISBN 9780631195955].
Bucholtz, M. and K. Hall ‘Theorizing identity in language and sexuality
research’, Language in Society 33, 2004, pp.469–515.
Butler, J. Gender Trouble: Feminism and the Subversion of Identity. (New York:
Routledge, 1990) [ISBN 9780415900430].
Eder, D. ‘Serious and playful disputes: variation in conflict talk among
female adolescents’ in Grimshaw, A. (ed.) Conflict Talk. (Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press, 1990) [ISBN 0521335507] pp.67–84.
Eder, D. ‘Go get ya a french!’: romantic and sexual teasing among
adolescent girls’ in Tannen, D. (ed.) Gender and Conversational
Interaction. (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1993)
[ISBN 9780195081947] pp.17–31.
Hasund, I. K. and A.-B. Stenström ‘Conflict talk: a comparison of the
verbal disputes between adolescent females in two corpora’, Corpus-
based studies in English. Papers from the Seventeenth International
Conference on English Language Research on Computerized Corpora.
(Amsterdam: Rodopi, 1997) pp.119–133.
54
Chapter 5: Constructing gender and sexual identities
Introduction
In this chapter we will focus on ways of thinking about language and
gender that have dominated the last decades in the field. First we will
consider why it is problematic to make generalisations about ‘women’s
speech’ or ‘men’s speech’. Gender can interact with many other social
aspects of speakers’ identities, for example, with their ethnic, social
class and age background. Gender is also influenced by the practices
and views of smaller groups that individuals are members of, such as
their families, friendship or other ‘Communities of Practice’ (CofP) they
belong to.
The so-called ‘social constructionist’ model, which we introduce in this
chapter, allows researchers to highlight this heterogeneity of gender,
the fact that not all women and men use language in the same way. It
is sometimes seen as the fourth phase in language and gender studies,
following deficit, dominance and difference models (see Chapters 2 to 4).
55
Language and Gender
56
Chapter 5: Constructing gender and sexual identities
Activity
Does this mean that factors such as social class, ethnicity or age pre-determine
the language use of women and men? For example, do all adult black British
middle-class women speak the same? Why not?
Activity
Eder (1990) and Hasund and Stenström (1997) as well as Pichler (2009) show that
gender interacts with social class, but Pichler’s explanation is very much framed
by social constructionist thinking. Using the quotation from Pichler and from
what you have read so far, try to explain the relationship between language,
gender and other social variables from a social constructionist approach. Then
check with the explanation offered in the following section.
57
Language and Gender
levels; for example, Coates noted that when the girls are 12 years old
they change their topics rapidly, interrupt each other, express their
disagreement openly and produce few supportive minimal responses.
By the time they are 14 years old they seem to have appropriated
conversational behaviour that has frequently been associated with
adult white middle-class British women, sustaining one topic over
several minutes, producing many minimal responses and supportive
rather than disruptive overlaps (Coates, 1999, pp.133–36).
Activity
Compare the following two extracts from the girls’ talk from Coates (1999). Which
one is likely to represent the girls’ talk when they were younger, and which one
when they were older? Give reasons to support your choice. For transcription
conventions return to Chapter 2. Remember, for example, that all the words that
are aligned within one stave indicate simultaneous speech. The square brackets
capture the exact onset and end of the simultaneous speech.
Extract 1
58
Chapter 5: Constructing gender and sexual identities
Extract 2
Extract 2 contains a sample of the girls' talk when they are older. As Coates
(1999, p.136) sums up: 'The girls don't change topic frequently any longer, this
turn-taking is now very collaborative, with frequent use of minimal responses,
supportive simultaneous talk and hedges'.
59
Language and Gender
Activity
After reading Eckert and McConnell-Ginet’s chapter ‘Communities of Practice:
where language, gender and power all live’ consider which Communities of
Practice you are a member of. What are the non-linguistic and linguistic practices
that define these CofPs? Are you aware of how your membership in these
Communities of Practice influences you? In what way may it affect how you
present yourself as either masculine or feminine?
60
Chapter 5: Constructing gender and sexual identities
Table 5.1
Table adapted from Bucholtz (2011, p.226). Note that ‘discourse’ is used
differently in this table from how it is used in the remainder of this
chapter. Bucholtz uses it here to refer to discourse or language/style.
As this list of features shows, nerd girls aim for standard grammatical
and pronunciation forms and witty use of language (for example,
puns). These linguistic practices (together with a range of non-linguistic
practices such as their choice of books, clothing and leisure-time
activities) allow the girls to ‘construct’ a nerd identity which, in many
ways, stands in opposition to dominant models of young femininity.
