Application of TRIZ in Improving The Creativity of Engineering Experts
Application of TRIZ in Improving The Creativity of Engineering Experts
net/publication/289130874
CITATION READS
1 149
2 authors, including:
Iouri Belski
RMIT University
73 PUBLICATIONS 348 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Iouri Belski on 12 January 2016.
ScienceDirect
Procedia Engineering 131 (2015) 792 – 797
Abstract
Numerous researchers have extensively studied two causes of problem solvers’ inflexibility which impede creativity: the
Einstellung effect [1] and design fixation [2]. The former has been demonstrated experimentally on numerous occasions and
is induced by prior experience. The latter is a result of our fixedness on the functions of things which we regularly use. This
paper focuses on the third cause of inflexibility, which has not been researched adequately – the detrimental effect of
professional expertise on creativity [3]. This detrimental effect is a natural consequence of extensive professional experience
and the possession of large amounts of domain knowledge. After approximately 10 years in a profession, due to the
construction of effective knowledge schemas, the short-term memory limitations which normally impede effective idea
generation can partly or even completely disappear. As a result, experts attain an ability to search for solutions to problems
quickly – without significant cognitive and time effort. Although this ‘quickness’ of experts in suggesting solutions is
advantageous, it also creates negative consequences. Experts’ solutions are usually confined to their domain-specific
knowledge and do not utilize novel ideas. This study reviews existing evidence related to the detrimental influence of expertise
on creativity and discusses how the application of TRIZ tools of Substance-Field Analysis and Method of the Ideal Result can
minimize this negative influence.
© 2015
© 2015Published
The Authors. Published
by Elsevier Ltd. by Elsevier
This Ltd.access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license
is an open
Peer-review under responsibility of the Scientific Committee of TFC 2011, TFC 2012, TFC 2013 and TFC 2014 – GIC.
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
Peer-review under responsibility of the Scientific Committee of TFC 2011, TFC 2012, TFC 2013 and TFC 2014 – GIC
Keywords: Engineering creativity; fixation; memory search; Substance-Field analysis; Method of the Ideal Result.
1. Introduction
It has been established that expertise requires not only a large amount of domain knowledge, but also many
years of professional practice [4, 5]. In order to become an expert in semantically rich domains, such as
engineering, science and medicine, extensive professional knowledge must be acquired prior to being permitted
to work as a professional. This acquisition of basic professional knowledge usually occurs at university. After
1877-7058 © 2015 Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
Peer-review under responsibility of the Scientific Committee of TFC 2011, TFC 2012, TFC 2013 and TFC 2014 – GIC
doi:10.1016/j.proeng.2015.12.379
Iouri Belski and Ianina Belski / Procedia Engineering 131 (2015) 792 – 797 793
graduating, specialists spend years learning to apply their discipline knowledge in real professional situations. It is
usually considered that 10 years of extensive professional practice is the very minimum to accumulate expertise
[6]. As a result of many years of professional practice, experts become superior in task recognition and most of
the time markedly outperform novices and individuals on intermediate level skills in memory recall in their
professional domains [5].
On the other hand, many studies demonstrated that under certain conditions novices and intermediates can
outperform experts in their domains of expertise. Chase and Simon [7], for example, showed that novices
performed better than experts in recall of randomized chess boards. It has been reported that, although expert
radiologists performed better than ones with limited experience in remembering X-ray films with clinical
abnormalities, they did worse in remembering films that did not contain abnormalities [8]. Similarly, experts in
electronics required longer time than novices to establish atypical faults in electronic circuits [9]. There is also
anecdotal evidence that experts are usually less capable than novices in suggesting fresh and novel ideas. This
paper focuses on the detrimental influence of expertise on creativity and endeavors to determine the reasons
behind this influence and to suggest the ways to minimize it.
