Models of Adult Learning
Models of Adult Learning
a literature review
Reference: Tusting, K. & D. Barton (2003) Models of Adult Learning. National Research and Development
Centre for Adult Literacy and Numeracy (NRDC). 45 pages.
Crown Copyright.
Preface
This book was originally published as a research review by the National
Research and Development Centre for Adult Literacy and Numeracy (NRDC).
The work was funded by the Department for Education and Skills in England
as part of Skills for Life: the national strategy for improving adult literacy and
numeracy skills. The original NRDC project was directed by David Barton and
Diana Coben and we are grateful to Diana Coben and Rachel Hodge for
detailed comments on earlier drafts of the original review.
We are pleased that the review can now reach a wider audience, both of
people working in adult language, literacy and numeracy, and also in the field
of adult learning more generally.
The views expressed are ours, as authors, and do not necessarily reflect
those of the Department for Education and Skills.
November 2005
1
CONTENTS Page
Summary ...................................................... 3
Introduction ................................................... 6
Models from psychology ............................... 8
Behaviourism .................................................................................... 8
Cognitivism ....................................................................................... 9
Cognitive constructivism ................................................................. 12
Developmental theories .................................................................. 13
Activity theory and social constructivism ......................................... 18
Situated cognition ........................................................................... 20
Brain science .................................................................................. 26
Models from adult education....................... 27
Distinctive characteristics of adult learning ..................................... 28
Humanistic psychology ................................................................... 30
Critiques of andragogy .................................................................... 31
Self-directed learning ...................................................................... 34
Learning how to learn ..................................................................... 37
Informal learning ............................................................................. 39
Reflective and experiential models ................................................. 40
Transformative learning .................................................................. 46
Postmodern perspectives ............................................................... 48
Models of learning in contexts of rapid change 50
Summary and conclusions ......................... 53
References ................................................. 58
2
Summary
Ideas about what learning is and how it is achieved are central to all aspects of
adult learning, including work in adult basic skills. Theories of learning provide a
starting point for principles of teaching. Any curriculum or training course has
views of learning built into it and any teaching plan is based upon a view of how
people learn.
Most educational research is on children and most views of learning have been
developed in the context of children learning within a formal educational system.
Inevitably such views have been tied into child development and compulsory
schooling. In contrast, this book is a review of models of learning that have
focused on adults. It is a wide-ranging review and covers ideas from many fields
about how adults learn: the aim is to provide ideas that are useful for research into
teaching and learning. The theories covered are ones that have proved useful in
relation to the education of adults.
3
The key ideas about how adults learn are that:
1. Adults have their own motivations for learning. Learners build on their existing
knowledge and experience. They fit learning into their own purposes and become
engaged in it. People’s purposes for learning are related to their real lives and the
practices and roles they engage in outside the classroom.
3. Adults have the ability to learn about their own learning processes, and can
benefit from discussion and reflection on this. They are able to learn how to learn.
For instance, there are different learning styles that people synthesise. Teaching
can enable learners to develop their range of learning styles.
5. Adults reflect and build upon their experience. Reflective learning is generated
when people encounter problems and issues in their real lives and think about
ways of resolving them.
6. Reflective learning is unique to each person, since it arises out of the complexities
of their own experience. A great deal of learning is incidental and idiosyncratically
related to the learner: it cannot be planned in advance. While there are things that
can be done to encourage reflective experiential learning, there is no set of steps
that can be followed to guarantee it will happen.
4
Introduction
This book summarises a wide-ranging review of literature on adult learning,
drawing out the different models of adult learning and their significance for
research and development in adult language, literacy and numeracy. Most
research in learning has focused primarily on children, and most views of learning
have been developed within the context of children being educated in a formal
school system. However, when one moves away from models of child
development, and examines the models produced from disciplines looking at
settings beyond compulsory schooling, a very different view of learning emerges.
The aim of this review is to survey these fields and thereby provide useful ideas
for developing teaching and learning for adults.
Historically, the field that has addressed learning most directly is psychology.
Early theories developed in the USA and Europe saw learning principally as a
phenomenon of the individual. This review outlines the principal features of these
theories, focusing on behaviourism, cognitivism, cognitive constructivism and
developmental psychology. At the same time, within the fields of sociocultural
psychology, activity theory and situated cognition, work in the Soviet Union
developed understandings of learning as a form of social participation, and this
has been followed by more recent research in Europe and the US that takes the
same approach. We summarise the main features of this body of work.
One key idea within the field of adult learning theory is the model of the adult as a
self-directed or autonomous learner. Any model of adult learning that claims to be
complete has to take into account the self-directedness of much learning, and the
fact that the majority of learning in people’s lives takes place outside formal
learning provision. We present a summary of work on self-directed learning,
informal learning and learning how to learn. Another influential idea in the field has
been that adults learn primarily through reflection on their experience. We briefly
5
present the ideas of the primary theorists in the field of reflective and experiential
learning. Critical reflection is often central to those models of adult learning which
see learning as individually and/or socially transformative, and these
transformative models will be described. We also examine the claims made by
theorists taking a postmodern approach to learning.
We conclude by summarising the main findings of the review and suggest that a
full understanding of adult learning must be a complex one. Rather than seeing
learning principally as an individual, cognitive phenomenon, it must take into
account the interrelationship of the many factors in the learning situation, and
place the learner’s contexts, purposes and practices at the centre. We also list the
implications of the review for our understanding of adults’ learning.
The first significant psychological theories of learning were developed within the
field of behaviourism. Coming from a paradigm that limited scientific study only to
those things which could be observed directly, behaviourist psychologists avoided
using any internal ‘mentalist’ concepts, such as thought, to explain behaviour.
They restricted their explanations to those material parts of the situation that could
be seen and described. Their explanations for human behaviour were therefore
expressed purely in terms of conditioned responses to environmental stimuli.
The best-known theory of learning and instruction to emerge from this field is
Skinner’s ‘operant conditioning’ (see Skinner 1974 for a summary of his work).
This approach uses reinforcement to shape changes in behaviour gradually, by
breaking down a complex behaviour into a series of much smaller steps, and
immediately rewarding any change in the desired direction. The learner then
tends to repeat this behaviour, thus operating on their own environment in order to
elicit more positive reinforcement.
Variants of this approach have been used to address problem behaviours such as
smoking, weight gain, drug use and phobias, through ‘behaviour modification’
programmes. It has also formed the basis of a number of instructional models.
These tend to see the role of the teacher as primarily one of ‘delivering’ or
‘transmitting’ learning by breaking complex learning down into smaller, simpler
tasks. These tasks are practised repeatedly, and students are rewarded for
correct completion. Pre-determined learning outcomes, phrased purely in terms of
observable behaviours, are set at the start of a course or session. Behaviour that
approximates to these outcomes can then be measured and rewarded.
6
Behaviourist models of learning offer simplicity, control, a method for approaching
the teaching of complex behaviours in relatively straightforward ways, a way of
measuring whether this has been achieved, and an attractive appearance of
scientific rigour. However, in considering the potential for their application to adult
language, literacy and numeracy provision, they have significant weaknesses.
The behaviourist paradigm is concerned only with physical, observable
behaviours so that the mental processes of understanding and making sense of
things are beyond its remit. While it may be useful for the small sub-set of desired
learning outcomes that do not require the learner to experience changed
understandings in order to change behaviour patterns, it is not enough to draw on
in helping people who want to learn to read, write and do maths. These are
complex practices that bring together observable behaviour with non-observable
understandings and beliefs.