As Bucholtz (2011, p.227) argues, ‘[f ]or girls, nerd identity also offers an
alternative to the pressures of hegemonic femininity – an ideological
construct that is at best incompatible with, and at worst hostile to,
female intellectual ability’. Reading Bucholtz’s chapter will allow you
to develop your understanding of the language practices and identity
work carried out by nerd girls.
This notion that speakers use linguistic and other resources to
‘construct’ their identities is central to the social constructionist
approach to gender, which we consider in the following section.
61
Language and Gender
(although many women, do, of course, swear). So, we could say that
a woman who wants to present or construct herself as feminine in a
traditional sense may choose to avoid swearing. Equally, if we would
like to construct an alternative (resistant or perhaps simply tough or
cool) feminine identity, women may choose to swear more, use more
non-standard grammatical or accent features.
This approach attempts to move away from ‘gender dichotomy’, that
is, binary and static notions of gender, which have frequently been
labelled ‘essentialist’ (but see critiques of the negative connotations
of ‘essentialist’ in Holmes, 2011). Instead, the ‘social constructionist
approach’ links language and gender studies to postmodern thinking
on identity as something that is not predetermined and fixed but
constructed. Linguists are, unsurprisingly, particularly interested in
the language resources that speakers use for these constructions of
gender identities, although individuals can use many other modalities,
including clothing, hairstyle as well as body posture and movement.
Activity
Does this mean that ‘anything goes’, that is, that speakers can and do choose to
present themselves exactly as they wish? Consider the following quotation from
Judith Butler (1990) to answer these questions. ‘Gender is the repeated stylization
of the body, a set of repeated acts within a rigid regulatory frame which congeal
over time to produce the appearance of substance, of a ‘natural’ kind of being
(Butler, 1990, p.33)’. You will first have to reflect on what Butler means when she
speaks of a ‘rigid regulatory frame’ and of ‘acts […] which congeal over time to
produce the appearance of […] a ‘natural’ kind of being’.
62
Chapter 5: Constructing gender and sexual identities
Indexicality
For language and gender scholars, the question about which linguistic
‘acts’ or practices constitute gender is an important one. Elinor Ochs
highlights that it is very rare for language features to ‘index’ gender
directly:
…the relation between language and gender is not a simple
straightforward mapping of linguistic form to social meaning
of gender. Rather the relation of language and gender
is constituted and mediated by the relation of language
to stances, social acts, social activities, and other social
constructs.
(Ochs,1992, pp.336–37)
Ochs (1992, p.341) gives an example of this ‘indirect indexicality’
from Japanese, where linguistic features that index the stance ‘coarse
intensity’ are used to constitute a speaker as male, whereas features
which index ‘delicate intensity’ are associated with and therefore
position speakers as women. If we return to our example of swearing
this means that swearing itself does not constitute masculinity, but it
indexes first and foremost toughness or coarseness. On a second level
we can then argue that toughness and coarseness signal masculinity
(but also many other aspects of social identity, such as working-class
membership), so that a ‘performance’ of conventional femininity may
well be indexed by opposition to or lack of swearing.
Discourses
Linguistic resources are, however, not restricted to grammatical and
conversational features. Many language and gender scholars have
been particularly interested in what they refer to as ‘discourses’. There
are many different definitions of ‘discourse’, but the way it is understood
here is as an ideology, a way of thinking and seeing the world, reflected
in language. For example, a speaker could be influenced very much
by dominant ideologies which see men as the breadwinner and not as
the prime carer of children. A careful, in-depth analysis of this speaker’s
talk about subjects such as work, parenting, or gender equality could
reveal a range of different discourses (that is, ways of speaking that
reflect ideologies) on masculinity, fatherhood, gender roles, etc. These
discourses shape the (gender) identities that individuals construct in
their spoken (and also written) language use.
Coates sums up this relation between discourse(s) and feminine gender
identity in the following way:
‘...there is no single unified way of doing femininity, of being
a woman. In the contemporary developed world, many
different versions of femininity are available to us. More
mainstream discourses position us in more conventional
ways, while more radical or subversive discourses offer
us alternative ways of being, alternative ways of doing
femininity. We are unwittingly involved in the ceaseless
struggle to define gender...’
