Understanding Millennials
Understanding Millennials
research-article2017
BASXXX10.1177/0007650317726985Business & SocietyWeber
Article
Business & Society
1–36
Understanding the © The Author(s) 2017
Reprints and permissions:
Millennials’ Integrated sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0007650317726985
https://doi.org/10.1177/0007650317726985
Ethical Decision-Making journals.sagepub.com/home/bas
James Weber1
Abstract
Focusing on millennials, individuals born between 1980 and 2000 and
representing the largest generational population in our history, this research
seeks to understand their ethical decision-making processes by exploring
the distinctive, yet interconnected, theories of personal values and cognitive
moral reasoning. Utilizing a decision-making framework introduced in
the 1990s, we discover that there is a statistically supported relationship
between a millennial’s personal value orientation and stage of cognitive
moral reasoning. Moreover, we discover a strong relationship between
three of the four value orientations and a corresponding stage of cognitive
moral reasoning. The theoretical and practical research implications of our
discovery about millennials’ decision making are discussed.
Keywords
cognitive moral reasoning, ethical decision making, personal values
Exploding on to the business scene in the 21st century are millennials, indi-
viduals born between 1980 and 2000. They account for approximately 80
million people, exceeding the population of the baby boomers (born between
1946 and 1964) by 4 million. In 2016, there was a greater number of 26-year-
olds in the United States than any single group of individuals of a given age
(Ethics Resource Center, 2013).
Millennials are acknowledged as a powerful group in the workforce as
they become established leaders of business organizations, an influential
population of consumers, and a significant pool of investors. The sheer mag-
nitude of their potential, or increasingly actual, influence on business has
caused executives and policy makers to commit extensive resources to under-
stand, accommodate, and influence millennials. Goldman Sachs reports that
they have learned their lesson and accept that newly hired employees are not
as interested in staying with the firm for the long run. Rather, millennials are
seeking experience to catapult them into higher paying jobs with other firms
after a few years. According to a LinkedIn poll of a dozen of investment
banks surveyed in 2015, millennials stay an average of 17 months on the job,
compared with a 26-month average a decade earlier (Huang & Gellman,
2016).
Given the lifetime of experience with technology, millennials understand
the value of their talents and the role they can play in society. LinkedIn and
Oracle hired an army of millennial consultants, who charge as much as
US$20,000 an hour for their expertise, focusing on how to manage and mar-
ket to other millennials (Manjoo, 2016). Millennials also have exerted them-
selves into the media industry as an important target market. Verizon
Communications and Hearst teamed up to produce multiplatform online
video channels aimed at millennials from the heartland. In addition, Conde
Nast Entertainment created a demographic called cultured millennials, whose
members demonstrate a preference for artisan beer (Lyall, 2016).
While understood as the most educated and technology-savvy population
in the history of humankind (Philips, 2014), little is known about millennials
as moral actors in society and the ethical decision-making processes they
bring to their professional and personal lives. What lies at the core of the mil-
lennials’ thought processes leading to a reflective reasoning framework that
give rise to decisions and behavior? How do these cognitive processes—per-
sonal values and cognitive moral reasoning, for example—connect? If there
are linkages, how strong are they or under what circumstances do they occur?
Explorations focusing on the influence of antecedents of ethical decision
making abound in the business ethics literature (see O’Fallon & Butterfield,
2005, for a comprehensive review of this literature). Yet, this literature is
limited as it typically assesses individual, isolated elements of the complex,
Weber 3
Personal Values
“A value is a conception, explicit or implicit, distinctive of an individual or
characteristic of a group, of the desirable, which influences the selection from
available modes, means, and ends of action” (Kluckhohn, 1951, p. 395). This
generally accepted definition of a value conveys what is important to us in
our professional and personal lives. A person may place greater importance
on a set of values (e.g., being courageous, living a life of prudence, or temper-
ance) versus other values (e.g., accumulation of wealth or fame, or preferring
to be alone rather than with friends or family).
A particular, singular value may be very important to one person but unim-
portant or less important to another person even if they belong to the same
family, are employed by the same business organization, or are members of
the same generation. Values are deeply personal and individualistic. For
example, Hanke and Vauclair (2016) built on the earlier work by Hornsey and
Wohl (2013) to investigate the role forgiveness plays across various cultures.