Cognitivism
Cognitivism rejects the model that sees learning only as changes in observable
behaviour, and instead understands learning as consisting of changes in mental
constructs and processes, the development and increasing sophistication of
‘mental maps’ and ‘schemata’ for representing the world. Since these processes
are not directly observable, the development of theory often proceeds by making
inferences about these internal cognitive processes, primarily by setting up
laboratory experiments designed to be interpretable in these terms.
7
Cognitivist theorists include Gagné (see, for example, Gagné 1985), who
developed a model in which learning is primarily about information processing.
Gagné’s theory of instruction is based on a taxonomy of learning outcomes and
he suggests that particular conditions are necessary to achieve these different
learning outcomes. This includes both those internal to the learner, such as the
skills and capacities the learner has already mastered, and those external to the
learner, such as the conditions arranged by the teacher or facilitator. He studied
the conditions under which successful learning occurs, and tried to describe these
objectively so that they could be replicated in other instructional settings.
The roots of Gagné’s thinking in behaviourism are clear, both in his terminology
and in the ‘chaining’ structure he describes. However his focus on internal
information processing, rather than externally observed behaviour, places him in
the realms of the cognitivists. His work has been particularly influential in the US
where his ideas have been taken up by the military and the educational
establishment.
Cognitive constructivism
8
‘objective’ knowledge. The next step in developing understanding was when
theorists became aware that learners themselves played an active role: not
merely in assimilating but also, and more importantly, in constructing the things
that they were learning. While cognitivist models of learning focus on learners
developing representational models of knowledge provided to them by their
environment, cognitive constructivism shifts the focus to the learner’s own process
of actively constructing these models through interaction with their environment
Developmental theories
Perhaps the best known and most influential of these stage theories of
development is Piaget’s cognitive theory of child development, mentioned above.
9
In this model, children pass through four stages of cognitive development, each
associated with different forms of cognitive structuring, reaching a final ‘formal-
operational’ stage in their teens which marks the first time they are able to deal
with abstract logic.
Piaget’s model has been very influential within education, but has also been
criticised. One of the reasons is that a ‘privileging’ of formal abstract thought is
implicit in the four-stage theory. While this is very common in Euro-American
cultures, it is becoming increasingly clear that abstract thought is only one of
many modes of thinking, and is not necessarily the most powerful or appropriate
in every situation (see sections on situated cognition and experiential learning for
more on this). Piaget’s model has therefore been seen as limited and culturally-
specific and, of course, it applies principally to children.
However, the basic idea that there are stages of cognitive development has also
been applied in several different ways within the context of adult education.
(Merriam and Caffarella (1998) contains a good review of such work.) Perry’s
(1970) study, based on work with college students, suggests that undergraduates’
thought processes typically develop along a continuum of nine positions, from
absolutism through relativism to contextualised reasoning. Similarly, King and
Kitchener (1994) identify seven stages in the process of developing reflective
judgement. Belenky et al.’s influential study, ‘Women’s Ways of Knowing’ (1986),
is again based on work in academic institutions but also used research with
women in parenting classes. It suggests that women’s thought develops in five
stages, from silence, through received knowledge, subjective knowledge and
procedural knowledge, to constructed knowledge.
Baxter Magolda’s work (1992) followed a group of male and female college
students over five years, interviewing them yearly. It identifies a progression in
developing epistemological reflection from absolute, through transitional and
independent, to contextual forms of reasoning. Kohlberg’s work (1981; 1984)
focused on moral development, claiming that there are six phases of moral
development through which people must pass, from pre-conventional ideas of
obedience and punishment, through conventional morality concerned with gaining
the approval of others and following laws, to post-conventional morality which, in
its most developed form, is guided by principles and conscience.
10
Some theories of development identify a small number of discrete cognitive
stages that people supposedly pass through in a relatively linear fashion.
However, many of these models were developed through researching small
samples of particular sorts of learners, often college students, so we cannot
assume that they can be directly applied to all adult learners. And there is
little evidence to suggest that adults’ needs with regard to language,
literacy, numeracy are necessarily related to any more generalised stage of
cognitive development. Whereas cognitive developmental models imply a
fairly reductionist, linear process of progress, we know that adult learners
typically exhibit ‘spiky profiles’, that is to say, varying levels of ability and
confidence, even within different elements of literacy and numeracy
practices. This ‘spikiness’ is likely to extend to their lives as a whole.
Again, these models of development suggest a single linear path through which
everyone is expected to progress. This absolutist thinking has been questioned by
theorists who suggest that similarities in people’s development are more likely to
relate to the contexts, cultures and communities within which they grow, than to
development understood as a phenomenon which acts in the same way
irrespective of context.
Neugarten (1976) points out that every society has expectations about age-
appropriate behaviour, but these differ from one society to another and are
socially constructed rather than absolute. She argues that when life changes
occur in a different order from that which society expects, they are experienced as
problematic. Riegel (1976) develops a more complex understanding of human
development as consisting of at least four dimensions: inner-biological, individual-
psychological, cultural-psychological, and outer physical. He argues that periods
of equilibrium between these dimensions are the exception rather than the rule.
He suggests that when any two (or more) of these dimensions are in conflict,
there is potential for change, and that development is therefore normally an
ongoing, continual process.
Tennant and Pogson (1995) explore the significance of this development literature
for adult education practice. They argue that more recent work moves beyond
stage theories and comes to understand that adults develop through having the
experience of dealing with real-life problems, rather than as an inevitable
progression through a pre-ordained series of stages. The problems adults
encounter in the real world are often not the kind which can be addressed by
formal-operational logic. They tend to be open-ended, contradictory and
11
ambiguous. They seldom have a single logical or correct solution, but
nevertheless require a commitment to a single course of action such as buying
one house or choosing one job from among several possibilities. This sort of
problem is not ‘solved’, but is ‘resolved’ in an ongoing process. Tennant and
Pogson claim that, as a result of experiencing this type of problem, adults develop
a form of reasoning that they call ‘dialectical thinking’, in which working through
the formal logical properties of a task becomes less important than the ability to
draw on one’s accumulation of experience in dealing with similar problems.
Tennant and Pogson suggest that notions of fixed phases and stages and ideal
end-points should be abandoned, with the focus shifting to processes of change
and transformation and the learning involved in these, acknowledging the multiple
and non-linear pathways through which adults tend to pass. (They cite Merriam
and Clark’s (1991) ‘Lifelines’ study of work, love and learning as one which takes
this perspective.) This changes the focus from normative life stages to the social
and cultural processes that trigger changes and developments. It involves (but is
not limited to) understanding the socially-prescribed life course patterns in
different historical, social and cultural settings, as well as paying attention to
psychological development. By acknowledging the social and historical dimension
of development, adult educators can try to distinguish changes and learning
experiences that genuinely transform and liberate from those that merely key into
socially-approved life course expectations.
The Soviet school of sociocultural theory or activity theory (Vygotsky 1962; 1978;
Wertsch 1985a; 1985b; 1991) offers a different focus, bringing together theories of
development and a constructivist approach. This was an attempt to develop a
different form of psychology from the behaviourist understandings that were
predominant at the time. Rather than studying behaviour in a decontextualised
way, this new form of psychology examined how the human mind develops in the
context of ongoing, meaningful, goal-oriented action and interaction with other
people, mediated by semiotic and material tools.
