Chapter One
Chapter One
INTRODUCTION
1.0 Introduction
The Malay language has existed since 500 years ago (Collins, 1989). Already
at that time, the Malay language was used as the lingua franca within the South East
Asian region (Asmah, 1976). Idris Aman (1996) reminds us that the Malay language
Archipelago. He adds that the Malay language was a language of high cultural
esteem which played a crucial role in the local economy, knowledge, law and
The Malay language comprises of several regional dialects. These dialects are
within these states were first formally established by colonial officials (Collins, 1989).
colonial official by the name of R.O. Windstet in 1935 (Collins, 1989). These dialects
were categorized according to geographical location mainly for the advantage of the
from Asmah, 1985) but according to Zaaba (Fazal Mohamed, Zaharani, Nor
Hashimah & Harishon, 2011), the number of dialects in Malaysia was eight, namely
the dialects of Johor, Melaka, Negeri Sembilan, Perak, Kedah, Kelantan, Terengganu
1
and Pahang. It has been attested that each state in Malaysia has its own local dialects
(Asmah,1977, cited in Fazal Mohamed, Zaharani, Nor Hashimah & Harishon, 2011).
These local dialects are a variation of the Malay language and differ in some linguistic
aspects (Abdul Hamid Mahmood, 1977 cited in Teeuw, 1961:43). These local
(Yule, 2000). Idiolects exist within these local dialects because of individual
recorded texts, transcribed word lists, regional dictionaries and other features exist.
Some of the reasons that explain these variations are historical (Asmah, 1981) and
geographical factors (Abdul Hamid Mahmood, 1981), and also the social background
of the speakers themselves (Adi Yasran, 2005). The regional dialects of Malaysia can
BM PURBA
Pre-historic Malay
BM KUNO BM INDUK
Ancient Malay Parent Malay
BM KLASIK
Classical Malay
Many studies have been conducted on the dialects found in Malaysia. Among
the most studied dialects is the Kelantanese dialect. Collins (1989:242) mentions that
almost every Malaysian linguist born in Kelantan has written about his or her
kampong dialect [and they] have provided valuable information about Kelantan
Malay. Abdul Hamid Mahmood (1996) supports this claim by saying that the
Kelantanese dialect continues to gain attention with more and more studies being
conducted on it. Studies that have been carried out on the Kelantanese dialect have
transcribed word lists, regional dictionaries, etc. (Collins, 1989). However, not many
studies have been carried out to investigate the extralinguistic features of Malay
A distinction must be made between language and dialect. Very often people
see both as similar. However, the difference between a dialect and a language is that
1989).
understood and used interchangeably (Chambers & Trudgill, 2002). This is because
3
accent refers to the way in which a speaker pronounces and therefore refers to a
(Chambers & Trudgill, 2002:5). In this study, dialect is defined not only in terms of
the differences in phonology but also in terms of the differences in grammatical and
lexical terms.
Dialects are found the world over and the attitudes people have towards
dialects is sometimes not very encouraging (Holmes, 2008; Honey, 1998). Several
factors have contributed to their perception. One of the factors is that mass media and
(Honey, 1998). Dialects, which are the non-standard variety of a language, are often
associated with substandard, low status, often rustic forms of language, generally
associated with peasantry, the working class, or other groups lacking in prestige
type of accent that rates high in status and competence (Honey, 1998). RP began to
gain prominence when the East Midlands dialect merged within the locale of
Cambridge, London and Oxford during the 15th century. Eventually, awareness on
codifying the dialect and pronouncing the words correctly ensued. With a heightened
awareness on speaking correctly, schools began to acquire and disseminate the now
standard form of accent to students and thus the rest of the population (Honey, 1998).
The obsession with sounding correct or posh (Holmes, 2008) lies in the fact
that being articulate in RP meant the opening of doors to better lifestyle opportunities.
Honey writes that standard forms are the expression of a complex of values
context of Malaysia, reflect a tension for Malaysians who are caught between
modernity and retaining traditions, i.e. between speaking the more standard form of
the Malay language (or even English) or speaking Malay in their dialects, either
northern, southern or east coast. Investigating dialects also serves to show how
politician campaigning to people within his or her locale or even when a religious
figure gives lectures or sermons in his or her dialect. There are instances where some
Malaysian speakers completely drop any hint of their dialect so as to maintain their
social status or even to exclude themselves from any so-called stereotypes associated
with a particular dialect. Regardless of the ideas Malaysians have about dialects be it
linguistic scenery and will continue to reflect the vastness of Malaysias linguistic
diversity.
to fascinate the researcher with its nuances and rhythms. When listening to speakers
of the Kelantanese dialect, one can and at times cannot distinguish what it is being
said due to the differences in lexical items and pronunciation between the Kelantanese
5
dialect and standard Malay. In addition, understanding the dialect and how it is used
on an extralinguistic level is an area that has not been widely studied by many
researchers including even those from Kelantan, thus making this study even more
relevant.