They surveyed 41,975 subjects in 30 countries to uncover many cultural influ-
ences on the role forgiveness plays in individual’s lives and interpersonal
4 Business & Society 00(0)
each value as having equal membership (or weight) in the value orientation,
Weber was able to assign greater weight to certain values that more closely
represented the value orientations discussed by Rokeach (see Appendix A).
These categorizations and the weights assigned to the values are discussed
later in this article.
Researchers discovered a link between an individual’s personal values and
decisions made (Allport, Vernon, & Lindzey, 1960; Beyer, 1981) as well as
actions taken (House, Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman, & Gupta, 2004; Posner,
2010). Simply put, why people decide to act the way they do is often influ-
enced by their specific values.
Millennials are beginning to be assessed in terms of their values and the
impact of these beliefs on their behaviors. Carmichael (2016) refuted the
belief that millennials are not a hard-working generation more concerned
with “flex-time, beer carts, and nap rooms” by discovering that they see
themselves as “work martyrs” even more than prior generations (43% of the
millennials in the study compared with 29% of all survey participants).
Millennials overwhelmingly agreed with the following statements: “I feel
guilty for using my paid time off” and “No one else at my company can do
the work while I’m away” (Carmichael, 2016, p. 3).
Millennials exhibit positive social values when their value systems were
explored in a 2014 Millennials Impact Report (Johnson, 2015). Millennials
reportedly want to “make a difference” in the world, and want to align them-
selves with organizations that are making a positive impact on society
(Johnson, 2015, p. 4). While millennials recognize that technology has made
their life easier, few individuals from this generation manifested a poorer
work ethic when compared with members of previous generations. Millennials
seem to share many of the work-oriented values of past generations, accord-
ing to the research reported by Carmichael (2016) and Johnson (2015), but
constitute a complex and often-conflicted generation with various values and
beliefs.
When applied to business ethics, researchers found that certain personal
values create an ethical orientation that influences or promotes ethical behav-
ior or, conversely, leads to unethical behavior (Argandona, 2003) and are
connected to selected behavior in real-life situations (see Schwartz & Bardi,
2001, for a summary of previous literature). Turning to a business context,
Gehman, Treviño, and Garud (2013) advance the notion of values practices,
where values are recognized for their critical role in organizational perfor-
mance and add a normative dimension to behavior in business.
Therefore, we argue that insights into the millennial generation’s personal
value orientation can reveal an important dimension of their ethical decision-
making process leading to decisions and behaviors. This is the first step
6 Business & Society 00(0)
females, given that all of Kohlberg’s initial test subjects were males and, in
subsequent research, female subjects would be capped at Stage 3 of the
framework. This challenge was addressed by Kohlberg and his associates,
and adjustments were made to the stage scoring method. When Derry (1987,
1989) tested Gilligan’s challenge, she found no conclusive evidence discrim-
inating between female and male managers’ moral reasoning orientation.
Donleavy (2008) explored the Kohlberg–Gilligan controversy, and concluded
“ . . . what differences do exist [between an ethics of justice and an ethics of
care], within a business context, are far outweighed by the other explanatory
factors” (p. 816).
Krebs and Denton (2005) offered 11 propositions challenging
Kohlberg’s initial longitudinal study and the model’s structural consis-
tency and other measurement issues. Many of their objections are addressed
by Kohlberg and his associates in the rescoring of the stages. Other chal-
lenges are rendered inconsequential as these challenges tend to focus on
the weaknesses in Kohlberg’s initial longitudinal study of all male sub-
jects, which is avoided in this research, and the use of generalized moral
reasoning dilemmas, which are replaced in this study with business-con-
text dilemmas. Based on the decades of empirical research using Kohlberg’s
Stages of Moral Development Model applied in a business setting, we are
confident in using this theoretical framework to assess the cognitive moral
reasoning exhibited by millennials as a portion of their ethical decision-
making framework.
Educators believe that personal values and cognitive moral reasoning are
connected and should be used in various pedagogical approaches. Kvalnes
(2014) posited that personal values affect the reasoning process when stu-
dents face opportunities for dishonest behavior. Sivanandan, Thurasingam,
and Cho (2013) drew a strong connection between personal values and cogni-
tive moral reasoning when business students face various ethical and legal
challenges. Finally, Holland (2013) acknowledged that cognitive moral rea-
soning and personal values must be fused when students encounter the social,
legal, professional, and ethical aspects of information processing in the busi-
ness environment.