12
interaction. Without this social interaction, higher mental functioning would not
emerge. His main aim was to specify the processes involved in this.
While acknowledging that the internalisation of the process changes its nature,
Vygotsky claimed that the specific structures and processes of intramental
functioning can always be traced to their intermental precursors. Therefore, social
relations underlie the development and learning of all higher cognitive functioning.
This leads to an understanding of higher mental functions, such as thinking,
voluntary attention and logical memory, as being potentially social as well as
individual activities. An example would be when two people ‘remember something
together’, prompting or ‘scaffolding’ one another until recall is achieved.
Vygotsky’s work also draws attention to the role of mediating tools or artefacts,
both material and non-material, in this interactional process. People’s interactions
draw on concepts, strategies, and technologies, including writing and other
representational technologies, which mediate the meanings constructed. Vygotsky
and colleagues showed the significance of such mediational artefacts through a
series of experiments with children and with adults with impairments such as
Parkinson’s disease. These demonstrated that very simple mediational tools, such
as coloured cards or paper templates, enabled people to perform tasks they could
not otherwise do (Vygotsky 1978). Vygotsky’s work supports not merely a social,
but a sociocultural theory of learning, which sees cognition as distributed both
between the people present in the interaction, and across such mediating ‘tools
for thinking’ as are present in the culture more generally.
This is clearly a very different understanding from that developed by the cognitivist
model described above. Rather than focusing on the role of the individual actor in
constructing meaning, Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory has interaction with other
people at its very core.
13
Activity theory was further developed by Vygotsky’s associates Leont’ev (1978;
1981) and Luria (1976; 1978). Translations of these writers into English in the
1970s led to their ideas being taken up by researchers in the West who developed
the potential offered by understandings of human action and learning in terms of
activity systems: that is, as communities of people engaging in a common activity.
Studies of learning in activity systems draw our attention to the role of interaction
and mediational artefacts (both material and semiotic) in goal-oriented activity,
developing a very different sort of understanding from the much more
individualistic cognitivist or behavioural models.
Activity theory has been developed more recently in psychology through the
works of Cole, Engeström, Scribner and colleagues. A good summary of the work
in this field can be found in Cole, Engeström and Vasquez’ ‘Mind, Culture and
Activity’ (1997), which draws together seminal papers from the ‘Quarterly
Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition’. This collection
shows the historical development of an approach that studies the actions of
people participating in routine, culturally-organised activities. It generates a
psychology that places context, rather than individual cognition, at the centre of
our understanding of human thought and activity.
Situated cognition
When psychologists began to look at how cognition occurs in real situations rather
than in decontextualised laboratory settings, a situated model of learning and
cognition began to emerge (Lave 1988; Rogoff and Lave 1984; Scribner and Cole
1981). Many of the cognitive processes that had been seen as universal and
transferable within cognitive psychology were now understood to be closely tied to
the particular experimental situation in which they were exercised. It became
clear that decontextualised studies of learning could give very misleading results.
A particularly good example is the Adult Math Project in the US, reported in Lave
(1988). This research combined traditional testing of mathematical skills with
ethnographic observations of the mathematical practices in which people engaged
in their everyday lives: for instance, while doing their grocery shopping in the
supermarket. It was found that adults performed very differently in experimental
settings and in normal everyday activities. Murtaugh (1985) states that, on
average, US grocery shoppers scored 59% correct in calculations in
decontextualised tests, and 98% correct when shopping in a supermarket. Similar
phenomena have been found with Brazilian street children whose ability to
perform complex calculations while selling in the streets is not reproduced in
‘school maths’-type tests and settings (Nunes et al. 1993).
This distinction between the results people achieve in tests and the way they deal
with real-life problems is brought out by Tennant and Pogson (1995). Drawing on
the work of Robert Sternberg (see, for example, Sternberg 1985), they outline
common differences between typical test problems and real-life problems. While
written test problems are already defined, a key skill in adult life is the ability to
define a problem and its parameters in the first place. Test problems usually have
a single correct answer, whereas everyday life problems often require people to
find a resolution from many different possibilities, none of which is necessarily
completely right or wrong. While a test would normally provide all the information
14
necessary to get the right answer, problems in everyday life often have to be
resolved without enough information, or on the basis of conflicting pieces of
information. Even when decisions have been made, feedback in everyday life is
rarely unambiguous. Finally, where test problems are normally required to be
solved alone, most of the problems encountered in everyday life are addressed in
conjunction with other people. Therefore, the thinking and problem-solving
processes in which people engage in everyday situated activity are often very
different from the decontextualised cognitive skills addressed by intelligence tests
and the like.
This is not to say that transfer between the learning setting and the everyday life
setting (where these are separate) is not possible, but it highlights the fact that
this is not an unproblematic process. Evans’ (2000) research with adults on the
relationship between mathematical thinking and emotion develops a model of
transfer which acknowledges its complexities. He draws attention to the need for
transfer to be seen not as directly carrying over the same ‘skill’ from one context
to another, but as ‘…a series of reconstructions of ideas and methods from the
context of learning, so that they are appropriate for the target setting’ (p.232). In
addition he claims it is necessary to take account of the often unpredictable
influences of affect and emotion, and of the ways in which meanings flow along
semiotic chains. He suggests that the ability of a signifier to take on different
meanings within different discursive practices provides both the basis of, and
limitations on, translation of learning between different practices.
Tennant and Pogson (1995) relate the historically powerful distinction between
abstract thought and situated practice to the distinctions made between the
theoretical and practical in Western culture, beginning with Greek thinkers such as
Plato. ‘Practical’ intelligence emphasises practice rather than theory, direct
usefulness rather than intellectual curiosity, and procedural usefulness rather than
declarative knowledge. Everyday action and thought tends to have immediate,
visible consequences and a real-life end in mind, which is not the case for
‘academic’ intelligence.
15
rather than simply problem ‘solving’. Within this model, adult development and
adult intelligence must be reconceptualised as comprising practical knowledge
and expertise, not cognitive processes alone, and learning is something that
happens through sustained engagement in this practice.
16
Other detailed studies of practice in a variety of settings reinforce this perspective
on learning in practice. Singleton (1998) is an edited collection of studies from
Japan which shows similar processes at work in the way people are trained in
traditional arts such as Noh theatre, calligraphy and martial arts, and in everyday
socialisation in a variety of Japanese settings. The book ‘Understanding Practice’
(Chaiklin and Lave 1996) includes studies of a wide variety of activities, from
university examinations to maritime navigation. These show how, in each of these
settings, learning as situated social practice is ongoing in the community’s
activities, developing an inherently contextualised understanding of learning to
challenge the prevailing decontextualised cognitive models.
The accumulated evidence of this field therefore convinced some to shift from “a
view according to which cognitive processes (and thus learning) are primary and a
view according to which social practice is the primary, generative phenomenon,
and learning is one of its characteristics” (Lave and Wenger 1991). This means
that whenever people engage in social practice, learning will inevitably take place.
This is an understanding of learning that moves beyond looking only at changes in
people’s thought processes, to seeing learning as becoming able to participate in
particular sorts of social practices. Learning is understood to be embedded in
other forms of social participation, and therefore provision that helps people to
engage in social participation is likely to be of more use than provision that aims
to equip people with decontextualised skills.