Malaysia, the language that experienced standardization was the Malay Language or
When the British colonized Malaya, they brought along the English language.
Consequently, with the many changes that the British imposed upon Malaya, they
eventually set up English schools for the privileged few who were taught the English
(Asmah, 1992). Standard Malay began to materialize formally with the codifying of
the Malay language in 1904 by R.J. Wilkinson (Asmah, 1992). Before this, the Malay
language was written in jawi or the Arabic script (Asmah, 1992). Later on, the
Malay language was romanized because the British government wanted the Roman
language ensued as is expected when a country gains its independence and establishes
its identity (Asmah, 1992). The Malay language continued to be taught in schools and
Malaysians (Asmah, 1992). With this awareness too, in the year 1957, the Malay
Asmah, the influence of the southern dialect had spread due to the hegemony of the
Johor Empire in the nineteenth century (1976:4). The Johor Empire extended over
areas around the vicinity of the Straits of Melaka, the eastern coast of Sumatera and
its adjacent islands, Pahang, Melaka and Terengganu. The southern dialect is easily
distinguished because of the final schwa // sound (Asmah, 1976). This characteristic
of the southern dialect can be heard on the mass media even today. The distribution
From the map, it can be seen that in Peninsular Malaysia, the geographical
division mainly caused by the natural landscape of the country has resulted in people
living in designated areas. People in these designated areas were unable to converse
with people from other areas; hence, the differences in dialects arose (Abdul Hamid,
The dialect that is being investigated against Standard Malay in this study is
the Kelantanese dialect. The Kelantanese dialect stems from Bahasa Melayu Induk or
Parent Malay (refer to Figure 1.0). All the local dialects in Malaysia except for the
Johor dialect had arisen from this same origin (Adi Yasran, 2005).
Geographically, the Kelantanese dialect can be found in the east coast region
mainly in Kelantan, the border between Kelantan and Terengganu, the border between
Kelantan and Pahang and some districts in South Thailand such as Golok River,
Narathiwat, Yala and Pattani (Abdul Hamid, 1977). It had its origins in Pattani (Adi
Yasran, 2005, Collins, 1989). According to Cuisinier (1936), Kelantan and Siam (the
old name for Thailand) were geographically accessible and this had inadvertently
influenced the local lingo. Such influences could be seen in the words within the
Kelantanese dialect that described the supernatural and mystical (Abdul Hamid
Mahmood, 1996: 1011). This is not surprising because Kelantan and Pattani had
already forged a diplomatic relationship for a very long time. In addition, the bond
between Kelantan and Thailand can be seen even from the dialectal similarities
broadcasted through a radio station from Narathiwat (Abdul Hamid Mahmood, 1977).
There are also some words loaned from the Thai language that have been assimilated
into the Kelantanese dialect lexicon such as bo (/pho/) which means stop
morphological and lexical aspects. The Kelantanese dialect differs from Standard
Malay and the other local Malay dialects because of its historical relationship with
Patani (Adi Yasran, 2005 & Collins, 1989). Inevitably, the Kelantanese dialect
possesses some phonological similarities with the Patani Malay dialect (Collins, 1989
& Mohd Taib, 1961). Collins and Hussin Dollah (1988:882) go on to say that the
dialectal similarities between the Kelantanese speakers and the people from Patani
were so close that the relationship between both regions was almost inseparable (cited
1.4.1 Vowels
The Kelantanese dialect possesses more vowels than the Standard Malay
dialect.
The difference is that the front and back vowels for the Kelantanese dialect are
10
1.4.2 Consonants
//
aspects:
sentence
sentence precedes the verb (Abdul Hamid Mahmood, 1996). However, the passive
structure for the Kelantanese dialect is different from that of Standard Malay.
The structure is passive because of the inclusion of the prefixes di-, ber-,
and ke-an or with the use of the word kena before the verb (Sulaiman Masri &
Ahmad Khair Mohd Nor, 2004).Additionally, the passive Standard Malay structure is
only possible when a transitive verb is within the sentence. The similarities between
the English and Standard Malay language structure for passives are as follows:
Active Passive
(Source: http://mindabahasa.wordpress.com/2007/06/16/15/)
12
The passive structure in the Kelantanese dialect differs from that of Standard
Malay too with the use of emphatic markers which are /nyo/ and /anyo/ before the
Active Passive
from that of Standard Malay is the use of the word di after the verb (Abdul Hamid,
Active Passive
English translation Mamat hits the snake. The snake was hit by
Mamat.
13
The word sudah or the contracted form dah means done or completed in
English. Abdul Hamid (1977) states that the use of the word doh in the Kelantanese
dialect differs from the use of sudah or the contracted form dah in Standard Malay.