Psychological investigations also reveal strong linkages between values
and cognition. Daniel, Dys, Buchmann, and Malti (2014) examined Swiss
children and their development of sympathy, moral emotions, and social jus-
tice values, and compared these influences with moral reasoning at ages 6, 9,
and 12 years. The integration of values and cognition in children’s morality
occurred from middle childhood to early adolescence. Confirmatory evi-
dence was discovered by Pennycook, Fugelsang, and Koehler (2015), reveal-
ing the individual’s willingness to engage in analytical reasoning overrode
their gut feelings. Pennycook and colleagues (2015) found that analytical
thinkers were more skeptical about religious, paranormal, and conspiratorial
concepts. A five-factor model of personality in ethical decision making was
constructed to clarify the influence of empathy and personal values on ethical
competency (Pohling, Bzdok, Eigenstetter, Stumpf, & Strobel, 2016).
Supportive evidence led the authors to recommend that human resource man-
agers consider the relation between values and ethical decision making when
making hiring decisions at their firms.
A strong interconnectivity between a personal value orientation and moral
reasoning did not emerge in the management and business ethics literature
until the mid-1980s. A review of the business ethics literature uncovers a
handful of studies that explicitly look at the interconnectivity between per-
sonal values and cognitive moral reasoning. Directly relevant to our explora-
tion of millennials’ values and moral reasoning are the findings from Emler
et al. (1983); Weber (1993); Abdolmohammadi and Baker (2006); Myyry,
Siponen, Pahnila, Vartiainen, and Vance (2009); and Lan, Gowing, Rieger,
McMahon, and King (2008, 2010).
Emler and colleagues (1983) investigated morally mature subjects—
adults—and considered a person’s general value orientation, rather than
assessing the value preference for a single or limited number of values. After
measuring adult moral reasoning using Rest’s Defining Issues Test, they
found that subjects previously classified as right-wingers, but were asked to
respond as political radicals, produced higher levels of moral reasoning.
10 Business & Society 00(0)
value types affect the postconventional level of moral reasoning” (Lan et al.,
2010, p. 195).
With these scholarly investigations as a foundation, we offer the following
hypotheses as testable measures to understand the ethical decision-making
processes exhibited by our subjects, specifically their personal value orienta-
tions and stages of cognitive moral reasoning.
Hypotheses Development
Based on the initial explorations found in the six relevant studies profiled
above (Abdolmohammadi & Baker, 2006; Emler et al., 1983; Lan et al.,
2008, 2010; Myyry et al., 2009; Weber, 1993), there appears to be ample
evidence to suggest potential relationships between millennials’ personal
value orientations and stages of cognitive moral reasoning. Therefore, we
hypothesize as follows:
Stage 3 level of cognitive moral reasoning. Although the focus may be on the
decision maker—personal—there is an adult emphasis toward enhancing
one’s reputation among their immediate peer group, whether this be at home,
at school, or in the workplace. At this stage, the reasoning focus is conven-
tional—a focus on the immediate group—and consequential—a focus on
seeking positive outcomes for the referent group, family, peers, colleagues,
and coworkers. Therefore, we argue that most of the millennials responding
to our instruments will likely demonstrate a Personal-Competence value ori-
entation and reasoning at Stage 3 of Kohlberg’s stages of cognitive moral
development model.
moral reasoning when the apparent conflict is resolved. The personal value
orientation aligns with lower stages of cognitive moral reasoning, as we dis-
cussed earlier, due to the emphasis on the individual decision maker and a
concern for seeking attention from those in authority. We posited that a per-
sonal value orientation may be aligned with the cognitive moral reasoning
Stage 3—the good boy/nice girl reasoning.
Yet, the moral value orientation may be more closely related to higher
stages of moral reasoning. An emphasis on the postconventional level, or
Stage 5, of cognitive moral reasoning and the reliance upon universal ethical
principles would support the moral value orientation. Therefore, the personal-
moral value orientation may be aligned with Stages 3 and 5, reflecting both
lower and higher stages of cognitive moral reasoning. However, theoretical
foundations of cognitive moral reasoning and empirical findings based on
this theory do not support the occurrence of simultaneous reasoning at such
disparate stages (Colby & Kohlberg, 1987; Weber, 1993).