Brain science
Even within the field of brain science, which might seem to be the place where
one might most expect to find individual internalised models of human thinking,
recent research has demonstrated the socially-situated nature of brain
development and the dialectical interpenetration of individual thinking and learning
with social context. Early theories in brain science tried to map different areas of
the brain to different thought processes, developing concepts such as the idea
that the ‘right brain’ was the location of creativity and the ‘left brain’ the location of
rational processing.
17
particular tasks to seeing it as a network of paths working and combining in
parallel. These pathways are continually developed and recombined as people
interact with the world around them. The brain is physically formed by this ongoing
process of interaction between ‘intelligence’ and what they call ‘extelligence’, that
is, the cultural capital available in a given context (Stewart and Cohen 1997).
Cohen and Leicester suggest that the brain is best understood not in terms of
areas reserved for particular sorts of processing, but rather as ‘permeable,
branching and flexible moving pathways criss-crossing and recursively interacting
with each other and with incoming information from the external world (imagine
Spaghetti Junction “reeling and writhing” and repeatedly re-assembling).’
Gee suggests that our capacity for learning can be explained in terms of the brain
engaging in this sort of ongoing interaction with the world, only in a much more
complex way involving recursive interrelationships between many millions of
neurons. Again, this is an intrinsically social model of learning, in that the
interactions between the neural networks and the world beyond always take place
within a socially-constructed world, with the resources drawn on being socially and
historically constituted.
18
psychologies on which many of these models draw. We then consider certain
elements in models of adult learning that have become particularly influential,
including ideas about self-directed learning, learning how to learn, informal
learning, reflective and experiential learning and transformative learning. While
most of these models come from studies of adult education, we also mention work
from research in higher education where this is relevant.
As we have shown above, until the early 1970s most theories of learning came
from psychology. Many of these psychological theories of learning are general in
nature, often assuming that similar learning processes occur for everybody,
whether children or adults. In contrast, theories emerging from the field of adult
education have tended to focus more on what is distinctive about adult learning in
particular. Coben and Llorente (2003) draw attention to the strengths and
weaknesses of this approach. While it has been good to encourage the
development of research and theory in the field, it has also closed off some
possibilities of dialogue with other fields of social sciences, pedagogy in particular.
This has encouraged attention to the differences between working with adults and
working with children, rather than to the core issue of the purposes and practices
of ‘education for all’.
In the early 1970s several influential books were produced which argued that
there is something particular about the way adults learn, and that research and
practice in adult education should reflect these particularities. These included
Houle’s ‘The Design of Education’ (1972), Kidd’s ‘How Adults Learn’ (1978), and
perhaps most influentially Knowles’ ‘The Adult Learner: A Neglected Species’
(Knowles 1973; Knowles et al. 1998).
19
Early editions of Knowles’ work make a clear distinction between pedagogy as
suitable for children and andragogy as suitable for adults. However, he later came
to recognise that different models of teaching and learning are appropriate for
different situations. A pedagogical strategy rather than an andragogical one may
be most appropriate in some contexts, such as when learners are completely new
to a particular subject area (Knowles 1980). However, he claims that the
andragogue will only use pedagogical strategies as a first step, and will do
everything possible to make the learner themselves responsible for their own
learning. He abstracts principles of teaching from these theories of learning.
The implications of the theory of andragogy are that we need to know why
people are engaged in learning and make the reasons why we are teaching
particular things, in particular ways, explicit. We should support the
development of self-direction in learners, be ready to draw on their own
experiences where possible, and be aware of the tasks they are engaged in
outside the learning environment and the social roles and stages these are
associated with. We should relate learning to genuine problems and issues
in people’s lives, rather than just focusing on decontextualised topics and
skills, and we should understand and respect people’s own motivations for
learning.
Humanistic psychology
Underlying Knowles’ model (and many of the other models of adult learning that
focus on self-transformation and self-development) are humanistic theories of
personal development, such as those of Rogers (Rogers and Freiberg 1994) and
Maslow (1970). Humanistic psychology takes human potential and desire for
growth as a basic assumption, and sees people as having unlimited potential to
improve themselves and seek fulfilment. Maslow and Rogers both assume that
people have an intrinsic drive towards growth and self-direction.
Rogers developed his ideas on the basis of years of dealing with clients in
therapy. His theory is built around the idea that there is a single force of life known
as the ‘actualising tendency’: a built-in motivation, present in every life form, to
make the very best of their existence. He developed a client-centred approach to
therapy based on avoiding directiveness and helping the client to reflect on their
experiences.
20
Models of adult learning from humanistic psychology imply that people
have an intrinsic drive for self-development and that this can and should be
fostered in education. Another significant implication of these models,
particularly in the context of adult language, literacy and numeracy
provision, is that people’s more basic needs have to be fulfilled before
learning can take place. Someone who is hungry, thirsty, sleepy or ill,
feeling excluded, or feeling unsafe is unlikely to learn effectively, because
these needs will take priority. It therefore becomes an essential part of the
learning process to address issues of this nature. This is especially
important given that many learners in the field of adult basic skills live in
conditions of social difficulty, many have health problems, and many have
had negative experiences of school and other forms of prior learning which
can cause them to feel unsafe in any formal learning environment.
Critiques of andragogy
Although the theory of andragogy has been widely taken up, it has also been
criticised (Brookfield 1994; Edwards et al. 1995), often on the basis that it is less a
theory of adult learning than an ideal state for adult learners to be in – a
prescriptive, rather than a descriptive model. People have also questioned
whether this model is really a description of the specific characteristics of adult
learning, or is merely specific to the types of situations that adult learners tend to
be in. This would suggest that it may also apply to some forms of children’s
learning. The model as it stands largely ignores the significance of the context in
which the adult learning takes place. Merriam and Caffarella (1998) cite a variety
of empirical studies that have attempted to test the theory, with very mixed results.
Hanson (1996) asks whether there is any need for a separate theory of adult
learning at all. She argues that there is little real evidence for an absolute
difference between adults and children in terms of their learning. The differences
that many theorists believe accumulate with increased age and increased
experience may not be as significant as the theory of andragogy assumes. Rather
than having a generalised theory of adult learning, based on unsubstantiated,
culturally-specific, abstract individualist assumptions about what all adults have in
common, she calls for analysis of individuals’ own particular characteristics, of the
settings and social contexts within which they learn and of their relationships with
peers and tutors. She claims that these all contribute to the learning process in
specific ways and that:
Hanson, A. (1996) 'The search for a separate theory of adult learning: does
anybody really need andragogy?', p. 107
The principal difference in context, culture and power between child learning and
adult learning remains that adult learning is usually voluntary, although this is
changing in certain settings, for instance where provision of welfare benefits has
21
been linked to compulsory basic skills education. This means that adults normally
engage in formal provision in response to needs and wants that they experience
in their own lives, rather than because they are following an external directive.
Adults are also likely to have other things going on in their lives that can affect
their experience of learning. We must not overlook practical issues such as time,
money and childcare, all of which need to be addressed if people are to engage
with provision.
Brookfield argues (like Mezirow, see below) that the ability to be critically reflective
is only developed as adults pass through experiences of breadth, depth, diversity
and differential intensity, over a long period of time. This makes possible critical
reflection on the assumptions, beliefs and values assimilated during childhood
and adolescence, and assessment of the accuracy and validity of those norms for
the contexts of adult life.
