Paper 3 Finally Done
Paper 3 Finally Done
Jaret James
University of Arizona
Overview:
The purpose of life can be defined a million different ways by a million different people,
but it’s a safe bet that most would answer that their purpose in life is to be happy. However,
happiness is also a very abstract state of one’s life, and it varies from person to person. There is
no universal formula for what it takes to be happy, and although there are many different aspects
in life that are thought to bring joy to life, the fact of the matter is that it differs for everyone. So,
we start to look at things that are potentially thought to be linked with emotional satisfaction, and
of course one of the first things that we tend to think about is this age old question “can money
buy happiness?” In the first twenty-some odd years of our lives, we meticulously complete
thousands of assignments and papers and projects, not because we want to but because we
believe the end goal is a “good” job that pays “good” money. From the day we are born we are
conditioned to work and work because we are eventually going to have a family who needs a
house and food and clothes, and none of that can be obtained without money. So, for those who
do not fall into the particularly wealthy category by the end of the journey, it may seem to be an
obvious answer: “of course money can buy happiness”. However, the reality is people don’t look
into the question in terms of what burdens higher amounts of wealth can bring. They see it as a
very one-sided question where higher monetary wealth only brings benefits, and their
assumption is that money and happiness are directly proportional, meaning the common belief
would be that more money leads to a happier life. So, are they right? Do higher salaries correlate
After initially sitting down and looking at the very popular debate, I believed that with
limitations, higher salaries would lead to an increase in happiness, but the two were not infinitely
linear. I believed that eventually there would be a point where the higher amounts of money
would no longer have any more marginal utility in terms of “units” of happiness, and at that
point the burden of having an abundance of wealth would outweigh the true benefit of whatever
it brings. This point (whatever monetary value it may be) would be the theoretical peak of
happiness. After that, happiness would plateau, and it would even eventually end up decreasing
After reviewing the research, the framing question turned out to be much more
complicated than anticipated, but my initial hypothesis showed a very strong resemblance to
what the researchers anticipated. That being said, just like many of the seemingly obvious
questions that we look at, there isn’t a direct “yes” or “no” answer because there are simply too
many other variables that factor into what it takes to make people happy. In other words, some
studies believed that you cannot just look at however much money someone makes and
magically create a gauge as to how happy they should be. Some people may have higher amount
of money, but if spending that money on materials and such doesn’t fit the individual's
personality, then it’s essentially useless. Other cases hypothesized that higher salaries do increase
one’s evaluation of their life, but their emotional well-being remained unchanged. This ended up
paralleling very well with my original hypothesis, as both proposed positive effects of money
While looking into this interesting topic, the research that was found offered more or less
two distinct answers in terms of how money can (or cannot) affect our happiness. On one side,
scholars gathered some 450,000 responses through assorted questions of everyday Americans
with a variety incomes and found out that while money doesn’t always correlate to a stronger
emotional well-being of an individual, it does lead to a better outlook on life. On the other side,
researchers focused more on how people spent whatever money they had, and they in turn found
out that money can indeed lead to increases in happiness if the money is spent on things that
suited the buyer’s personality. As a general consensus to most of the work done in these journals
and the initial hypothesis, income showed positive correlations with life evaluation, and spending
did seem to increase happiness when it was closely associated with an individual’s disposition,
but it was not unanimous throughout the research. There were certain limitations and exceptions
that ultimately went against the mentioned findings that prohibited income and emotional
One of the big ideas emphasized in some of the research was the underlying differences
how money affects people’s life evaluation versus the actual state of their emotional well-being.