Where does this place the Janus-headed individual reasoner? Weber
(1993) suggested that one possible resolution is to hypothesize that an indi-
vidual with a personal-moral value orientation would reason at Stage 4 (p.
446). This places the individual within reach of Stage 5 cognitive moral rea-
soning, recognizing the moral value orientation and desire to apply universal
ethical principles, but not too distant from Stage 3 reasoning, supported by
the personal value orientation and a concern for earning approval from those
in authority or those who the decision maker holds in importance. As noncon-
secutive stages of reasoning would violate one of the tenets of Kohlberg’s
stages of cognitive moral development model and be counter to empirical
research, this compromise of a Stage 4 association with a personal-moral
value orientation seems reasonable. Therefore, the combination of a personal-
moral value orientation appears to be more interrelated than initially imag-
ined and associated with the properties of Stage 4 conventional level of
cognitive moral reasoning. Therefore, we hypothesize as follows:
Method
Sample
The sample consists of 208 junior and senior undergraduate business students
at a private university in the eastern region of the United States. All of the
subjects were born between 1981 and 2000, thus members of the biological,
age-based millennial generation. The average age of our sample is 21 years,
with 122 females (59%) and 86 males (41%). Although the regional- and
educational-limiting characteristics of this sample are noted later in this arti-
cle, this work does represent an initial effort in analyzing future business
leaders’ ethical decision-making processes to understand this generation and
what they may bring to the business workplace in the future.
Instruments
The Rokeach Value Survey was used to assess the importance individuals
assign to 18 terminal (personal or social end-states of existence) values and
18 instrumental (competence or moral modes of conduct) values. The
Rokeach Value Survey is widely accepted in values research (Vauclair,
Hanke, Fischer, & Fontaine, 2011) and, specifically, in assessing business
students’ values (McCarthy, 1997).
However, an important modification to the original Rokeach Value Survey
characterizing recent research is used here: Respondents are asked to rate,
rather than rank, their value preferences. Previous research found that by ask-
ing subjects to rate (rather than rank) values on a traditional Likert-type scale
enables subjects to consider fewer items at once, and makes the resulting
importance given to each value more plausible and reliable (Miethe, 1985).
The rating system allows individuals to appraise different values as being
equally important to them, and permits for the possibility that a given value
item will be negatively valued.
Weber (1990a) provided an important progression in the data analysis by
introducing quantitative-based membership or weights for each of the 36 val-
ues based on five prior works (Weber’s initial classification is shown in
Appendix A). For example, Weber found that A Comfortable Life and Inner
Harmony are consistently associated with, or grouped through factor analysis
to, a personal value orientation by all five of the prior investigations. Thus,
each of these values receives the maximum weight of 5, and are assigned to
the personal value orientation. Salvation, however, is found to be associated
with the personal value orientation in only four of the five prior studies and
once with the social value orientation; thus, Weber not only gives this value
18 Business & Society 00(0)
method and manual (Colby & Kohlberg, 1987; Weber, 1991). A randomly
selected sample of 21 surveys (10% of the total 208 surveys) was given to
two experienced Kohlbergian scholars for a blind, interrater reliability check.
One expert agreed with 20 of the 21 initial assessments of the predominant
stage of cognitive moral reasoning, whereas the other expert agreed with 18
of the 21 initial assessments. In all four cases where there was a discrepancy,
the difference was a single stage (3 times the experts coded the survey as
predominantly Stage 3, where the initial coding was Stage 4; and once the
expert coded the survey as predominantly Stage 3, when the initial coding
was Stage 2). The interrater reliability check was confirmed at a 90.4% rate
for complete agreement and 100% for a within one-stage agreement, well
within the acceptable standards for this test (Derry, 1987). From these
responses, a predominant cognitive moral reasoning stage score for each sub-
ject is determined. The complete instrument is shown in Appendix B.