22
combination of factors in these specific settings and purposes. Therefore
there may not be a singular ‘right’ model of adult learning that can be
applied in teaching. The important thing is to understand the particularities
of the contexts and practices in which adult learners are engaged.
Certain elements of the andragogy model have been taken up and developed in
depth, and have become influential concepts within adult education research. We
will now move on to describe and assess the contributions of writers who have
focused on the elements of self-directed learning, learning how to learn, informal
learning, reflective and experiential learning, and transformative learning.
Self-directed learning
Since then, the concept has proliferated and is often encountered in adult
education literature, used in many different ways. Brockett and Hiemstra (1991)
offer one attempt to make sense of the field. They draw attention to the confusion
that has surrounded the term, and suggest that it is necessary to distinguish
between self-direction as an instructional method, and self-direction as a
dimension of personality. Surveying the existing empirical research in the field,
they conclude that self-directed learning is not an unusual phenomenon, but a
way of life that cuts across socio-economic strata, including supposedly ‘hard-to-
reach’ groups who do not engage readily with formal learning provision.
There are complex relationships between these concepts, and often some
conceptual slippage between them. For instance, learner-control in an
instructional domain is different from autodidaxy outside formal education.
Autonomy in learning does not necessarily give rise to general personal
23
autonomy, nor vice versa. The concepts are also complex in their own right.
Autonomy has both a personal and a situational dimension – people who are
autonomous in some settings can become very dependent in others, or while
dealing with some areas of work rather than others. Most research on autonomy
in learning has tended to focus on self-management skills or qualities of personal
autonomy, at the expense of the dimension of the learner’s construction of the
situation.
Candy calls for alternative methods of research that see learning as a search for
meaning and coherence in one’s life, rather than as the acquisition of quantities of
information. Thus the emphasis is on the personal significance of the learning to
the learner, rather than on ‘how much’ is learned. He also argues that it is
necessary to place the discussion about self-direction into a broader and less
individualistic framework. He suggests that interpretive approaches often
exaggerate the extent to which the individual’s intentions influence the actions
they engage in, and underestimate the power of social and cultural factors, such
as structures of class, gender, ethnicity and age, to limit people’s freedom and
choices.
Candy claims that there are important constraints on the extent to which people
can or should strive to be self-directed, especially when learning formal or
technical bodies of knowledge, as opposed to acquiring greater self-knowledge.
He points out that you cannot simply ‘graft on’ self-directedness in instruction to
an education system that is antipathetic to that goal. Approaches to the
development of self-directed learning must be congruent with the underlying
assumptions of the model which means, among other things, honouring the right
of learners to be self-directed with regard to their own self-directedness. It is
therefore not necessarily adequate simply to apply learning methods that
emphasise learner-control.
24
possibilities for self-directedness that adult learners experience within
current structures of provision, and also of any constraints there may be
upon it.
One outcome of the focus on self-directed learning has been the development of
the idea that adults can and should become aware of their own learning
processes and how to manage them. ‘Learning how to learn’ is a concept that has
become increasingly popular, particularly with the advent of policies encouraging
lifelong learning. The trailblazer in this field was Smith (1983; 1990). His basic
point was that adults benefit from actively learning about the cognitive processes
thought to be involved in learning, so that they can put them into practice. His
book, ‘Learning How to Learn: Applied Learning Theory for Adults’ (Smith 1983),
combined explicit description of a theory of learning with practical guidelines for
putting learning how to learn into application in a variety of different settings.
These included self-education, group learning projects, learning within educational
institutions, learning systematically through reflection on everyday experience,
and even learning through intuition and dreams.
Smith suggests that there are four distinctive characteristics of adult learners.
Firstly, they have a different orientation to learning from children, since they
choose for themselves to engage in education and therefore value time spent in
learning. Secondly, they have an accumulation of experience that forms the basis
for new learning, and this sum of experience becomes increasingly idiosyncratic
as people get older because of the unique nature of each person’s path through
life. Thirdly, different developmental tasks await adults at different points in their
lives, and education is sought during periods of transition. Finally, adult learning is
often characterised by anxiety and ambivalence related to negative experiences
of early schooling, the contradictory status of being both an autonomous adult and
a dependent student, and other similar emotional challenges. Central to the theory
of learning underlying his work is the intensely personal nature of the learning
process, and the demonstrated capacity of adults to assume partial or total
responsibility for educating themselves.
Elements from many of the models described above can therefore be found
underlying Smith’s work. His distinctive contribution is to suggest that it is in
developing an explicit understanding of these models (including, for example,
becoming aware of one’s preferred learning style) that adults can learn how to
learn and become autonomous learners. Since the publication of the first book he
has gone on to lead empirical research in the field of learning how to learn (see,
for example, Smith 1987; 1990).
Work on learning how to learn implies that adult learning provision should
not merely focus on delivering learning or skills, but also needs to include
space for discussion and reflection on learners’ experiences of their
learning, how they learn best, and what they can do to gain more control
over and improve their own learning.
25
Informal learning
There is some overlap between the literature on self-directed learning and that on
‘informal learning’, a term that has come into favour in recent years. As with self-
directed learning, the term has been used in a variety of ways: to describe the
way adults learn outside formal provision; to refer to unplanned or unpremeditated
learning, or learning which has not been formally structured; to refer to provision in
the community as opposed to that which is provided by formal educational
institutions; or to refer to any non-accredited provision. This makes a synthesis of
the field of informal learning as a whole very difficult. However, it is clear that
much of the research examining the way people learn over the course of their
lives has brought to light the crucial importance of some form of informal learning
in adults’ lives, and that this has often been overlooked in favour of work that
situates learning primarily within formal provision.
Coffield’s (2000) report on the ESRC’s ‘The Learning Society: Knowledge and
Skills for Employment’ programme is powerfully entitled, ‘The Necessity of
Informal Learning’. He underlines the importance of informal learning in the
formation of knowledge and skills, describing formal learning in institutions as
being merely ‘the tip of the iceberg’. The research programme discovered that
informal learning is often necessary to do the job, while formal learning is often
dispensable. Coffield calls for a profound change in the thinking of government,
employers, practitioners and researchers to reflect this insight.
This assessment of the significance of informal learning in adults’ lives has been
reinforced by a nationwide Canadian telephone survey (Livingstone 2000) which
found that over 95% of adults were involved in some form of informal learning
activity which they thought of as being significant.
26
Reflective and experiential models
Adult learning has also been said to be distinctive in that experience, and
reflection on this experience, has a central role to play in the process. Each adult
life is an accumulation of a unique set of experiences and contexts. Many theories
of adult learning developed within adult education look at the significance of this
experience for learning.
(1) suggestions, in which the mind leaps forward to a possible solution; (2) an
intellectualization of the difficulty or perplexity that has been felt (directly
experienced) into a problem to be solved, a question for which the answer
must be sought; (3) the use of one suggestion after another as a leading idea,
or hypothesis, to initiate and guide observation and other operations in
collection of factual material; (4) the mental elaboration of the idea or
supposition as an idea or supposition (reasoning, in the sense in which
reasoning is a part, not the whole, of inference); and (5) testing the hypothesis
by overt or imaginative action.