Years of professional experience considerably change the way practitioners conceptualize domain
knowledge. When faced with a problem, novices usually focus on surface structures of the problematic
situation. Experts analyze problems on an abstract level and normally disregard surface structures of the
problematic situation while making judgments [10]. There are a number of theories that attempt to explain the
differences in performance of experts and novices [11]. All these theories essentially agree on the fact that over the
years of extensive professional practice, experts develop special memory structures (schemas) that integrate their
domain-specific knowledge. These schemas allow experts to rapidly (and often automatically) search their long-
term memory (LTM) for information and actions that are the most appropriate in every particular professional
situation [5, 12, 13]. In essence, these schemas substantially reduce or fully remove short-term memory (STM)
limitation in experts [3, 14], leading to superior expert performance in recognition and recall.
Wiley [10] reported that a large amount of domain knowledge can be a disadvantage to experts’
creativity. In a series of three experiments that engaged subjects with different levels of baseball knowledge that
used an adapted version of Mednick’s remote association test (RAT) [15], she discovered that expert knowledge
can act as a mental set:
“It appears that domain knowledge not only biases a first solution attempt but also fixates the high-
knowledge subject by defining and narrowing the search space, preventing a broad search, and decreasing the
chances of finding an appropriate solution” [10].
Although large baseball knowledge does not compare in depth and breadth with domain knowledge of experts in
science, engineering and medicine, Belski and Belski [3] advocated that domain-specific schemas can
detrimentally impact on the creativity of experts from semantically rich domains [3]. In order to explain why
creativity can be diminished by domain knowledge, Belski and Belski [3] proposed to model human knowledge
as presented in Figure 1.
The model in Figure 1 consists of three main areas that represent three different categories of human
knowledge. The larger ellipse, ‘1’, symbolises all the knowledge that has been acquired by humans so far. The
smaller ellipse ‘2’ depicts the knowledge gained by a specific person during her/his years of study and
experience, her/his knowledge base. The circle, assigned as ‘3’, stands for all the expert knowledge of this particular
individual. Although both areas ‘2’ and ‘3’ in Figure 1 designate knowledge possessed by the same individual,
the way knowledge from these areas is searched and deployed by this individual differs fundamentally.
Expert knowledge is characterised by well-developed knowledge schemas that reduce limitations of STM
for the expert knowledge area ‘3’ and make searches of knowledge enclosed in ‘3’ very efficient. Moreover,
when the individual faces a problem from her/his professional domain, the expert knowledge area ‘3’ that
794 Iouri Belski and Ianina Belski / Procedia Engineering 131 (2015) 792 – 797
contains knowledge schemas is automatically searched first [16]. When this schema-activated search is successful
and a suitable solution is found in ‘3’, it is unlikely that the knowledge base area ‘2’ will be further searched for
more solution ideas [16]. Essentially, the individual’s solution ideas become bounded by her/his domain
knowledge.
When the individual considers a problem from outside of her/his domain of expertise, because schemas for that
area of knowledge have not been established, an automatic schema-driven search is not activated by default.
Therefore, the individual must utilise some general search strategy to seek for solutions. If a general search
strategy is in use, STM limitations for the area of expert knowledge ‘3’ are not reduced and, thus, expert
knowledge ‘3’ does not have searching priority over the area of all individual knowledge ‘2’. Consequently, this
general search for solutions may fully explore the knowledge contained in area ‘2’. This means that when the
individual faces a problem that is outside of her/his domain of expertise, the entire individual’s knowledge is
searched for solutions. As a result, solution ideas are not constrained by her/his expertise but restricted only by the
individual knowledge base.
In sum, when the individual faces a problem from her/his expert domain, solutions are likely to be confined
to the area of expert knowledge ‘3’. Solution ideas from the rest of her/his individual knowledge area ‘2’ are
unlikely to be considered at all. As a result, for problems that relate to the domain of expertise, a
substantial part of the individual’s knowledge is not searched during idea generation. Accordingly, experts’ ideas
can be limited to the knowledge area of their expertise.