Results
To assess the hypotheses linking the personal value orientation to a corre-
sponding stage of cognitive moral reasoning, a cross-tabulation table was
created (see Table 2). The assessment of the data in the cross-tabulation table
is based on an if–then relationship. For example, if the subject’s personal
value orientation is X, then the subject likely reasons at Stage Y. A chi-square
test is traditionally used to assess the statistical relationships within the con-
tingency table of the data distribution. As the expected frequencies—or dis-
tribution—is unknown in this exploration, a chi-square test of association is
used, rather than the more common chi-square goodness-of-fit test (Glass &
Hopkins, 1984). The chi-square test of association helps us determine if the
distribution of the subjects’ personal value orientation and stage of cognitive
moral reasoning are independent or dependent upon each other. The mathe-
matical properties and equation underlying the test of association are shown
in Equation 1: The Mathematical Properties and Equation Underlying the
Chi-square Test of Association.
r c
( Oi, j − Ei, j )2 .
χ2 = ∑∑
i =1 j =1
Ei , j
millennials in this study are assessed using the chi-square test of association,
and show a significant dependent relationship between the two variables (N
= 208; χ2 = 20.6624; p = .014). Therefore, we have overall support for the
hypothesized expectation that millennials’ personal value orientation is
related to their stage of cognitive moral reasoning.
We next explore the additional hypotheses that link specific personal
value orientations to particular stages of cognitive moral reasoning. We
adopted the first (of four) approach suggested by Sharpe—calculating
residuals. Delucchi (1993) recommended that the researcher identify
those cells with the largest residuals—the difference between the observed
frequencies and the expected distribution. A positive residual identifies
where the observed frequency is larger than the expected; a negative
residual reflects the reverse relationship. The larger the residual, the
greater the contribution of the cell to the magnitude of the chi-square
value. Variations among the residuals are affected by the overall size of
the population pool and the expected distribution within that sample. In
our sample, the sample populations range from 102 millennials (personal-
competence value orientation) to only 15 millennials (the social-compe-
tence value orientation). As stated by Agresti (2007),
Personal- 24 − 19 = +5 48 − 43 = +5 29 − 34 = −5 1 − 5 = −4 102
Competence
orientation
Social-Competence 1 − 3 = −2 5 − 6 = −1 9 − 5 = +4 0 − 1 = −1 15
orientation
Personal-Moral 10 − 11 = −1 19 − 25 = −6 23 − 20 = +3 6 − 3 = +3 58
orientation
Social-Moral 4 − 6 = −2 16 − 14 = +2 9 − 11 = −2 4 − 2 = +2 33
orientation
Column totals 39 88 70 11 208
Note. Observed frequency less expected distribution = residual. Bold values indicate hypothesized
relationship.
This exploration provides additional insights into the emerging role mil-
lennials may play as employees in the business workplace, as consumers in
the marketplace, and as investors in the financial markets. The confirmation
of the consistency of millennials’ personal value orientations and stages of
cognitive moral reasoning with previous explorations using managers as sub-
jects may give comfort to businesses seeking to understand how to accom-
modate or appeal to new millennial employees, consumers, or investors.
This discovery confirms other research exploring millennials and their
work habits. Hansen and Leuty (2012) explored generational differences in
work values, and conclude that the measured differences across baby boom-
ers (typically senior managers in businesses today), generation X, and mil-
lennials (our subjects) are quite small, and perceptions of work provide a
stronger explanatory power than age differences. An earlier summary of gen-
erational work values research acknowledges that differences are not always
found (Parry & Urwin, 2011), so perhaps there is more similarity across gen-
erations than differences when it comes to personal values, moral reasoning,
and workplace practices. Therefore, much of the managerial research that
evaluates behavior may be applicable to the newest generation of managers,
customers, and investors—millennials.
Alternatively, some variations may exist, and millennials may offer new
perspectives or practices within their ethical decision-making processes, as
employers are discovering. Firms are challenged to match the millennials’
ethics with their own organizational ethical culture. Are firms able to create a
culture or working environment that is attractive to this new generation of
employees? Perhaps if a firm emphasizes its concern for social impact or
fosters sustainable business practices, millennials with similar interests might
be willing to join and stay longer with that firm. There is a clear incentive for
firms to retain their valuable investments—new millennial employees. An
understanding of millennials’ personal value orientations, cognitive moral
reasoning, and the linkage between the two decision-making criteria may be
helpful for the firm. Will other firms be able to capitalize on their prior efforts
at organizational loyalty to motivate employee retention? It is plausible that
the millennials’ perception of “organizational flight” (Derry & Jago, 2015)
can be countered through incentives offered by businesses, given the millen-
nials’ personal value orientation.