(Coates, 1996, p.261)
63
Language and Gender
Activity
There are many different discourses that speakers can draw on when they
speak about a topic; frequently these are competing discourses, that is, they
are informed by very different and even opposing ideologies. Read Pichler
(2011) ‘Hybrid or in between cultures: traditions of marriage in a group of British
Bangladeshi girls’ and study the following extract ‘Kissing in public’ from the same
group of friends. Then consider which (competing) discourses the four British
Bangladeshi girls draw on when they talk about traditions of marriage and kissing
in the street. What types of gender identities are the girls constructing on the
basis of these discourses?
64
Chapter 5: Constructing gender and sexual identities
66
Chapter 5: Constructing gender and sexual identities
Activity
Read Cameron’s (2011) chapter and then try to answer the following questions,
which will also be helpful in your summary of the main issues addressed in this
chapter.
• In what way does this chapter challenge traditional views on and approaches
to language and gender?
• What is performativity (see also Butler’s quote above)?
• Does the label ‘gay’ really mean ‘homosexuality’ when it is used by the young
men? What type of masculinity is being performed by the young men?
• How is this performance accomplished by them, that is, what linguistic and
discursive resources do they draw on?
• How do gender and sexuality interact in the identity-performance of these
young men?
• What is Cameron’s view of the notion of gendered conversational styles?
To answer these questions also consider the following extract and quotation
from the chapter: ‘[..] I hope it makes us think twice about the sort of analysis that
implicitly sees the meaning (and sometimes the value) of an interaction among
men or women primarily in the style, rather than the substance, of what is said’
(Cameron, 2011, p.261).
67
Language and Gender
Learning outcomes
After working through this chapter and having done a substantial
amount of reading on the topic you should:
• have an understanding of the relationship between language,
gender and sexuality
• have an understanding of the relationship between language,
gender and other (sociocultural) aspects of identity, such as social
class, ethnicity and age
• be able to discuss the social constructionist approach to language
and gender critically, contrasting it with previous explanatory
frameworks
• have read and reflected on a range of Communities of Practice
(CofP) studies on language and gender
• have developed a critical understanding of concepts such as
‘indexicality’, ‘performativity’ and ‘discourse’.
68
Chapter 5: Constructing gender and sexual identities
5. What has research and theory on language and sexuality been able
to offer language and gender studies?
6. In what ways can sexuality be said to be intertwined with gender?
Your answer needs to be based on a critical discussion of language,
gender and sexuality research and theory.
69
Language and Gender
Notes
70
Chapter 6: Language and gender in public settings
Essential reading
Ehrlich, S. ‘Trial discourse and judicial decision-making: constraining the
boundaries of gendered identities’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011)
pp.356–70.
Holmes, J. and S. Schnurr ‘Doing ‘femininity’ at work: more than just
relational practice’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.315–31.
Reynolds, K.A. ‘Female speakers of Japanese in transition’ in Coates and
Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.291–99.
Shaw, S. ‘Governed by the rules? The female voice in parliamentary debates’
in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.300–14.
Talbot, M. ‘Public Talk’ in Talbot, M. Language and Gender. (Malden, MA:
Polity Press, 2010) second edition [ISBN 9780745646053] pp.184–203.
West, C. ‘When the doctor is a lady: power, status and gender in physician-
patient encounters’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.468–82.
Further reading
Baxter, J. (ed.) Speaking Out: The Female Voice in Public Contexts.
(Houndmills: Palgrave Macmillan, 2006) [ISBN 9781403994080].
Cameron, D. ‘Theorising the female voice in public contexts’ in Baxter, J.
(ed.) Speaking Out: The Female Voice in Public Contexts. (Houndmills:
Palgrave Macmillan, 2006) [ISBN 9781403994080] pp.3–20.
Carli, L. ‘Gender, language, and influence’, Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 59(5), 1990, pp.941–51.
Coates, J. ‘The social consequences of gender differences in language’ in
Coates, J. Women, Men and Language. (Harlow: Pearson Longman, 2004)
third edition [ISBN 9780582771864].
Coates, J. ‘Language, gender and career’ in Mills, S. (ed.) Language and
Gender: Interdisciplinary Perspectives. (Longman, 1995)
[ISBN 0582226317].
Crawford, M. Talking Difference: on Gender and Language. (London: Sage,
1995) [ISBN 9780803988286].
Edelsky, C. ‘Who’s got the floor?’ in Tannen, D. (ed.) Gender and
Conversational Interaction. (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1993)
[ISBN 9780195081943] pp.189–226.
Herring, S. et al. ‘Participation in electronic discourse in a “feminist field” in
Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.171–82.