In Standard Malay, sudah or dah appears before the verb. However, in the
Kelantanese dialect, doh appears after the verb. The differences can be seen below:
1.4.3.3 The Use of the Adverb of Degree Sangat and Benar in a Sentence
The word sangat in Standard Malay carries the meaning very in English.
means correct or true in Standard Malay. However, benar can also be used as an
sangat is placed before the adjective. However, in the Kelantanese dialect sangat
14
However, in the case of the adverb benar, it is placed at the same position
for both dialects that is, after the adjective and before the verb. For example:
There are some words in the Kelantanese dialect that are not found in Standard
15
There are some words found both in the Kelantanese Dialect and Standard
Malay, but they are pronounced differently and have different meanings, for example:
16
Thus, the previous paragraphs show that there are some distinct differences
but also about being extralinguistically apt when uttering them. In addition,
(Salzmann, 1993:198, cited in Raja Rozina, 2004), for solidarity (Raja Rozina, 2004)
and for social identification (Suzuki, 1998, cited in Guan & Park, 2009).
what to say and what not to say in any specific cultural context (Govindasamy,
1994). Maintaining good rapport and achieving solidarity is done through the use of
politeness.
Politeness in general terms is being tactful, modest and nice to other people
(Yule, 2000:134). When one does that, one is regarded as positively associated with
tolerance, restraint, good manners, and showing deference to other people (Blum-
Kulka, 2005:257). According to Brown and Levinson (1987, 1978), the aim of
17
Communicating politeness is connected with the concept that was put forth by
Goffman (1955) i.e. the consideration of another persons public self-image or face.
Goffman (1967) contends that an individual is vested with a face that portrays his or
her self-image in public and it should be treated with the utmost care. When people
interact with each other, they will consciously or sub-consciously consider certain
variables that help them determine the form that their speech will take (Longcope,
1995: 69). Such variables, amongst others, are the consideration of being polite when
interacting. In maintaining good rapport and achieving solidarity with the other
Goffmans face theory was extended by Brown and Levinson (1978, 1987).
According to them, face is the public self-image that every member wants to claim
for himself (1978:66). This public self-image reflects two types of faces which are
the positive face and the negative face (Brown and Levinson, 1978, 1987). The
positive face concerns the desire to be appreciated and approved by selected others
while the negative face concerns a persons want to be unimpeded and free from
or non-verbally, occurs atypically to the face of the other speaker, then this would be
threaten a persons negative face would be to impose our needs over the other
18
an individual has five super strategies to help him or her mitigate the FTA.
person chooses to do the FTA, he can express his intention by going on record baldly
chooses to go on record with redressive action, he will need to submit his intention to
Basically, this is a simple dynamic of the lesser the imposition of the act, distance and
power of the other participant, the less polite one will have to be (Culpeper, 1996).
better.
(Pillai, 2008)
and Asian cultures at large. Politeness in the Asian context is the overall aptitude and
attitude of an individual of not offending another persons self worth or face. In the
19
Malay language, the overall aptitude or attitude of not offending another persons self
worth and face is known as one having budi bahasa (Raja Rozina, 2004). Budi
bahasa is of the utmost importance within the Malay community because upholding
another persons face is more important than ones own face. Jamaliah (2000)
emphasizes that being polite is valued highly within the Malay community (and other
be positively evaluated by society as being a person who has nilai diri or self-worth
and also has air muka or face. This individual is then considered as one who has
adab i.e. manners and grace (cited in Noor Ashikin Ghazali, 2006).
Malay dialects using Brown and Levinsons politeness theory. The argument here is
certain mechanism is employed to maintain solidarity among the speakers. For some
study intends to discover how speakers who use two different Malay dialects maintain
good rapport during the conversations with reference to Brown and Levinsons
politeness theory.
20
The Kelantanese dialect and Standard Malay may come from the same origin
but both dialects do not share similarities in phonological aspects and even in the
meaning of some words. Differences may also occur at the extralinguistic level. It is
therefore the aim of this study to look into what politeness strategies appear when
speakers of two Malay dialects converse. This study also aims to add to the existing
speakers?
The current study can provide a platform for other researchers to pursue
21
Besides that, this study also hopes to add to our knowledge of how Brown and
Levinsons theory can be utilized when analyzing Malay dialects. Although current
research has shown that there are some shortcomings, the theory has been able to
It will be interesting to see how the theory can be used for the study of the
This study is limited to only two Malay dialects. Therefore, the findings
cannot be representative of the other Malay dialects. This study also only looked into
the aspect of politeness and excluded other influencing factors such as power,
educational background, demographics, gender and culture. In addition, the study did
not include video recordings of the conversations between the participants. Thus,
only what was heard was analyzed; the body language and facial expressions of the
1.11 Conclusion
On the whole, this chapter has discussed the aims, rationale and purpose of the
study. This chapter has also introduced the concept of politeness that is integral to the
foundation of this study. The next chapter will discuss Brown and Levinsons theory
22
CHAPTER TWO
LITERATURE REVIEW
2.0 Introduction
This chapter provides an overview of the relevant ideas that provide the basis
for the theoretical framework used in this study. A section is dedicated to defining
politeness theory. The subsequent sections discuss local studies which incorporate
Brown and Levinsons theory. The next sections deal with dialect studies in
Malaysia.