It can be inferred that a practitioner working in a particular field gradually develops a tendency to refer for
solutions to her/his collection of domain knowledge that is also continuously growing. Growing domain
knowledge is slowly clustered into schemas that, in turn (after many years in the profession), gain automatic
priority to search LTM for solutions.
Fig. 1. Map of human knowledge: (1 – all human knowledge; 2 – all knowledge acquired by an individual; 3 – expert knowledge possessed by this
individual) [3].
“mechanical engineers offer mechanical solutions for problems which await electrical answers and vice versa
– electrical engineers only think of electrical solutions to every problem they encounter” [3].
Iouri Belski and Ianina Belski / Procedia Engineering 131 (2015) 792 – 797 795
Most contemporary engineering problems are open-ended and require knowledge that is beyond one
discipline. Therefore, it is expected that an engineering expert must be capable of looking beyond her/his domain
knowledge when solving problems.
In order to ensure that expert’s solutions are not confined to her/his domain knowledge, it is necessary to stop
the activation of an automatic schema-driven search of expert domain knowledge and:
x to replace the schema-driven search with some effective search strategy that exploits all the knowledge acquired
by the expert in full, and/or
x to engage the expert in reframing a problem in order to help her/him to see the problem situation in a novel way.
These strategies can be executed by means of the TRIZ tools.
2.3. TRIZ tools for engineering experts: searching long-term memory more efficiently
The effectiveness of the systematized Substance-Field analysis (Su-Field), Method of the Ideal Result (MIR),
Situation Analysis, the 40 Innovative Principles as well as the Contradiction Table to minimize the inefficiencies
of short-term memory have been considered in [3]. Let us specifically focus on the ability of Su-Field analysis
and MIR to stop the activation of an automatic schema-driven search of expert domain knowledge. The former can
help to replace the schema-driven search with some effective search strategy. The latter is useful in engaging
experts in problem reframing.
Human thoughts are usually unstructured. STM has a low storage capacity and short duration of time for
which it stores information. These characteristics significantly complicate the efficient retrieval of information
from LTM storage during idea generation. When an expert solves a problem within his professional domain,
the limitations of STM can be practically eliminated due to the schemas that are developed over 10 or more
years of professional experience. These schemas trigger an automatic search that is confined to expert domain-
specific knowledge. This makes engineering experts superior problem solvers. This also can impede experts’
creativity.
It has been shown that the TRIZ tools of Su-Field analysis and MIR can help in minimizing the
detrimental effect of expertise on creativity. Su-Field analysis offers expert the means to effectively search
through their knowledge base. MIR can stop the activation of an automatic schema-driven search by engaging
experts in problem reframing.
References
[1] M . Bilalic´, P. McLeod, and F. Gobet, Inflexibility of experts—Reality or myth? Quantifying the Einstellung effect in chess masters,
Cognitive Psychology, vol. 56, pp. 73-102, 2008.
[2] D . G. Jansson and S. M. Smith, Design fixation, Design Studies, vol. 12, pp. 3-11, 1991.
[3] I. Belski and I. Belski, Cognitive foundations of TRIZ problem-solving tools, in Proceedings of the TRIZ-Future Conference 2008, The
Netherlands, 2008, pp. 95-102.
[4] K . A. Ericsson, R. T. Krampe, and C. Tesch-Romer, The Role of Deliberate Practice in the Acquisition of Expert Performance, Psychological
Review, vol. 100, pp. 363-406, 1993.
[5] K . Anders Ericsson and T. J. Towne, Expertise, Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, vol. 1, pp. 404-16, 2010.
[6] M. T. Chi, Two approaches to the study of experts’ characteristics, The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance, pp. 21-30,
2006.
Iouri Belski and Ianina Belski / Procedia Engineering 131 (2015) 792 – 797 797
[7] W . G. Chase and H. A. Simon, Perception in chess, Cognitive Psychology, vol. 4, pp. 55-81, 1973.