Holmes, J. ‘Women’s talk in public contexts’, Discourse and Society 3 (2),
1992, pp.131–50.
Holmes, J. ‘Social constructionism, postmodernism and feminist
sociolinguistics’ in Coates and Pichler (eds) (2011) pp.600–10.
Holmes, J. ‘Men, masculinities and leadership: different discourse styles
at work’ in Pichler, P. and E. Eppler (eds) Gender and Spoken Interaction.
(Houndmills: Palgrave Macmillan, 2009) [ISBN 0230574025] pp.186–210.
Holmes, J. and M. Stubbe ‘‘Feminine’ workplaces: stereotype and reality’
in Holmes, J. and M. Meyerhoff (eds) The Handbook of Language and
71
Language and Gender
References
Eakins, B. and G. Eakins Sex Differences in Human Communication. (Boston,
MA: Houghton Mifflin, 1978) [ISBN 9780395255100].
Goffman, E. Frame Analysis. (New York: Harper & Row, 1974) [ISBN
9780140551099].
Goffman, E. Forms of Talk. (Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, 1981) [ISBN
9780631128861] ‘Footing’, pp.124–57.
Lakoff, R. ‘Why can’t a woman be less like a man?’ in Lakoff, R. Talking
Power: the Politics of Language. (San Francisco: Basic Books, 1990)
[ISBN 9780465083596].
Tannen, D. (ed.) Framing in Discourse. (Oxford: Oxford University Press,
1993) [ISBN 9780195079968].
Introduction
With the exception of Chapter 3, most of the previous chapters tended
to focus on language and gender in informal, conversational settings
among friends and couples. Indeed, interest in language and gender in
work and other public settings has become particularly strong only in
the last decade, and has resulted in an increasingly substantial body of
research (see suggestions for reading).
In this chapter we will begin by examining how public talk is different
from informal everyday conversations. We will consider some of the
72
Chapter 6: Language and gender in public settings
73
Language and Gender
Activity
Which of these two functions of talk is predominant in the public sphere and
which in the private sphere? Is it correct to assume that in public contexts talk
always functions in one way, and in private contexts in another? Or can you think
of situations where both functions are relevant, to similar or different degrees?
The activity above will have helped you to understand that it depends
very much on context which function of talk is predominant in a
specific situation. Speakers in public and institutional contexts can
draw on the rapport-building or social function of talk, for example,
when tutors engage with students just before or after a formal
lecture, or even when they joke or reveal personal anecdotes during
a seminar or tutorial. However, it is true to say that much public or
institutional talk exemplifies its referential function: just think of a cross-
examination in court or a police interview, a job interview, an academic
conference or any other public meeting, a medical examination, an
exchange in Parliament or a political debate. (If you wish to refine your
understanding of how functions of talk can shift during an interactional
exchange you may want to read up on Erving Goffman’s (1974, 1981)
notions of conversational frames, refined in Tannen’s 1993 work).
Traditionally women have been associated with the private sphere, and
men with the public. This means that women have had less access to
and practice in talking in public, and therefore in talking that is about
report rather than rapport, in creating or maintaining status rather than
equal social relationships. As Coates (2004, p.197) argues, ‘[t]the public
domain is a male-dominated domain, and the discourse patterns of
male speakers have become the established norm in public life’. The
continuing relevance of and historical link between ‘public/private’ and
‘male/female’ binaries and their relationship to language and discourse
practices is discussed also by Cameron (2006) and Talbot (2010).
Even in the twenty-first century many public domains, including
academia, businesses and the British Parliament, are still male
dominated. Nevertheless, it is also true to say that in the twenty-first
century it is not as unusual for women to be found in these public
arenas as it once was. It is therefore interesting to consider if this
change has affected interactional patterns at work and in other public
and institutional contexts, or whether the old dichotomy of male/
public = referential/competitive talk and women/private = rapport/
collaborative talk still holds.
74
Chapter 6: Language and gender in public settings
75
Language and Gender
Activity
The question these studies encourage us to ask is: does gender override
occupational status or vice versa? Or, perhaps we should ask to what extent
and in what ways gender interacts with occupational status? Try to answer
these questions on the basis of your essential reading and some further reading
suggested above.
Activity
On the basis of the following extracts discuss the different conversational styles
employed by male leaders as described by Holmes (2009). What are the linguistic/
discursive characteristics of these styles? How do the styles allow male leaders to
‘do’ masculinity in different ways in their workplaces?