the word politeness itself. Politeness can mean many things to many people. This
can be seen in the study conducted by Ide (2006) where the concept of politeness was
different between American English and Japanese (cited in Yuka, 2009:60). If one
were to look up the meaning for the word polite, a variety of synonyms and
definitions will appear. This is the case for how Americans would look at being polite
as similar to being friendly whilst the Japanese do not share this sentiment (Ide, 2006
cited in Yuka, 2009). The Japanese would see politeness and friendliness as two
separate entities. For example, politeness in the Japanese context is reflected in the
use of Japanese honorifics which are a system to express only politeness, and not
23
their definitions can be challenged as the very nature of their definitions is still open
for scrutiny, which then only adds up to the incongruity of the definition of politeness.
Back in the 18th century, politeness was associated with a particular group in
2005) to a particular group in society mainly because they were the ones who were
socially and politically privileged. France (1992) says that politeness was thus
structure, and it was a means of enforcing social differences (cited in Watts, 2003:33)
would often be associated with good manners, as polite behavior, including polite
dictionary definition means behaving in a way that is socially correct and shows
Learners Dictionary, 2008). Showing care for other peoples feelings requires one to
Dictionary, 2008). These qualities have to be taught and are generally not inherent in
24
Nonetheless, as has been pointed out earlier, politeness can mean different
things to different people. For some, being polite is a mechanism for people to avoid
face wants and preferences for the use of certain politeness strategies (Knapp-
Janney, 1984, cited in Watts, Janney & Horst Arndt, 2005). Therefore, a mechanism
Kasper (1990:194) also sees politeness as a set of strategies utilized to defuse anger
and minimize antagonism because humans have the potential to be involved in hostile
conflict avoidance which can be measured in terms of the degree of effort put into
comity.
assertive such as becoming verbally less explicit, prosodically less emphatic and
kinesically less direct and also by signaling acceptance of the partner by wording
negative messages in positive ways, using a pleasant tone of voice or smiling etc.
25
(Watts, Janney & Arndt, 2003:50). Thus, politeness is not only a sign of good
that politeness is culture bound. Kunmer (2005) says that different cultures vary from
one another and how each culture and perhaps even sub-culture defines politeness can
be extremely different. Watts (2003) would describe these differences across cultures
as being culturally relative. Cultural relativity refers to scenarios where one culture
would not share another cultures interpretation of politeness due to factors such as
Most Asian languages reflect politeness in the use of honorifics and forms of address.
Braun (1988) has documented forms of address across cultures and concludes that
forms of address are correlated highly with the parameters of dominance and social
distance in all speech communities which form part of the linguistic system (cited
Children of all Malaysian ethnicities are commonly taught to address their older
neighbours as Aunty or Uncleeven though they are not related. Malay children are
taught to address their aunts and uncles with a designated term such as Mak Long
and Pak Long for the eldest aunt and uncle and Mak Su and Pak Su for the
youngest aunt and uncle. These forms of address are a reflection of politeness in
language in the Malaysian context. The Japanese also have politeness embedded in
their language. An example would be the intricate honorifics system. It is said that
difficult it is to theorize politeness. Watts (2003) says that it is not easy to define
language, we would be able to base a theory of politeness against that theory. This is
When there is a theory of human language, a linguist can try to abstract away from
the ways in which we use it (human language) (Watts: 2003:48) and thus define what
politeness is. Watts reiterates that politeness should be assigned a timeless, placeless
Unfortunately, as Watts himself points out, this is not the case. Human language will
(Watts, 2003:48).
depending on how one sees it. Nevertheless, regardless of all the different opinions
people have when defining politeness, it can be agreed upon that politeness is a
persons consideration towards another person. Cupach & Metts (1994: 2) have
observed that people agree tacitly to support each others performance or face;
hence, being considerate is an act of reciprocity which in turns deflects any potential
27
politeness was an extension to Goffmans (1967) face theory. Goffman came about
with this theory where he describes that an individual tends to act out what is
sometimes called a line- that is, a pattern of verbal and nonverbal acts by which he
expresses his view of the situation and through this his evaluation of the participants,
especially himself (2005:5). Goffman goes on to clarify that face is the positive
social value a person effectively claims for himself (2005:5). Positive social value
refers to ones identity and esteem (Ng, 2009). Goffman (2005) also describes that a
person functions as a social actor. If one does not support anothers face or
performance, then one has caused embarrassment to another resulting in the person
having an out of face experience or losing face (Cupach & Metts, 1994).