Despite the apparent neatness of this list, it is crucial to understand that these
stages are not presented as a recipe or algorithm to be followed slavishly. He
describes the process of reflective thinking as dynamic, ‘messy’, and full of false
starts and wrong turnings. He makes the important point that the eventual logical
form of the solution is the end of a process, rather than a starting-point. The
logical product can neither be predicted nor attained without engaging in the
messiness of the process. Therefore the fostering of reflective thought does not
come in teaching logical form or structure, but in encouraging the pondering of
real issues and problems. It is through this process of thinking, making
connections, and having the clear gradually emerge from the unclear that
concepts and ideas are formed and that learning happens.
In more recent years, Kolb’s has been the name most closely associated with
reflective and experiential learning. His ‘Experiential Learning: Experience as the
Source of Learning and Development’ (1984) is framed as a ‘systematic
statement’ of the theory of adult learning and its applications to education, work
and adult development. Drawing centrally on the work of Lewin, Dewey and
Piaget, and reviewing other contributions to the field, Kolb proposes a model of
27
the underlying structure of experiential learning as a continual process of
experience of and adaptation to the world, rather than as a series of outcomes.
Kolb’s well-known theory of learning styles comes out of this idea of a four-stage
cycle. He suggests that four different learning styles are associated with the four
different parts of this cycle. Ideally the learner would draw on different styles at
different moments in the cycle, but the uniqueness of each individual’s experience
means that different people have often acquired different distributions of the four
learning styles.
Many see reflection as the crucial element in Kolb’s cycle. Brookfield (1994) writes
that critical reflection is the ‘idea of the decade’ for educators looking to prove the
uniqueness of adult learning. They take evidence from developmental psychology
to claim that it is only adults who are capable of this type of thinking, involving
questioning, reframing or replacing existing assumptions, taking alternative
perspectives on ideas that were previously taken for granted, and potentially
coming to recognise the hegemonic aspects of dominant cultural values. Boud,
Keogh and Walker (1985) also see reflection as the part of Kolb’s cycle that is
most important in turning experience into learning. This volume brings together
articles that address the role of reflection in learning in different ways, within a
framework that stresses the need to take time out for active reflection so that
learning can come out of experience.
Kolb’s work, and particularly the idea of different learning styles, also underlies a
great deal of contemporary ‘learning to learn’ practice. Smith’s (1983) work,
described above, claims that:
A growing body of research now emerges that leaves little doubt that there is
a sound basis for taking seriously what has come to be called learning style –
and that style represents a viable component of the whole learning how to
learn concept.
However, much of the work around the concept of learning styles over-simplifies
Kolb’s analysis. A vast number of different resources and instruments now exist to
help people diagnose their own or their students’ learning style and then to
provide teaching to match this, in ways that sometimes make it appear that a
learning style is an essential or fixed trait. Kolb’s idea is rather that the
development of each learning style comes in a process of dialectical adaptation to
28
experience. The ’ideal learner’, far from favouring one particular style, would
develop a balance between all four stages in the cycle, and therefore would
master all four learning styles as appropriate.
Some have argued that Kolb’s own neat four-part distinction is itself based on an
over-simplified model of experience. Miettinen (2000) compares Kolb’s analysis of
experience to Dewey’s, showing how the subtleties of Dewey’s philosophical
approach (which insists, as explained above, on the impossibility of a simple
algorithmic approach to reflection) are lost in this four-part model. Therefore, the
idea of identifying one’s own or one’s students’ learning style and then teaching to
this should not be taken up uncritically.
The work of Dewey and Kolb highlights the central importance to reflective
and experiential learning of finding solutions to real-world problems. In both
of these models, this process involves a variety of different types of
thinking and modes of adaptation to the world. While the idea of identifying
different students’ preferred learning styles has become influential in adult
education, for Kolb and Dewey it is the integration of multiple learning
styles or ways of thinking in an ongoing process that is most important.
Others who have developed ideas around reflective and experiential learning
include Jarvis (1987), whose model of adult learning in social context is based on
the idea that learning becomes possible whenever there is a disjuncture between
biography and experience. On the basis of interviews and discussions held at a
series of workshops with adult educators, he develops a model of learning
according to which there are nine potential responses to this disjuncture: three
non-learning responses, three non-reflective responses, and three reflective
learning responses. This suggests that if there are many different types of
learning, no single set of principles for adult learning is likely to cover them all.
The real strength of Jarvis’ model is that it allows for the fact that it is the
interaction between individuals’ experience and biography that makes learning
possible. If the disjuncture between biography and experience is either too small
or too great, then the experience that occurs is more likely to result in
meaninglessness than in meaning construction. This interaction will vary from
person to person, depending on their particular biography and the particular
experiences that they undergo. Thus experience can serve both as a spur and as
a barrier to learning, and educators need to try to understand the complexities of
learners’ biographies, rather than seeing a simple correlation between experience
and learning.
Also, given that meaning comes from an interaction between the person and
society, a person must be seen as being intrinsically a person-in-society, whose
mind and self are themselves socially constructed through this ongoing learning
process. He therefore claims that self-development is not necessarily the highest
end-product of education. If our aim is for the enhancement of the person-in-
society, then education needs to seek to develop both the individual and the
social good.
Tennant and Pogson (1995) examine the various ways in which adult educators
have attempted to incorporate experience into learning. The justification for the
29
centrality of experience within adult education practices comes from a variety of
work: Kolb and Dewey, as described above; Freire’s critical literacy, addressed
below; the social and cognitive constructivists, such as Piaget and Bruner, who
stress the interactive nature of the relationship between learning and experience;
and Rogers’ and Maslow’s stress on the emotionally laden nature of the
relationship between experience and learning. They point out that in order for
learning to occur, the learner must in some way go beyond experience alone.
Instead, experience must be mediated, reconstructed or transformed in some
way. They therefore ask the crucial question, how and under what conditions can
people reconstruct their experience and thereby learn from it?
30
Transformative learning
Freire’s ‘pedagogy of the oppressed’ was developed through work with the poor
and socially excluded in Brazil. He believed the role of education was to liberate
people from systematic oppression, and developed a method that taught literacy
not as a set of decontextualised skills but as a means of political participation and
action.
Freirean methodologies start from people’s lived experience, eliciting and working
with words and concepts that are already familiar to people in their everyday lives.
Education is not seen as something which the teacher brings along and deposits
in the students – the ‘banking’ image of education – which Freire argues merely
perpetuates the structures of oppression that have led to social exclusion in the
first place. Instead, the teacher is expected to transcend the divide between
themselves and the students by committing ‘class suicide’ as an educator and
being reborn (through an ‘Easter experience’) as a joint educator/educatee with
the students (Taylor 1993).
31
This rebirth is what enables all the people participating in the experience to
engage in a real process of dialogue, beginning by discussing their initial words
and concepts and drawing out the connections and broader meanings within
which they are situated. It is through this dialogue that people develop both
literacy and an awareness of the broader structural relations of power and
inequality that have led to their social exclusion. Freire calls this awareness
‘conscientizaçâo’. In his work, this term implies not merely ‘conscientisation’ in the
sense of awareness, but also praxis – that is, acting to make a real difference in
the world. The goal of such ‘problem-posing’ education is to engage teachers and
students in directly addressing these problems in ongoing liberatory action.