Janet’s Holmes does not use a version of the stave transcript and although many
of her transcription conventions are similar to the ones introduced in Chapter 1 of
this subject guide, there are some differences. The following is taken from Holmes
(2009, p.206):
yes Underlining indicates emphatic stress
[laughs] : : Paralinguistic features and other information in square
brackets, colons indicate start/finish
+ Pause of up to one second
... //......\ ... Simultaneous speech
... /.......\\ ...
() Unclear utterance
(hello) Transcriber’s best guess at an unclear utterance
? Rising or question intonation
- Utterance cut off
… Section of transcript omitted
XM/XF Unidentified Male/Female
[voc] Untranscribable noise
tangi [‘funeral’] Maori words appear in italics with translation in
square brackets
77
Language and Gender
78
Chapter 6: Language and gender in public settings
Activity
Now read Janet Holmes and Stephanie Schnurr (2011) and give examples of how
femininity is being done and judged in different (feminine and masculine) work
environments, or ‘Communities of Practice’.
Activity
Do you think that this dilemma also exists for women in other cultures and
languages? Can men find themselves in similar dilemmas?
79
Language and Gender
‘filibustering’, that is, by speaking for so long that the other party does
not have any time to debate the issue. Shaw finds that 90 per cent
of these illegal interventions are produced by men; moreover, these
transgressions are usually left unchallenged, so it is fair to say that
‘this type of rule-breaking has to some degree been accepted as a
masculine ‘norm’ in debates’ (2011, p.308). In statistical terms women
are outsiders in the context of the British Parliament and their struggle
to adapt to the androcentric interactional practices of this specific
Community of Practice confirms their status as outsiders.
Activity
Read Susan Ehrlich (2011). Then consider to what extent an androcentric view is
expressed indirectly by both the trial judge and the Court of Appeal judge of a
sexual assault trial in Canada. Consider particularly how the judges equate a lack
of the victim’s verbal refusals to have sex as signs of consensual sex, whereas the
victim and attorney invoke an alternative, feminist discourse, which positions the
victim’s act of compliance as a way of preventing more extreme and prolonged
physical violence against her.
Activity
Consider the following table (adapted from Carli, 1990). How were tentative and
assertive men and women judged by other men and women with regard to
trustworthiness and likeability? Did men and women agree in their judgement
of tentative and assertive female speakers? Consider the findings and discuss the
possible reasons for the different ratings.
Table 6.1
Trustworthiness Likeability
Male judge Female judge Male judge Female judge
Male speaker
– tentative language 6.73 6.73 5.93 6.53
– assertive language 6.80 7.00 6.40 6.93
Female speaker
– tentative language 8.40 5.47 8.00 5.80
– assertive language 6.33 7.27 6.53 7.73
80
Chapter 6: Language and gender in public settings
Learning outcomes
After working through this chapter and having done a substantial
amount of reading on the topic you should:
• be able to discuss conversational strategies and functions that
have been associated with talk in a wide range of work, public and
institutional contexts.
• have an understanding why it can be problematic to claim that talk
at work is always report talk, serving referential purposes, and talk in
informal contexts always rapport talk, serving social functions.
• have read and reflected on a range of empirical studies on language
and gender in public settings.
• have studied the relationship between gender and occupational
status with respect to language use.
• have examined empirical evidence of attitudes towards women’s
and men’s transgressions of normative femininity and masculinity in
work contexts.
81
Language and Gender
Notes
82
Appendix 1: Sample examination paper
Notes
84
Appendix 2: Sample Examiner’s report
85
Language and Gender
Introduction
A key feature of the English programme is that every subject guide
contains a past examination paper from a recent year, as a sample
to students new to the course, and a copy of the corresponding
Examiners’ report. Additionally, students can access an archive of past
papers and Examiners’ reports on the VLE.
Since Language and Gender is a new course launching in the
2013–2014 academic year, there are neither any past papers or
past Examiner’s reports. However, as this guide includes a Sample
examination paper so that students can familiarise themselves with the
format and types of questions likely to be asked, it seems only fair to
offer some sample Examiner’s comments regarding some of the ways
in which the questions might be approached.
Please note: there are no ‘model answers’ to these, or any other
questions, on the English programme – English is not an information-
based subject, but rather one that invites debate, argument and
exploration based on research both into text and context. Thus, what
follows here are some pointers and suggestions about how you might
tackle the kinds of questions asked in the Sample examination paper in
order to help you prepare for assessment.