Perhaps a lesson can be learned from the results found when scholars
administered the Motivators Assessment to more than 4,000 millennials.
Elton and Gostick (2017) found that millennials’ preference for “changing
the world” or “making a difference” might cause them to stay longer at a
business than previously thought or observed. If a firm is able to provide
opportunities for millennial employees to believe they are making a
Weber 25
Appendix A
Weights Assigned to Each Value in the Rokeach Value Survey
Based on the results from Weber (1990a), each value in the Rokeach Value
Survey is associated with one of the four value orientations, with the mem-
bership weight assigned to a specific value orientation:
Appendix B
The Adapted Moral Judgment Interview (AMJI)
(Source: Weber, 1991, pp. 314-317)
we are more interested in your answers to the “why” questions than to the
“what” questions. Even if you give a long description of what you think is
right or what you think should be done, it is of no help if you do not explain
why you think it is right or why you think it should be done. Answer each
question the best you can. Please do not compare an answer to prior answers.
OK, please begin the Moral Judgment Interview by reading the first story
on the next page.
Roger. Roger worked for a small auditing firm and conducted an annual
audit of a machinery manufacturer. During the audit, he discovered that the
firm had received a large loan from the local savings and loan association. It
is illegal for a savings and loan association to make a loan to a manufactur-
ing firm; law to mortgages based upon residential real estate restricts them.
Roger took his working papers and a xerox copy of the ledger showing the
loan to his boss, the partner in charge of the auditing office. His boss listened
to Roger, and then told Roger, “I will take care of this privately. We simply
cannot afford to lose a client of this status. You put the papers you have
through the shredder.”
1. Should Roger shred his papers? _________ (Yes or No) Why, or why
not?
2. Does the illegality of the loan and Roger’s duty as an auditor make a
difference in Roger’s decision to shred his papers?
3. If Roger had been advised by one of his peers to shred his papers,
should Roger shred his papers?
4. Is it important for people to do everything they can to follow their
conscience? Explain.
5. Shredding papers is against the AICPA Code and covers up an ille-
gally made loan. Is Roger also morally wrong if he shreds his papers?
6. What if Roger’s career was threatened if he refused to shred his
papers?
7. Should people do everything they can to further their own careers?
Acknowledgments
The author is appreciative of the statistical analysis support offered by Dr. Amy
Phelps at Duquesne University.
Funding
The author disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, author-
ship, and/or publication of this article: The author is appreciative of the financial support
provided by a grant from the A. J. and Sigismunda Palumbo Charitable Trust in 2015.
References
Abdolmohammadi, M. J., & Baker, C. R. (2006). Accountants’ value preferences and
moral reasoning. Journal of Business Ethics, 69, 11-25.
Weber 31
Huang, D., & Gellman, L. (2016, April 8). Millennial employees confound big banks.
The Wall Street Journal. Retrieved from https://www.wsj.com/articles/millen-
nial-employees-confound-big-banks-1460126369?mg=prod/accounts-wsj
Johnson, J. A., Hogan, R., Zonderman, A. B., Callens, C., & Rogolsky, S. (1981).
Moral judgment, personality, and attitudes toward authority. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 370-373.
Johnson, M. (2015, March). Stop talking about work/life balance! TEQ and the mil-
lennial generation. Workforce Solutions Review, 4-7. Retrieved from http://www.
ihrimpublications.com/WSR_Online_Archives/Meagan%20Johnson-WSR_
MAR2015web.pdf
Kluckhohn, C. (1951). Values and value orientations in the theory of action. In T.
Parsons & E. A. Shils (Eds.), Toward a general theory of action (pp. 388-433).
New York, NY: Harper.
Kohlberg, L. (1971). Stages of moral development as a basis for moral educa-
tion. In C. M. Beck, B. S. Crittenden & E. V. Sullivan (Eds.), Moral educa-
tion: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 23-92). Toronto, Canada: University of
Toronto Press.
Krebs, D. L., & Denton, K. (2005). Toward a more pragmatic approach to morality:
A critical evaluation of Kohlberg’s model. Psychology Review, 112, 629-649.
Kvalnes, O. (2014). Honesty in projects. International Journal of Managing Projects
in Business, 7, 590-600.
