Teaching For Understanding Perkins Article
Teaching For Understanding Perkins Article
TEACHING FOR
UNDERSTANDING
David Perkins
American Educator: The Professional Journal of the American
Federation of Teachers; v17 n3, pp. 8,28-35, Fall 1993.
the past and the present. Students are asked to think through
concepts and situations, rather than memorize and give back on the
quiz.
little good! What use can students make of the history or mathematics
they have learned unless they have understood it?
In the long term, education must aim for active use of knowledge and
skill (Perkins, 1992). Students garner knowledge and skill in schools so
that they can put them to work--in professional roles--scientist,
engineer, designer, doctor, businessperson, writer artist, musician--
and in lay roles--citizen, voter, parent--that require appreciation,
understanding, and judgment. Yet rote knowledge generally defies
active use, and routine skills often serve poorly because students do
not understand when to use them. In short, we must teach for
understanding in order to realize the long-term payoffs of education.
But maybe there is nothing that needs to be done. "If it ain't broke,
don't fix it." Perhaps students understand quite well the knowledge
and skills they are acquiring.
WHAT IS UNDERSTANDING?
astronauts fire the snowballs, they will begin to move away from one
another: Firing a snowball forward pushes an astronaut backward.
Moreover, each astronaut who fires a snowball will start to spin with
the very motion of firing, because the astronaut's arm that hurls the
snowball is well away from the astronaut's center of gravity. It's
unlikely that anyone would hit anyone else even on the first shot,
because of starting to spin, and the astronauts would soon be too far
from one another to have any chance at all. So much for snowball
fights in space.
you may listen carefully to the teacher and understand in the limited
sense of following what the teacher says as the teacher says it. But
this does not mean that you really understand in the more genuine
sense of appreciating these implications for situations the teacher did
not talk about. Learning for understanding requires not just taking in
what you hear, it requires thinking in a number of ways with what you
heard-- practicing and debugging your thinking until you can make the
right connections flexibly.
Research shows that very often students do not carry over facts and
principles they acquire in one context into other contexts. They fail to
use in science class or at the supermarket the math they learned in
math class. They fail to apply the writing skills that they mastered in
English on a history essay. Knowledge tends to get glued to the
narrow circumstances of initial acquisition. If we want transfer of
learning from students--and we certainly do, because we want them to
be putting to work in diverse settings the understandings they acquire-
-we need to teach explicitly for transfer, helping students to make the
connections they otherwise might not make, and helping them to
cultivate mental habits of connection-making (Brown, 1989; Perkins
and Salomon, 1988; Salomon and Perkins, 1989).
Certainly much more can be said about the art and craft of teaching
for understanding. However, this may suffice to make the case that
plenty can be done. Teachers need not feel paralyzed for lack of
means. On the contrary, a plethora of classroom moves suggest
themselves in service of building students' understanding. The teacher
who makes learning thinking-centered, arranges for rich ongoing
assessment, supports learning with powerful representations, pays
heed to developmental factors, inducts students into the disciplines
taught, and teaches for transfer far and wide has mobilized a powerful
armamentum for building students' understanding.
Much can be said about how to teach for understanding. But the "how"
risks defining a hollow enterprise without dedicated attention to the
"what"--what's most worth students' efforts to understand?
Page 12
A while ago I found myself musing on this question: "When was the
last time I solved a quadratic equation?" Not your everyday
reminiscence, but a reasonable query for me. Mathematics figured
prominently in my precollege education, I took a technical doctoral
degree, I pursue the technical profession of cognitive psychology and
education, and occasionally I use technical mathematics, mostly
statistics. However, it's been a number of years since I've solved a
quadratic equation.
The problem is, for students not headed in certain technical directions,
quadratic equations are a poor investment in understanding. And the
problem is much larger than quadratic equations. A good deal of the
typical curriculum does not connect--not to practical applications, nor
to personal insights, nor to much of anything else. It's not the kind of
knowledge that would connect. Or it's not taught in a way that would
help learners to make connections. We suffer from a massive problem
of "quadratic education."
What does generative knowledge look like (cf. Perkins, 1986, 1992;
Perrone, 1991a)? Consider a cluster of mathematics concepts rather
different from quadratic equations. Consider probability and statistics.
The conventional precollege curriculum pays little attention to
probability and statistics. Yet statistical information is commonplace in
newspapers, magazines, and even newscasts. Probabilistic
considerations figure in many common areas of life, for instance
making informed decisions about medical treatment. The National
Council of Teachers of Mathematics (1989) urges more attention to
Page 13
Or for instance, early this year, the Boston Globe published a series on
"the roots of ethnic hatred," the psychology and sociology of why
ethnic groups from Northern Ireland to Bosnia to South Africa are so
often and so persistently at one another's throats. It turns out that a
good deal is known about the causes and dynamics of ethnic hatred.
To teach social studies for understanding, one might teach about the
roots of ethnic hatred instead of the French Revolution. Or one might
teach the French Revolution through the lens of the roots of ethnic
hatred. It's knowledge that connects!
Whose history? It's been said that history gets written by the
victors. This theme addresses pointblank how accounts of history
get shaped by those who write it-- the victors, sometimes the
dissidents, and those with other special interests.
place, and the trouble with apples continues. Our efforts to serve up to
students the apple of plain old knowledge seem to be serving them
poorly.
What it all comes down to is this. Schools are providing the wrong
apple. The apple of knowledge is not the apple that truly nourishes.
What we need is the apple of understanding (which of course includes
the requisite knowledge).
Fortunately, many teachers are already far along the way toward
teaching for understanding, without any help from cognitive
psychologists or educational researchers. Indeed, some of our most
interesting work on teaching for understanding has been with teachers
who already do much of what the framework that we are developing
advocates. They are pleased to find that the framework validates their
work. And they tell us that the framework gives them a more precise
language and philosophy. It helps them to deepen their commitment
and sharpen the focus of their efforts.
The ideas discussed here were developed with support from the
Spencer Foundation for research on teaching for understanding and
from the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation for research
on thinking, for which I am grateful. Many of the ideas reflect
collaborative work with several good colleagues. I thank Rebecca
Simmons, one of those colleagues, for her helpful comments on a draft
of this paper.--D. P.
REFERENCES
Behr, M., Lesh, R., Post, T., and Silver, E. (1983). Rational-number
concepts. In R. Lesh and M. Landau (eds.), Acquisition of mathematics
concepts and processes (pp. 91-126). New York: Academic Press.
Brown, A.L., Ash, D., Rutherford, M., Nakagawa, K. Gordon, A., and
Campione, J.C. (in press). Distributed expertise in the classroom. In G.
Salomon (Ed.), Distributed cognitions. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Fiske, E.B. (1991). Smart schools, smart kids. New York: Simon &
Schuster.
Gardner, H. (1991). The unschooled mind: How children think and how
schools should teach. New York: Basic Books.
Inhelder, B., and Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from
childhood to adolescence. New York: Basic Books.
Reprinted with permission from the Fall 1993 issue of the AMERICAN
EDUCATOR, the quarterly journal of the American Federation of
Teachers.