From the face theory put forth by Goffman, Brown and Levinson introduce a
comprehensive and universal theory of politeness. Face, is the public self-image that
every member wants to claim for himself (Brown and Levinson, 1978:66).
According to Brown and Levinson (1978), every individual has two types of face,
positive and negative (cited in Watts, 2005). The positive face is an individuals
need to feel appreciated and approved by others (Watts, 2005). The negative
face is respecting other peoples personal space and desire for freedom (Watts, 2005).
It is an obligation for individuals to ease any form of hostility when conversing; thus
Brown and Levinson proposed a politeness strategy that would support or enhance
the addressees positive face by means of positive politeness and avoid transgression
Ideally, the speaker would assess his speech for any possible face threats that
he or she is about to make (Watts, 2005:86). The reason for this is to enable the
(1987:65) insisted that politeness must be given and that speakers need to say things
which help them to mitigate the imposition inherent in speech acts, and the face that
those speech acts threaten includes the face of the speaker as well as the face of
others.
The means of mitigating the imposition can be seen in the five superstrategies
Based on the diagram above, opting for any of the strategies carries the burden
of causing lesser or greater face loss for the individuals involved in the conversation.
This occurs due to three factors which are power, distance and ranking. If one
decides to borrow money from somebody, one can make that request by going on
29
record or off record. Borrowing money (not taking into account the amount of
money) by going off record would be a request that is layered with indirectness that
does not at all reflect the main intention of the borrower. For example, I really need
to get something at the grocer but I did not bring my wallet along (own example).
On the other hand, the request can also be made on record with two alternatives, that
is, without redressive action or by going bald on record which involves doing it in the
most direct, clear, unambiguous and concise way possible (Brown and Levinson,
record with redressive action. A request with redressive action can accommodate a
persons positive face by means of positive politeness and a persons negative face by
would be by first acknowledging any outstanding qualities the other person may
request sensitive to ones positive face is Hey, thats a great suit you have on! Is it
new? () By the way, may I borrow your car tomorrow? (adapted from Brown and
negative politeness (addressing negative face) would be to say You couldnt by any
chance loan me your car, tomorrow, could you? (adapted from Brown and Levinson,
1978:141). Longcope says that the borrower here tries to show his/her sensitivity to
the other persons personal space by implying that he/she does not think the other
30
a theory cited as exhaustively as Brown and Levinsons only proves that this theory
perceived to be. Kasper states that politeness is ethnocentric and that it is mostly
2001). This realization was made after numerous studies, mainly on non- English
speaking cultures, incorporated Brown and Levinsons theory only to discover that
some of the strategies posed by Brown and Levinson did not provide justifications for
their analysis (Chen, 2001). Furthermore, these strategies were unable to account for
(Watts, 2003:102).
Brown and Levinson (1987:62) claim that the face wants of both hearer and
bears more weight on the face of the hearer than ones own face. Hence, there is an
imbalance here. Werkhofer (1992) claims that all Brown and Levinsons theory does
goes on to say that the genuine intention of a person is not realized because the
individual must submit to certain manners of interaction that is in accordance with the
strategies provided by the said theory. This claim is further substantiated with
31
Pfisters (2010:1270) opinion that an utterance can be polite even if the speaker does
In relation to the current study, Lim (2003) discovered that in most cases the
strategy that is most common among Malay speakers is positive politeness (cited in
Noor Ashikin, 2006). Lim (2003) mentions that Malay speakers rarely pay attention to
the exact request; instead, what is of the utmost concern to them is how the utterance
is expressed. Thus, Brown and Levinsons theory would not be able to explain this
Brown and Levinsons model can only deal with certain elements of the data, for
example where participants are overtly and clearly polite (Mills, 2003:57-58).
Reiterating Mills claims, Fukushima (2002) says that certain politeness strategies
such as off-record strategies and dont do the FTA have not been given much
attention. This is probably due to the nature of the strategy being less obvious than
does not communicate politeness, this individual is taken as one who lacks a polite
demeanor (1987, cited in Pfister, 2010). Pfister (2010) says that Brown and
1270). This claim is rather presumptuous. The reason is, as Pfister (2010:1270) says,
in reality, there would be instances where an utterance can be polite even if the
speaker does not mean anything polite and what is implicated must be meant by the
different people. It was found that Uruguayans would comply with requests if the
request were made indirectly while the Japanese would request more directly
(Fukushima, 2002). Being direct does not necessarily mean the person is intrusive.