General remarks
Make sure you familiarise yourself with the Learning outcomes and
assessment criteria for this course (see the beginning of the subject
guide) and with all the Learning outcomes for each individual chapter.
This will help you to understand Examiners’ expectations. Also
remember that the Examiners will look for your critical understanding
of research, theory and methodology as well as for evidence that you
have engaged in in-depth, independent study. Simple regurgitation of
the material in your subject guide will not be looked upon favourably,
and may even constitute plagiarism (academic dishonesty).
Question 2
Many scholars have been accused of claiming that gender is relevant
in a particular stretch of conversational data, without having explicitly
engaged with the question of why they think gender is relevant in
these data. For example, when we consider a stretch of talk produced
by a group of female friends, are we justified in saying that the way
they talk is gendered, that they engage in ‘women’s conversational
style’, just because they are women? What if we find that different
groups of women differ in their conversational practice? To answer
these questions we need to think about ‘warrants of analysis’ and, in
relation to that, questions of methodology. Any attempts to this answer
must make sure that what is meant by ‘warrants of analysis’ has been
fully understood. A good way to make sure this understanding has
been gained is to turn to Joan Swann, ‘Yes, but is it gender?’ in Coates
and Pichler (2011), pp 161–70. This is Essential reading for Chapter 2 of
your subject guide and, together with Chapter 2 and, in particular, the
section ‘Analysing gender in spoken interaction’ will allow candidates
to gain a full understanding of the question. Candidates could, for
example, choose to focus on what Swann calls ‘analysts’ intuitions’,
‘quantitative patterns’ or ‘participants’ orientations as evident in the
text’. The critical discussion of these (or any other) three ‘warrants
of analysis’ will need to be exemplified by reference to relevant and
representative research. Not many studies are explicit about the
‘warrants of analysis’ they rely on or, in other words, which are explicit
about the kind of evidence scholars cite to support their claims that the
way in which speakers use language in this situation has something
to do with their gender. This means that candidates attempting this
question will need to study, examine and reflect on their selection of
language and gender research very carefully and critically in order
to be able to come to any conclusions about ‘warrants of analysis’
employed by the researchers. Candidates should also be aware that
frequently scholars will not only rely on one but on a combination of
‘warrants’.
Question 3
This question requires candidates to deal with what has become
known as the form-and function problem in language and gender
research, and in research on conversational practice in general. It
invites candidates to reflect on the multi-functionality of linguistic
forms and, in particular, on the various functions and meanings of
87
Language and Gender
Question 4
This question requires candidates to engage with Conversation
Analysis (CA) and, in particular, offer critical insight into and discussion
of CA theory and research on turn-taking. Many language and gender
scholars have drawn on the methodological approach of CA in their
discussion of naturally occurring conversational data. The second
part of the question therefore directs candidates to consider the
contributions that CA research and theory on turn-taking has made to
language and gender studies. For example, some of the most classic
studies on mixed-sex interaction adopted concepts and insights from
the turn-taking model. Several of these studies were particularly
interested in exploring whether what they saw as male dominance of
women in society at large was reflected on a micro-level in everyday
conversational interaction. Candidates attempting this question
will need to consider carefully to what extent and in what ways the
turn-taking model has allowed language and gender scholars to
find answers to this and other questions, and in what other ways the
turn-taking model has shaped language and gender research. One
obvious way to answer the question would be to follow the general
introduction to the turn-taking model with a critical discussion
of a small number of language and gender studies that rely very
explicitly on CA and the turn-taking model. Very good answers will be
particularly aware of the limitations of what the turn-taking model (and
CA) has to offer to language and gender studies.
Question 5
This question invites candidates to signal their awareness of how
problematic generalisations about gendered language use can be.
All answers would need to present an initial (critical) summary of this,
most famous, dichotomy in language and gender studies. That is,
candidates would have to demonstrate their understanding of what
characterises ‘collaborative’ and ‘competitive’ conversational styles.
Candidates would also have to consider the origins of this dichotomy
and its impact on language and gender studies. Above all, answers to
this question would have to present a careful and critical discussion
of research that either supports or challenges this ‘collaborative’ vs.