Lan, G., Gowing, M., Rieger, F., McMahon, S., & King, N. (2008). A study of the
relationship between personal values and moral reasoning of undergraduate busi-
ness students. Journal of Business Ethics, 78, 121-139.
Lan, G., Gowing, M., Rieger, F., McMahon, S., & King, N. (2010). Values, value
types and moral reasoning of MBA students. Business Ethics: A European
Review, 19, 183-198.
Lifton, P. D. (1985). Individual differences in moral development: The relation of sex,
gender and personality to morality. Journal of Psychology, 53, 306-334.
Lyall, S. (2016, May 15). Trying to pin down the mosaic of millennial tastes. The
New York Times. Retrieved from https://www.nytimes.com/2016/05/16/busi-
ness/media/trying-to-pin-down-the-mosaic-of-millennial-tastes.html
Manjoo, F. (2016, May 25). Corporate America chases the mythical millennial. The
New York Times. Retrieved from https://www.nytimes.com/2016/05/26/technol-
ogy/corporate-america-chases-the-mythical-millennial.html
McCarthy, I. N. (1997). Professional ethics code conflict situations: Ethical and value
orientation of collegiate accounting students. Journal of Business Ethics, 16,
1467-1473.
Miethe, T. D. (1985). The validity and reliability of value measurements. Journal of
Psychology, 119, 441-453.
Murphy, E. F., Jr., Mujtabab, B. G., Manyakb, T., Sungkhawanc, J., & Greenwood, R.
(2010). Generational value differences of baby boomers in Thailand. Asia Pacific
Business Review, 16, 545-566.
Myers, K. K., & Sadaghiani, K. (2010). Millennials in the workplace: A communica-
tion perspective on millennials’ organizational relationships and performance.
Journal of Business Psychology, 25, 225-238.
34 Business & Society 00(0)
Myyry, L., Siponen, M., Pahnila, S., Vartiainen, T., & Vance, A. (2009). What levels
of moral reasoning and values explain adherence to information security rules?
An empirical study. European Journal of Information Systems, 18, 126-139.
Nielsen Global Survey. (2015). Green generation: Millennials say sustainability
is a shopping priority. Retrieved from http://www.nielsen.com/us/en/insights/
news/2015/green-generation-millennials-say-sustainability-is-a-shopping-prior-
ity.html
O’Fallon, M. J., & Butterfield, K. D. (2005). A review of the empirical ethical deci-
sion-making literature: 1996–2003. Journal of Business Ethics, 59, 375-413.
Parry, E., & Urwin, P. (2011). Generational differences in work values: A review of
theory and evidence. International Journal of Management Reviews, 13, 79-96.
Pennycook, G., Fugelsang, J. A., & Koehler, D. J. (2015). Everyday consequences of
analytical thinking. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 425-432.
Philips, M. (2014, June 26). There are more 23-year-olds than any other age (and
they’re going to save the world). Bloomberg. Retrieved from http://shawameri-
can.com/there-are-more-23-year-olds-in-the-us-than-any-other-age-do-you-
know-how-to-sell-to-them/
Phillips, W. J., Fletcher, J. M., Marks, A. D. G., & Hine, D. W. (2016). Thinking
styles and decision making: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 260-
290.
Pohling, R., Bzdok, D., Eigenstetter, M., Stumpf, S., & Strobel, A. (2016). What is
ethical competence? The role of empathy, personal values, and the five-factor
model of personality in ethical decision-making. Journal of Business Ethics, 137,
449-474.
Posner, B. Z. (2010). Values and the American manager: A three-decade perspective.
Journal of Business Ethics, 91, 457-465.
Rest, J. (1986). Moral development: Advances in research and theory. New York,
NY: Praeger.
Reynolds, S. J. (2006). A neurocognitive model of the ethical decision-making pro-
cess: Implications for study and practice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91,
737-748.
Rokeach, M. (1968). Beliefs, attitudes and values. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York, NY: The Free Press.
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical
advances and empirical tests in twenty countries. Advances in Experimental
Social Psychology, 25, 1-65.
Schwartz, S. H., & Bardi, A. (2001). Value hierarchies across cultures: Taking a simi-
larities perspective. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32, 268-290.
Sharpe, D. (2015). Your chi-square test is statistically significant: Now what?
Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 20, 1-10.