[8] M . Myles-Worsley, W. A. Johnston, and M. A. Simons, The influence of expertise on X-ray image processing, Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, vol. 14, p. 553, 1988.
[9] D . Besnard and M. Bastien-Toniazzo, Expert error in trouble-shooting: an exploratory study in electronics, International Journal of Human-
Computer Studies, vol. 50, pp. 391-405, 1999.
[10] J. Wiley, Expertise as mental set: The effects of domain knowledge in creative problem solving, Memory & cognition, vol. 26, pp. 716-730,
1998.
[11] F. Gobet, Expert memory: A comparison of four theories, Cognition, vol. 66, pp. 115-152, 1998.
[12] F. Gobet and H. A. Simon, The Roles of Recognition Processes and Look-Ahead Search in Time-Constrained Expert Problem Solving:
Evidence from Grandmaster Level Chess, Psychological Science, pp. 52-55, 1996.
[13] H. A. Simon, Information-processing theory of human problem solving, Handbook of learning and cognitive processes, vol. 5, pp. 271-295,
1978.
[14] P. A. Kirschner, J. Sweller, and R. E. Clark, Why Minimal Guidance During Instruction Does Not Work: An Analysis of the Failure of
Constructivist, Discovery, Problem-Based, Experiential, and Inquiry-Based Teaching, Educational Psychologist, vol. 41, pp. 75-86, 2006.
[15] S. Mednick, The associative basis of the creative process, Psychological Review, vol. 69, p. 220, 1962.
[16] M. L. Gick, Problem-Solving Strategies, Educational Psychologist vol. 21, pp. 99-120, 1986.
[17] A. Belski, I. Belski, T. C. Teng, and R. Kwok, Application of Substance-Field Analysis for Failure Analysis, in Proceedings of the 13th
ETRIA world TRIZ future conference 2013, Paris, France, 2013, pp. 483-490.
[18] I. Belski, Improve your Thinking: Substance-Field Analysis. TRIZ4U, Melbourne, 2007.
[19] I. Belski, Improvement of Thinking and Problem Solving Skills of Engineering Students as a result of a Formal Course on TRIZ Thinking
Tools, in Proceedings of the 13th International Conference on Thinking, Norrkoping, Sweden, 2007, pp.11-17.
[20] I. Belski, Reinventing Triz Thinking Tools: Substance – Field Analysis, in Proceedings of the 5th International Conference TRIZFutures 06,
Kortrijk, Belgium, 2006, pp.35-40.
[21] I. Belski, J. Baglin, and J. Harlim, Teaching TRIZ at University: a Longitudinal Study, International Journal of Engineering Education, vol.
29, pp. 346-354, 2013.
[22] I. Belski, Teaching Thinking and Problem Solving at University: A Course on TRIZ, Creativity And Innovation Management,
vol. 18, pp. 101-108, 2009.
[23] I. Belski, I Wish The Work To Be Completed By Itself, Without My Involvement: The Method Of The Ideal Result In Engineering Problem
Solving, in Proceedings of World Innovation and Strategy Conference, Sydney, Australia, 1998, pp. 199-206.
[24] I. Belski, Solving problems with Method of the Ideal Result (MIR), in Transactions from the 11th Symposium on Quality Function
Deployment, Novi, Michigan, USA, 1999, pp. 192-203.
[25] G. Altshuller, Creativity as an Exact Science, Gordon & Breach Science Publishing House, New York, 1984.
[26] I. Belski, T. C. Teng, A. Belski, and R. Kwok, TRIZ Wins a Tender: Designing a Crash Barrier, in TRIZ Future Conference 2012, Lisbon,
Portugal, 2012, pp. 425-436.
[27] C. M. Tay, T. C. Teng, E. L. Sioh, A. J. Shi, and C. N. Sim, Vehicle barrier, International Patent, WO 2006/088428, 2006.