The notion of face based on the definitions laid by Brown and Levinson places
However, many scholars disagree with this notion of face because based on empirical
research, face for some societies is not individualistic. Instead, face is mostly based
Mau (1994) lends support to the idea that face cannot be limited to the confines of an
independent member of society. Mao (1994) says that there are two views of face
that is the individual (Brown and Levinson, 1978) and social (Matsumoto, 1988 and
33
Culpeper (1996) said that if one intends to discuss politeness, one must include the
deeming it as such and vice versa. Is it always the case that an utterance that is
use of strategies that are designed to have the opposite effect -- that of social
disruption (p.350). He says these strategies were meant to attack another persons
Levinson, Eelen (2001) stated that by conceptualizing face redress and FTAs, Brown
simply avoiding redressive strategies and instead using bald on record strategies
Additionally, there are scholars who are generally more receptive to Brown
and Levinsons theory. Cheng (2001) claims that despite the many criticisms against
Brown and Levinsons theory, especially for it being more production focused, the
theory if refined more carefully does take into account the face of the self (the
speaker). This claim is refuted by some scholars who claim that Brown and
Levinsons theory says politeness bears more weight on the hearer than on the
speaker. This submission is essential in Brown and Levinsons terms because one
or kesopanan is extremely important for the Malay community because ones good
manners or budi bahasa reflects ones upbringing which indirectly mirrors a parents
parenting abilities.
Malaysian children are taught to address their elders with specific forms of
address and are taught to speak with their elders with reverence. Requesting an item
from a parent is also made with utmost respect. Malaysian children are taught that
they should not have the audacity to ask their father directly for an item. For
example, children may tell stories about how a particular friend of theirs owns an item
with the intent of hinting indirectly to their father that they would like to have that
item too. This is a reflection of upholding politeness by beating around the bush
(Asmah, 1992). Part of the reason why Malaysian children do not go up front with
their requests is because Malaysian children are taught to not expect too much. Being
sensitive of their place in the family institution and because they do not want to be
accused of being rude, Malaysian children would never ask their parents or any adults
Asmah (1992), the use of a surrogate party. Communication via a surrogate party in
35
the Malay language arises when there is a big gap between the message originator
and its receiver (Asmah, 1992:185). Such situations can be seen when a mother-in-
law would not talk face-to-face with her son-in-law, instead she would use her
this happens because some people would say that they literally do not know how to
speak to the other party. In the example given previously, the mother-in-law probably
felt unsure about whether or not the matter that she wanted to convey to her son-in-
law was something that both of them would agree upon. Perhaps it was the intention
of the mother-in -law to save the face of her son-in-law and maybe her own face too.
Other than that, the Malay language also consists of two varieties, that is the
High Variety and the Low Variety (Asmah Haji Omar, 1988). Within these two
varieties, a set of sub-varieties exists too. These varieties all serve specific purposes
according to situations. This unique trait makes the Malay language sophisticated in
its own way and elements of politeness are also manifested through these varieties.
The High Variety of the Malay language is Royal Malay. Royal Malay is a
variety that is used exclusively within the royal grounds and whenever there is
interaction between commoners and the royal family (Suraiya, 1998). The Low
Refined Malay and Colloquial Malay (Asmah Haji Omar, 1988). Standard Malay or
Educated Malay is the Malay variety that is acquired in schools and used throughout
the mass media. The unique quality of these two varieties is that there is no
discrimination between the Low and High varieties. Usage of the High Variety
36
Politeness in the Malay language can be seen in every aspect of how the
language is used. It is known that in traditional Malay writing, the utterances are
usually very lengthy and indirect (Asmah Haji Omar, 1992). Major events such as
engagements and wedding ceremonies are a testimony to this claim. The beauty of
the interaction between both parties lies in the fact that despite the long-winded
between both parties is felt and a pleasant banter takes place between the families
representatives. Guests are entertained with humorous exchanges which are filled
with polite elements such as beating around the bush, imageries, and pantuns
(Asmah Haji Omar, 1992). This sub-variety is known as Refined Malay. To some,
being long-winded and indirect shows passivity and no strong conviction of ones
self. Asmah (1992:180) would refute this view claim by describing that one who has
such refined skills in the Malay language is one who is at the height of his finesse
and decorum and it is at this time that the speaker shows sensitivity to the hearers
negative face, that is by not being too upfront with the request and thus causing
Thus far, most of the studies conducted on Malay dialects have focused
mainly on the linguistic attributes of the dialects. These studies have been carried out
ever since the British colonized Malaya withR.O. Windstet being one of the very first
37
Mahmood, 1996). There were other studies conducted by British officers but these
the studies (Abdul Hamid Mahmood, 1996). These studies were carried out by
officers to aid them in understanding the local dialects so as to go about their duties
easily. These officers then shared their knowledge of the dialects with the other
younger officers (Abdul Hamid Mahmood, 1996). One dialect that was studied by
these officers was the Kelantanese dialect. Unfortunately, the Kelantanese dialect was
Mahmood, 1996). This was a grave error as the transcription of the dialects was
incompatible with the exact utterances of the Kelantanese dialect (Abdul Hamid
Mahmood, 1996).
institutions, studies on Malay dialects began to increase as more local graduates and
scholars delved into the Malay language (Khazaai, 1997). The methodology used to
conduct the studies on the Malay language and dialects alike were more systematic
and sound. One such establishment that contributed to the revival of the Malay
language is the Malay Language Department in the University of Malaya that was
once under the tutelage of Zainal Abidin Ahmad or Zaba and Tan Sri Prof. Emeritus
of them was Tan Sri Prof. Emeritus Dato Dr. Haji Ismail Hussein. Ismail Hussein
38
conducted a study on the phonological spread of the Malay dialects (Khazaai, 1997).