‘competitive’ dichotomy. Very good answers will link this discussion of
research to a discussion of the development of the field of language
and gender and, hand in hand with this, a discussion of the different
88
Appendix 2: Sample Examiner’s report
Question 6
Any attempt to answer this question would have to distinguish
carefully between stereotypical belief and empirical research on
(women’s) gossip, although a consideration of the former can be useful,
as long as it is then critically compared with the findings of the latter. In
order to answer the first part of the question, candidates need to draw
on research and/or data that contains instances of gossiping. A very
accessible, though in-depth, discussion of gossip is offered by Jennifer
Coates in one of your Essential readings for Chapter 4: ‘Gossip revisited:
language in all-female groups’ in Coates and Pichler (2011). One of
the most famous papers that confirms but also challenges quite a few
stereotypes of gendered language use, and also discusses examples of
male gossip, is Deborah Cameron’s ‘Performing gender identity: young
men’s talk and the construction of heterosexual masculinity’, also in
Coates and Pichler (2011.). One possible way of answering all parts of
this question would be to draw on these (or any other) two papers,
offering a critical and comparative discussion of the use and functions
of gossiping and its relation to gendered language use. Excellent
answers would offer a discussion of language and gender research that
may not focus on gossiping explicitly, but which may have nevertheless
produced data that contains instances of gossip for candidates to
identify and interpret.
Question 7
There is both a vibrant popular and lucrative commercial interest
in women’s and men’s conversational practices, especially in what
are seen as examples of – and reasons for – alleged male-female
miscommunication. This interest is well documented in popular
literature, for example in the genre of the self-help book on gendered
miscommunication. One way to approach this question would
be to study some examples of these claims about male-female
miscommunication in this or other type(s) of popular literature from a
critical, feminist linguistic perspective. Questions that could be asked
include: What claims are made about women’s and men’s language
use? To what extent are these claims supported by the empirical
evidence from language and gender research? To what extent do these
claims simply reflect popular stereotypes? In what other ways can some
of these claims seen to be problematic? Who is the target audience
of these self-help books (or other types of popular literature)? If they
are targeted at women, what does this tell us about gender norms?
An example of an extract from one such popular text about women’s
and men’s ‘miscommunication’ is reprinted in one of the Essential
readings for this course (see Tannen in Coates and Pichler, 2011), as is
an example of a feminist critique of Tannen’s popular work on gender
(see Troeml-Ploetz in Coates and Pichler, 2011). So a critical summary
of both these chapters, and especially of Troeml-Ploetz’s critique,
would offer a good foundation to answer this question. However, good
answers will discuss a range of different material and arguments, well
beyond what has been suggested in your Essential reading.
89
Language and Gender
Question 8
Many recent language and gender studies highlight that it can be
problematic to claim that all women and all men speak in the same
way. These studies suggest, for example, that previous findings on the
language use of white, middle-class, middle-aged women may not be
representative of other groups of women, for example, of adolescent
British Bangladeshi girls from a working-class background. Moreover,
sociolinguistic research has long established that individual speakers
change the way they speak depending on the situation they find
themselves in, so that it can be assumed that a woman praising her
child will adopt a different conversational style from the same women
when she chairs a board meeting in her company. In order to be able
to produce a valid essay for this question, candidates would have to
examine a selection of research that explores how language use is
not only gendered, but also how gender is related to, for example,
social class, ethnicity, job role and situational or historical context.
One of the recent concepts that has helped scholars to consider how
gender interacts with other sociocultural and situational factors is the
‘Communities of Practice’ (CofP), which candidates may find useful
to discuss in their answers. Excellent candidates would also have to
display an in-depth understanding of postmodern gender (and queer)
theory and its impact on language and gender studies, for example,
of gender as performative or, in other words, as gender as both
constructed in discourse and conversational interaction and shaped
by ideologies. Chapter 5 of your subject guide (‘Constructing gender
and sexual identities’) will provide you with a good starting point to
research this topic further.
Question 9
Over the last two decades language and gender research has been
dominated by what is frequently labelled the ‘social constructionist
approach’. Other terms used (almost) synonymously include
‘postmodern’, ‘performative’ and ‘dynamic’. What this approach and
these different labels highlight are both the heterogeneity and the
agency of (gendered) speakers. Any answer to this question would
need to offer a critical discussion of the social constructionist approach
to language and gender, exemplified by reference to a selection of
language and gender studies (which, for example, demonstrate that
not all women speak in the same way and that speakers have some
agency in negotiating their gender identities in everyday interaction).