Sivanandan, P., Thurasingam, A. S., & Cho, C. W. (2013). Impact of moral reasoning
on law and ethical decision-making: A comparative study on business majors at
higher learning institutions in the Klang Valley. International Journal of Business
and Economic Development, 1, 23-33.
Weber 35
Suddath, C. (2014, May 12-May 18). Selfies aside, millennials are more humble than
boomers. Business Week. Retrieved from https://www.bloomberg.com/news/
articles/2014-05-14/selfies-aside-millennials-are-more-humble-than-boomers
Swann, W. B. Jr., Gómez, A., Buhrmester, M. D., López-Rodríguez, L., Jiménez,
J., & Vázquez, A. (2014). Contemplating the ultimate sacrifice: Identity fusion
channels pro-group affect, cognition, and moral decision making. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 713-727.
Treviño, L. K. (1990). A cultural perspective on changing and developing organiza-
tional ethics. In R. Woodman & W. Pasmore (Eds.), Research in organizational
change and development (Vol. 4, pp. 195-230). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Treviño, L. K. (1992). Moral reasoning and business ethics: Implications for research,
education and management. Journal of Business Ethics, 11, 445-459.
Tuulik, K., Õunapuu, T., Kuimet, T., & Titov, E. (2016). Rokeach’s instrumental
and terminal values as descriptors of modern organization values. International
Journal of Organizational Leadership, 5, 151-161.
Twenge, J. M. (2010). A review of the empirical evidence on generational differences
in work attitudes. Journal of Business Psychology, 25, 201-210.
Twenge, J. M., Campbell, W. K., & Freeman, E. C. (2012). Generational differences
in young adults’ life goals, concern for others, and civic orientation, 1966-2009.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Retrieved from https://www.apa.
org/pubs/journals/releases/psp-102-5-1045.pdf
Vauclair, C.-M., Hanke, K., Fischer, R., & Fontaine, J. (2011). The structure of
human values at the culture level: A meta-analytical replication of Schwartz’s
Value Orientations using the Rokeach Value Survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 42, 186-205.
Warbreck, M. (2015, August 11). Marketing to millennials—Selfies,
hashtags and retweets. Retrieved from http://www.bizcommunity.com/
Article/410/423/132771.html#topstory
Weaver, G. R., Reynolds, S. J., & Brown, M. E. (2014). Moral intuition: Connecting
current knowledge to future organizational research and practice. Journal of
Management, 40, 100-129.
Weber, J. (1990a). Managerial value orientations: A typology and assessment.
International Journal of Value-Based Management, 3(2), 37-54.
Weber, J. (1990b). Managers’ moral reasoning: Assessing their responses to three
moral dilemmas. Human Relations, 43, 687-702.
Weber, J. (1991). Adapting Kohlberg to enhance the assessment of managers’ moral
reasoning. Business Ethics Quarterly, 1, 293-318.
Weber, J. (1993). Exploring the relationship between personal values and moral rea-
soning. Human Relations, 46, 435-463.
Weber, J. (2015). Discovering the millennials’ personal values orientation: A com-
parison to two managerial populations. Journal of Business Ethics, 143, 517-529.
Weber, J., & Gillespie, J. (1998). Differences in ethical beliefs, intentions, and behav-
iors: The role of beliefs and intentions in ethical research revisited. Business &
Society, 37, 447-467.
36 Business & Society 00(0)
Westerman, J. W., Bergman, J. Z., Bergman, S. M., & Daly, J. P. (2012). Are universi-
ties creating millennial narcissistic employees? An empirical examination of nar-
cissism in business school students and its implications. Journal of Management
Education, 36, 5-32.
Author Biography
James Weber (PhD, University of Pittsburgh) is professor of business ethics and
management, and executive director of the Institute for Ethics in Business at the
Palumbo-Donahue School of Business at Duquesne University. Along with Dr. David
Wasieleski, he is the coeditor of BAS 360, an annual book series sponsored by the
International Association for Business and Society, published by Emerald Group
Publishing. His research interests include the assessment of values, moral reasoning,
and ethical behavior on both the individual and organizational levels. He has pub-
lished in numerous academic journals such as Business & Society, Human Relations,
Journal of Business Ethics, and Organization Science. He is the coauthor of 15th edi-
tion of Business and Society: Stakeholders, Ethics, Public Policy, published by
McGraw-Hill Education, Inc.