In his book Malay Dialects in the Malay Peninsula, Ismail Hussein (1973) discussed
the dialectal concept of weak and strong dialects found in peninsular Malaysia. He
explained that this dialect mixture tends to eliminate the weaker dialects and sustain
the stronger dialects which were more prominent in the city (1973: 69). Ismail
Hussein (1973) also categorized the dialects in his study into fourteen divisions. They
are as follows:
Sungai Perak Malay Dialect This dialect is found along the Perak River and is
connected to Kampung Gajah at Selatan Parit.
Pulau Sayong Malay Dialek This dialect is found around the Perak River and
Kuala Kangsar town.
Central Perak Malay Dialect The dialect is found in the area from Kuala Kangsar
and towards the north and south of Perak.
Selangor Malay Dialect This dialect is found around Selangor and Kuala
Lumpur.
Negeri Sembilan Malay Dialect This dialect is found mainly within Negeri
Sembilan and is also heard along the border of
Melaka, all over Alor Gajah, Jelebu and Linggi.
Melaka Malay Dialect This dialect is widely spoken within Melaka and
around the coast of Selangor and south Melaka.
Lenga Malay Dialect This dialect is found in some areas of the Muar
River and Muar.
39
Johor Malay Dialect This dialect has a lot of similarities with Standard
Malay.
Pahang Malay Dialect This dialect is the least stable. There are influences
of the Terengganu and Johor dialects around the
central parts of the Pahang River and moving north
of Pahang; it has some resemblance to the
Kelantanese dialect.
Among the notable studies on local Malay dialects include dissertations such
as Loghat Kelantan: Suatu Cerakinan Kaji Bunyi Bahasa by Nik Safiah Karim
(1965), Fonem Dialek Kelantan by Abdul Hamid Mahmood (1971) and Noor
among undergraduates and post graduates alike. Scholars like Nik Safiah Karim,
dialect; she herself is Kelantanese. Nik Safiah had collected data by recording
conversations and transcribing the data (Zahid & Shah Omar, 2006). Nik Safiah
(1965) explains that there is a difference between the Kelantanese dialect speakers in
the city and those from the villages. This claim echoes Ismail Husseins observation
too. In her dissertation, she included a historical account of the Kelantanese dialect
Continuing on the same thread as Nik Safiah, Abdul Hamid Mahmood (1971)
studied the phonemes of the Kelantanese dialect. Abdul Hamid (1971) looked into
40
the phonemes and phonemic distribution of the dialect. His findings showed that the
nasal phonemes in the Kelantanese dialect were phonemic (Abdul Hamid Mahmood,
1971). Hashim Musa produced the next two studies on the Kelantanese dialect which
were equally important. The first dissertation on the Kelantanese dialect looked into
prosody and intonation in the Kelantanese dialect specifically in the Pasir Mas area.
The study discovered that a particular type of intonation was present (Abdul Hamid
Mahmood, 1996). The next dissertation written by Hashim Musa was Morfemik
According to Abdul Hamid Mahmood, other local institutions also studied the
Kelantanese dialect (1996). The studies conducted were more varied and covered a
wider aspect. However, from the list of examples given in his article, most of the
studies conducted on the Kelantanese dialect only investigated the linguistic aspect of
the dialect. One study is Gangguan dialek Kelantan dalam pembelajaran dan
interference of the Kelantanese dialect in the teaching and learning of the language:
A case study of schools in Kota Bharu) by Hamim Haji Abd Rahim (1987). Another
dialek Kelantan: Satu kajian permulaan di daerah Banggul (Some aspects of the
M. Pakhrudeen Haji Yusof (1982). Adi Yasran (2005) also looked into the linguistic
account for the basic syllable types in the Kelantan Malay dialect based on Optimality
Theory (OT) (Adi Yasran, 2011:122). His paper argues that the Optimality Theory
41
Zalina Mohd Zalzali (2003). She explored the lexical and phonological attributes of
the Terengganu sub-dialect within the Rusila province of Marang and Kampung Bukit
Besar in Terengganu. Zalina (2003) was also interested in seeing the dialect
continuumbetween the areas of Kuala Terengganu and the Rusila province. Another
study that looked into the phonological and lexical aspects of a sub-dialect is by Mohd
Januri Ayob (1999). Mohd Januri investigated the Perak Malay dialect in Bota and
Lambor. Other studies also investigated the influence of dialects and sub-dialects in
the teaching and learning process. These include Lee Wee Kiats case study on the
influence of the Negeri Sembilan dialect on the teaching and learning of Bahasa
Malaysia in schools (2002). Again, the focus of these studies was mainly on the
Basically, the studies on Malay dialects to date have been mostly based on the
linguistic features of the dialects. Effort must be made to increase the number of
42
Gidung (1992) looked into the concept of politeness in Malaysian culture by means of
kinship terms and greeting systems. She investigated these aspects of her native
strategies used in the colloquial Malay of the 1960s when making requests and
responding to offers and requests in Malay plays (Raja Rozina, 2004:i). From her