The question requires students to contrast the social constructionist
approach to language and gender with other, earlier approaches (or
explanations). This comparison will allow students to become even
clearer about what distinguishes the social constructionist approach
from the others. Excellent answers will, however, also be able to
demonstrate that not all aspects of the social constructionist model are
entirely new or different from previous approaches, and will be able to
critique not only the previous approaches to language and gender, but
also the social constructionist model itself. The theoretical discussion
should be supported by examples from empirical language and gender
studies throughout. One way of achieving this would be for candidates
to select one representative example of empirical research for each
90
Appendix 2: Sample Examiner’s report
Question 10
This question directs candidates to base their answers on a discussion
of at least two language and gender studies that use the Community
of Practice Approach. Candidates will need to engage carefully
and critically with these two studies in order to be able to gain in-
depth understanding of what the concept of the CofP has to offer
to language and gender studies. Answers will consider the question
with respect to data (collection), methodology and relevant (language
and gender) theory, among others. Good answers will also be able to
outline any limitations of this concept and of studies that adopt the
CofP approach. There is plenty of reading on the CofP in language and
gender studies, both theoretical and empirical, and several of these
papers are reprinted in Coates and Pichler (2011). As always, candidates
are expected to read well beyond the Essential readings, and excellent
answers will demonstrate a candidate’s ability to source reading from a
range of relevant sources, including academic journals.
Question 11
The question of whether gender is more or less important than speaker
status has interested scholars from the very beginning of language
and gender studies. O’Barr and Atkins’ study, based on courtroom data,
focused on this question as early as in the late 1970s. In particular,
they were interested in ascertaining whether what Lakoff had labelled
‘women’s language’ just a few years earlier, was, indeed, gendered
language or simply ‘powerless’ language of speakers with lower social
status (and/or less courtroom experience). The focus of this early
study was on what the scholars called ‘social status’; however, this was
clearly intertwined with occupational status. This is one of the studies
that candidates could draw on in their answers to this question. There
are many subsequent studies on work or other public settings which
equally investigate the question whether conversational style (and, in
particular, conversational dominance) is due to the gender or to the
occupational status of the speaker. Moreover, the last two decades
of language and gender studies have seen a surge of interest in talk
in public contexts, so even if not all of these studies will address the
above question explicitly, or focus specifically on conversational
dominance, many will provide candidates with useful material to
answer this question. Weaker answers would seek to make an absolute
generalisation about whether gender overrides occupational status
or vice versa. Better essays will acknowledge that these types of
generalisations are problematic. Excellent answers will show in-depth
understanding of the complex (inter)relation between gender and
occupational status (as well as a range of other sociocultural and
situational factors).
91
Language and Gender
Question 12
This question is almost identical to Question 5, except that it requests
candidates to focus on talk at work or other public and institutional
settings. It invites candidates to signal their awareness of how
problematic generalisations about gendered language use can be.
All answers would need to present an initial (critical) summary of this,
most famous, dichotomy in language and gender studies. That is,
candidates would have to demonstrate their understanding of what
characterises ‘collaborative’ and ‘competitive’ conversational styles.
Candidates would also have to consider the origins of this dichotomy
and its impact on language and gender studies. Above all, answers to
this question would have to present a careful and critical discussion
of language and gender research in work/public settings that either
supports or challenges this ‘collaborative’ vs. ‘competitive’ dichotomy.
Very good answers will link this discussion of research to a discussion
of the development of the field of language and gender and, hand in
hand with this, a discussion of the different ‘approaches’ to language
and gender, for example, of the ‘difference’ and of the ‘constructionist
approach’.
Question 13
This question directs candidates to the area of what sociolinguists
call ‘attitude studies’. That is, this question is not so much about how
men and women actually do use language, but about how women’s
and men’s use of language is evaluated by others. Frequently this
type of research requires some degree of experiment-like setting, for
example, presenting a range of ‘judges’ with samples of ‘assertive’ and
‘non-assertive’ speech that needs to be evaluated. This question also
allows students to think about gender norms. Questions that students
will need to think about include: Are assertive women still perceived
in more negative terms than assertive men? And, vice-versa, are
unassertive men still perceived more negatively than assertive men?
What is the empirical evidence for this, that is, what ‘attitude’ research
is there, and what type of data has been generated by this research
to allow us to seek informed answers to these questions? Chapter 6,
and, especially the section on ‘Attitudes towards assertive women’,
will provide students with a good starting point for this question. One
possible way of demonstrating academic excellence with this question
would be for candidates to make successful use of relevant empirical
data from studies that do not aim to investigate language attitudes
but explore women’s and men’s talk in public settings and provide
critical readers with data from which evaluations of women’s verbal
assertiveness and men’s non-assertiveness could be extracted. For
example, this could be data that captures the public talk of women
leaders and the reactions of others to this talk in spontaneous and
naturally occurring settings.
92