Life evaluation was defined as the way one perceives the quality of his or her life, and this
tended to be much higher for people who were experiencing more money coming into their bank
account (Kahneman and Deaton, para. 2, 2010). The group then looked at the emotional
well-being of the individual, which they defined as “the frequency and intensity of experiences
of joy, fascination, anxiety, sadness, anger, and affection that make one’s life pleasant or
unpleasant”(Kahneman and Deaton, para. 2, 2010) and judged based on three specific reports:
happiness, enjoyment, and the frequency of smiling or laughter (Kahneman and Deaton, para. 8,
2010). Interestingly enough, there was not an obvious linear trend when compared with higher
incomes. However, they did notice that when we looked at initially lower incomes, the two
factors seemed to rise with each other, but as predicted there eventually came a peak point in
which this was no longer the case. The research inferred that “beyond about $75,000/yr, there is
no improvement whatever in any of the three measures of emotional well-being” (Kahneman and
Deaton, para. 15, 2010). A journal headed by Nikhil K. Sengupta helped elaborate on Kahneman
and Deaton’s inference by putting it in terms of basic economics. As salaries get bigger, “the
diminishing effects of increases in raw income merely reflect the diminishing marginal utility of
each added dollar” (Sengupta and Osborne and Houkamau, para. 12, 2012). More simply put, if
someone is already making $100 million/yr, giving him/her another $1 million will not produce
as much happiness as if it were given to someone who may only be making $50,000 per year.
The higher salaries get, the less important the additional money becomes for whoever may be
earning it, and if the money has zero importance, then there is no way it could continue to
increase happiness.
When looked at through a lens that consisted of analyzing the comparisons between what
an individual is like and how they spend their wealth, another conclusion arose regarding
whether or not money can buy happiness. By surveying participants of various personalities and
incomes, researchers matched transaction histories with personality tests. The general groups of
personalities and purchases were split between extroverts and introverts (extroverted purchase
examples being pubs or motor sports, introverted purchases being gardening or health insurance)
who were then asked to complete questionnaires that measured life-satisfaction after the
purchases were made (Matz and Gladstone and Stillwell, para. 13-18, 2016). This served as an
attempt to see if people experienced greater joy when they purchased things that correlated with
the kind of person they were. After crunching numbers and organizing the vast amount of the
data they collected, researchers confirmed that “personality-matched consumption indeed results
in higher levels of happiness” (Matz and Gladstone and Stillwell, para. 23, 2016). Elizabeth W.
Dunn, professor at the University of British Columbia, led a similar study that also aimed at
discovering whether or not spending money properly could indeed lead to an increase in
happiness. Her team came up with eight ways that concluded the fact that money increased
emotional well-being when spent the “right”way, but her first remedy further supported Matz,
experiences bring greater joy than material items, and how important it was for individuals to
spend money on personal experiences because “[they] are more centrally connected to our
identities” (Dunn and Gilbert and Wilson, para. 4, 2011). In other words, people spend money of
different experiences, and different experiences make them happy, which leads to the conclusion
that if individuals spend money on things that associate with their personality, happiness can
theoretically be bought.
Conclusion:
Even after studying four different cases from all across the world, the framing question
still resulted in two relatively different answers. According to some researchers, money does lead
to a better outlook on the quality of one’s life, but emotional well-being is only positively
affected up until a certain monetary value. It is at this point where the individual supposedly
peaks and receives no get gain of happiness from any extra amount of money. If we switch sides,
other researchers do believe that money can buy happiness, but only if the individual makes
purchases that are contingent with his/her personality. However, just because the answers
seemed to be contradictory doesn’t mean that one is right and one is wrong. Rather than
accepting one as truth and scrapping the other as fiction, the two answers could be combined to
form an solution that both answers the initial question and visits all possible facets of discussion
Deaton, Angus & Kahneman, Daniel. (2010). High income improves evaluation of life but not
Dunn, E. W., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2011). If money doesn't make you happy, then you
Matz, S. C., Gladstone, J. J., & Stillwell, D. (2016). Money Buys Happiness When Spending Fits
doi:10.1177/0956797616635200
Sengupta, N. K., Osborne, D., Houkamau, C. A., Hoverd, W. J., Wilson, M. S., Greaves, L. M.,
& ... Sibley, C. G. (2012). How much happiness does money buy? Income and subjective