Freire’s work has been criticised on a variety of fronts (see Taylor 1993). His
writing can be dense and unclear, and verges in many places on the mystical. It is
not certain how far Freirean literacy programmes in reality actually start from
words and concepts that are central to learners’ everyday lives, and to what
extent they impose their own agendas. Although his programmes, and those
inspired by his work, engaged many thousands in literacy education in Brazil and
throughout the world, it is difficult to find evidence of Freirean programmes that
were directly responsible for achieving significant social change.
Postmodern perspectives
Postmodern and poststructuralist thought have been among the main strands in
social theory generally in recent years, but few have taken up these ideas within
adult learning. Rather than attempting to develop a unified model of adult learning,
the postmodern approach pays attention to diversity and fragmentation, seeing
the attainment of a single narrative explaining any phenomenon as a way of
imposing power that aims to silence dissenting voices, rather than as a
representation of truth.
Usher, Bryant and Johnson’s ‘Adult Education and the Post-Modern Challenge’
(1997) disputes the notion of critical reflection on experience as getting closer to
’truth’ or to more integrated perspectives, and the notion that higher perspectives
can be achieved by perspective transformation, as Mezirow suggests. They
demonstrate that experience is always an open text, and that the meaning of
experience can never be closed down by critical reflection. Instead, meaning
continues to be explored, constructed and reconstructed differently, in an
endlessly creative way.
This is a helpful corrective to those models that seek to impose a single model of
reflection or learning on what is clearly a very complex process. However, these
understandings can be misleading, in that their focus on fragmentation and
32
multiplicity can exclude the patterns that do emerge from the complexity of adult
learning interactions, and the ways in which different factors both reproduce and
change the regular patterns and meanings of social life. While there may not be
any single grand narrative that explains all the social processes involved, there
are still real and tangible outcomes of adult learning in terms of encounters that
can be described more or less accurately, meanings that people construct for
themselves, and changes that people experience.
While it is undoubtedly true that these changes are unstable and shift according to
the context within which people find themselves, they are still experienced as real
and significant by the people involved, and still have material and social
consequences. Something that changes over time in dialectical process is not
necessarily completely ‘open’. The choice is not between learning as fixed or
learning as open and fragmented, but between learning as product and learning
as process.
Most of the models of adult learning developed from within adult education
move beyond examinations of learning as a decontextualised process to
address questions relating to the meanings of, and motivations for, learning
in people’s lives. This may be in terms of self-direction, reflection,
autonomy, problem-solving or transformation and recalls, from a different
perspective, the intrinsically socially-situated nature of learning that
emerged from the review of the psychological literature. The key point to
take from this is that learning for adults is always related to their real lives,
their real problems and their real issues, and that we therefore need to try to
understand and make links with these, in order for provision to be
meaningful, relevant and effective.
33
Writers in the field of management learning have engaged directly with the issue
of learning in a context of rapid change for some time. Burgoyne and Reynolds
(1997) claim that the discipline of management learning started in the mid-1970s
as a response to a perceived need for reform in management education, training
and development. While the early research in management learning aimed to
create a single rational framework for understanding the purposes, processes and
effects of management education, training and development, in more recent years
much more attention has been paid to the particular dynamics of the different
contexts in which these processes are played out.
One important recent idea has been that of the ‘learning organisation’ which,
according to Tight (1996), arises directly from a concern about how organisations
can survive in the midst of rapid change. Its precursors were in the work of Argyris
and Schön (1978) on encouraging organisational learning through action
research, coupled with the concerns about quality and value for money associated
with concepts such as total quality management and total quality learning. The
idea of the learning organisation applies many of the ideas from lifelong learning
to the company situation. It is suggested that by encouraging every employee
within an organisation to personally invest in seeking continual learning and
improvement of quality, the company will develop a competitive edge and be more
likely to succeed. Without encouraging this orientation, companies will struggle to
cope with the speed of change in a global marketplace.
Vaill (1996), once again from the field of management learning, argues that the
rapid social changes we continually experience mean that we live in a world of
‘permanent white water’ which requires constant learning. His central thesis is that
our imaginative and creative initiatives and responses to the systems in which we
live and interact are, in fact, continual learning. He draws on a complex systems
model of human social life, suggesting that human systems are sociotechnical
ones in which, at millions of operational points, human will and human judgements
are exercised. These systems are under stress at a multitude of points, a fact
which builds turbulence and instability into the system.
It is therefore not in the nature of human social systems to run smoothly, and any
attempt at rational design will inevitably meet with unpredictable, emergent
change, or unintended consequences. These permanent white water conditions
are full of surprises, tend to produce novel problems, and feature events that are
‘messy’ and ill-structured, with everything in the system potentially being
connected to everything else. He claims that this experience of always doing new
things and dealing with new problems requires people in contemporary
organisations to be (or to become) extremely effective learners.
Vaill critiques what he calls the dominant theory of learning, which he sees as
resulting from people’s formative experiences of learning in institutions. This
dominant theory implies that learning is painful; that learning goals are given to
us, and that learning is a means to an end that is not of our own choice; and that
the person setting out to learn is less admirable than the person who has
completed learning. He claims that these common ideas about learning impede
the genuine practice of lifelong learning that he sees as being necessary, as well
as resting on profound and far-reaching basic assumptions about the nature of
learning of which most educators are unaware.
34
Learning opportunities in management settings, at workshops or conferences, are
often set up to mimic the school system, with some cosmetic alterations.
However, the learning that emerges from these situations is not the sort of
learning that is appropriate for permanent white water conditions. He proposes
that we should instead be aiming to develop ‘learning as a way of being’, a form of
learning that is self-directed, creative, exploratory and inventive, expressive,
rewarding in the process (not just when a goal is reached), occurring at the levels
of feelings and meanings (as much as ideas and skills), and being
deinstitutionalised and genuinely continual. This learning process requires
reflexivity about the process of learning, as well as about the content of what is
learned.
Rapid technological change has specific implications for adult learning, explored
in Lea and Nicoll’s ‘Distributed Learning: Social and Cultural Approaches to
Practice’ (Lea and Nicoll 2002). This book addresses the use of both new and old
technologies as mediational means in distance learning settings, claiming that the
historical distinction between face-to-face and distance learning has disappeared,
and exploring the way this changes conceptions of learning. They stress that the
changes associated with new technologies are not necessarily predictable,
because these new technologies are not used in isolation, but are introduced into
specific learning settings with existing social practices associated with them.
Therefore, rather than suggesting that there can be any single set of changes in
learning associated with new technologies, they underline the need to examine
the social and cultural practices surrounding technologies in different learning
environments, and to understand the specific changes that technology and
distance learning bring to particular places and settings.
The work described here would suggest that the best way to do this is to
encourage exploratory rather than controlled forms of learning, where the
learner is empowered to follow their own interests and desires in the
learning process, in ways that relate meaningfully to the practices in which
they are engaged.
35
However, these approaches cannot address the non-material aspects of learning
and understanding that are at the heart of learning. A cognitivist perspective
shows that behaviour does not necessarily correlate with understanding, and that
we need to find ways to build on people’s existing knowledge to help them make
sense of new information. Cognitive constructivism brings to light the learner’s
active role in this process, and the need to support learners in making their own
meanings and connections.
Developmental theories add the understanding that people pass through different
stages in their lives, in which they take on different social and cultural roles and
responsibilities. Provision needs to take account of this and find ways to
understand and respond to the sorts of practices and problems people engage
with in their lives outside the classroom. At the same time, provision needs to
recognise the socially- and culturally-shaped nature of these developmental
models, and the implications of the pressures people experience to meet
expectations about appropriate choices and behaviour at particular points in their
lives. It is important to resist inappropriate models of adult development,
particularly those that assume there is a single developmental path and end-point
towards which we should all be aiming.
Activity theory and social constructivism draw attention to the central role played
by social interaction and by material and symbolic mediational tools in the learning
process. This suggests that interaction with the learner, at the level that is right for
them, and the use of appropriate mediational tools and strategies, are both
necessary for learning. Studies in situated cognition underline the importance of
the practices within which learning is located, offering a new understanding of
learning as a form of participation in social practice, rather seeing it only in terms
of behaviour or cognitive processes. This socially- and historically-constructed
model of learning is supported from another field by advances in neuroscience,
which give biophysical explanations for our capacity for learning and show how
neural networks develop in a process of interaction within a wider social and
cultural context.
Moving on to models from the field of adult education, the theory of andragogy
suggests that adult learning is distinctive in a variety of ways. Adults are supposed
to be self-directed and they need to know why they need to learn something. They
have accumulated more and different experiences than children and their learning
is linked to their social role and stage or life. They engage in problem-centred
rather than subject-centred learning and they are internally, rather than externally,
motivated. However, critics of andragogy suggest that taking note of the different
contexts and practices in which adults engage is more important than identifying
what is intrinsically different about adults.
Work on self-directed learning suggests that this is not merely a desirable property
of an individual learner, but arises from a complex set of factors in social
interaction. Ideas concerning learning to learn demonstrate that it is useful for
adult learners to have space and time to reflect on their learning processes, as
well as on the particular topics addressed in class. Research has brought to light
the significance of informal and incidental learning in people’s lives and implies
the need to be aware of adults’ learning outside formal provision and to see
unplanned learning within the classroom as a resource. The work on reflective
36
and experiential learning shows the significance of real-life problems, situations
and issues for people’s learning and the uniqueness and unpredictability of each
person’s process of reflection.
Finally, some models have confronted the issue of learning in contexts of rapid
social and technological change, particularly in management learning and in
online and distance education. These models reinforce the arguments for flexible
approaches to learning and provision that engage centrally with the learner’s own
contexts and practices.
It is clear that it would be partial and misleading to see adult learning only as an
individual cognitive phenomenon, or even as something that can be fully
controlled by a teacher transmitting particular curriculum content. Instead, learning
is present in a dialectical interaction between individual, situational and social
factors. The learner’s contexts, purposes and practices are the most important
factors in the process.
1. Adults have their own motivations for learning. Learners build on their existing
knowledge and experience. They fit learning into their own purposes and become
engaged in it. People’s purposes for learning are related to their real lives and the
practices and roles they engage in outside the classroom.
3. Adults have the ability to learn about their own learning processes, and can
benefit from discussion and reflection on this. They are able to learn how to learn.
For instance, there are different learning styles that people synthesise. Teaching
can enable learners to develop their range of learning styles.
37
5. Adults reflect and build upon their experience. Reflective learning is generated
when people encounter problems and issues in their real lives and think about
ways of resolving them.
6. Reflective learning is unique to each person, since it arises out of the complexities
of their own experience. A great deal of learning is incidental and idiosyncratically
related to the learner: it cannot be planned in advance. While there are things that
can be done to encourage reflective experiential learning, there is no set of steps
that can be followed to guarantee it will happen.
38
References
Argyris, C. and D. Schön (1978). Organizational learning: a theory of action
perspective. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Boud, D., R. Keogh and D. Walker, Eds. (1985). Reflection: Turning Experience
Into Learning. London: Kogan Page.
39
Calder, J. Ed. (1993). Disaffection and Diversity: Overcoming Barriers for
Adult Learners. London: Falmer.
Coben, D. (1998). Radical Heroes: Gramsci, Freire and the Politics of Adult
Education. New York and London: Garland/Taylor and Francis.
Coffield, F. Ed. (2000). The Necessity of Informal Learning. Bristol: The Policy
Press.
Cohen, J. and M. Leicester, (2000). ‘The evolution of the learning society: brain
science, social science and lifelong learning.’ Lifelong Learning: Education
Across the Lifespan. J. Field and M. Leicester, Eds. London: RoutledgeFalmer.
Cole, M., Y. Engeström, and O. Vasquez, Eds. (1997). Mind, Culture and
Activity: Seminal Papers from the Laboratory of Comparative Human
Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
40
Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and Society. 2nd (revd) edition. New York:
Norton.
Gee, J. P. (1992). The Social Mind: Language, Ideology, and Social Practice.
New York: Bergin & Garvey.
Gee, J. P., G. Hull, and C. Lankshear (1996). The New Work Order: Behind the
Language of the New Capitalism. St Leonards, NSW: Allen and Unwin.
Hanson, A. (1996). ‘The search for a separate theory of adult learning: does
anyone really need andragogy?’ Boundaries of Adult Learning. R. Edwards, A.
Hanson and P. Raggatt, Eds. London and New York: Routledge in association
with the Open University.
Hochschild, A. (1997). The Time Bind: When Work becomes Home and Home
becomes Work. New York: Metropolitan Books.
Jarvis, P. (1987). Adult Learning in the Social Context. London: Croom Helm.
41
(1980). The Modern Practice of Adult Education: Andragogy versus
Pedagogy. Revised and updated edition. Chicago: Follett Publishing Company.
Knowles, M., E. F. Holton and R. A. Swanson (1998). The Adult Learner: The
Definitive Classic in Adult Education and Human Resource Development.
5th edition. Houston, TX: Gulf Publishing Company.
Lea, M. R. and K. Nicoll, Eds. (2002). Distributed Learning: Social and Cultural
Approaches to Practice. London and New York: RoutledgeFalmer with the Open
University.
(1978). The Selected Writings of A. R. Luria. M Cole, Ed. White Plains, NY:
M. E. Sharpe.
Maslow, A. (1970). Motivation and Personality. 2nd edition. New York: Harper
and Row.
42
Merriam, S. B. and R. S. Caffarella (1998). Learning in Adulthood: A
Comprehensive Guide. 2nd edition. New York: Jossey-Bass.
Miller, N. E. and J. Dollard (1941) Social Learning and Imitation. New Haven:
Yale University Press.
(1970) The Science of Education and the Psychology of the Child. New
York: Grossman.
Rogers, C. and H. J. Freiberg, (1994). Freedom to Learn. 3rd edition. New York:
Prentice Hall.
43
Scribner, S. (1983/1997). ‘Mind in action: a functional approach to thinking.’ Mind,
Culture and Activity: Seminal Papers from the Laboratory of Human
Cognition. M. Cole, Y. Engeström and O. Vasquez, Eds. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Ed. (1990). Learning to Learn Across the Lifespan. San Francisco: Jossey-
Bass.
Tennant, M. and P. Pogson (1995). Learning and Change in the Adult Years: A
Developmental Perspective. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Tight, M. (1996). Key Concepts in Adult Education and Training. London and
New York: Routledge.
Usher, R. S., I. Bryant and R. Johnston (1997). Adult Education and the Post-
Modern Challenge, London and New York: Routledge.
44
Wenger, E. (1998). Communities of Practice: Learning, Meaning and Identity.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
45