findings, she discovered that there was a general tendency to address positive face
functions through the use of emphatic stress, capitalization and excessive punctuation
2009: i). The research revealed that her participants employed certain linguistic
disagreements. Vasudevan (2008) had a group of students discuss certain issues. The
data showed that the participants in her study opted to use the bald on record strategy
participants lack the pragmatic skill to use the positive politeness, negative politeness
43
and the off record strategies to disagree in a polite and less assertive manner
communication between the Japanese and Malaysians. Having the advantage of being
able to speak Japanese, Suraiya sought to identify how one amends a sociopragmatic
failure when conversing with someone who comes from a different cultural
background. She justified this failure by saying that our different styles of speaking
Mohd Ali, 1998:5). From her analysis, Suriaya justifies that even if culture is said to
On a different note, Kuang, David, Lau and Ang (2011) sought to answer a
simple research question, that is Are front counter staff of Malaysian government
investigated this by analyzing what the counter staff said to their clients/ patients
during openings and closings. The results obtained showed that the counter staff from
government hospitals were likely to respond in a less polite manner as not only were
openings seldom performed with courtesy, the use of closings too were limited in
meetings. She investigated the types of speech acts and discourse strategies
employed in displaying politeness and whether these politeness traits are reflective of
the participants cultural background (Fariza, 2009). The research was conducted on
two organizations: the Ministry of Finance and a multinational company. The results
showed that the most common choice of speech act was directives which indirectly
displayed power. Other discourse strategies utilized during the meetings were hedges
and humor which helped to downplay the power distance between the superiors and
subordinates. Politeness strategies were also found during the meetings with the
intention of attending to the goals of the speaker and the hearer (Fariza, 2009).
theory was found inapplicable due to the nature of the theory itself i.e. it was more
individualistic.
Ghazali (2006). The dialects that she investigated were the northern dialect and the
southern dialect i.e. Standard Malay. The objectives of her study were to identify any
conversations of her respondents. She also wanted to see if there were any
and Levinson (1987) in Malay. By recording conversations, Noor Ashikin looked into
four out of the ten strategies addressed to negative politeness. They are:
45
1) The use of particles such as lah or la, pun and kan to soften
3) Indirectness;
4) Deflection expressions.
The research questions that guided her research were basically to investigate if
the four strategies were applied in the conversations between speakers of the two
dialects (Northern dialect and Standard Malay) within the domain of friends and
families and to see if usage of any of the strategies were more specific to either dialect
or was equally common in both dialects. Lastly, she wanted to see which of the four
strategies was more frequently used by her participants. The results from her findings
show that negative politeness strategies are applied by both groups of speakers either
in social or family domains (Noor Ashikin, 2006: i). The four strategies that she
investigated were found in the data but the frequency of each strategy differed.
Factors that affected the frequency included influence of the interlocutors, topics and
the speakers motives (Noor Ashikin, 2006:i). Generally, the study affirmed that
politeness is still very much emphasized in Malay culture and is still applicable to
46
Since speech is a natural human activity, there will be many types of speech
acts produced by humans. Speech acts as defined by Searle, Kiefer and Bierwisch
(1980: vii) is the assumption that the minimal unit of human communication is not a
sentence but rather the performance of certain kinds of acts, such as making
Speech act was a concept initiated by Austin (1995). Austin highlighted the
concept that speech acts can be categorized into two dimensions, both of which bear
truth conditions (Austin, 1955 cited in Allan, 2001:197). Austin (1962, 1963) claims
that there is a distinction between the constatives, which have truth values, and
act is performative if the conditions surrounding the speech act is felicitious. For
example:
I apologise.
(Austin, 1963:22)
Observe the verbs in the examples given and one will understand what is
meant by Austin that is an action is performed when the speech act is uttered under
the right conditions or felicitious conditions (Austin, 1963). This reality is the
opposite of constatives where the speech act uttered only bears true or false (Austin,
1963).
47
Searle then extends Austins theory by saying that whatever can be meant
can be said (1969:19) and this is known also as the principle of experessibility
(1968:415). Searle continues to say that when an utterance is made, the speaker
bears three distinct kinds of acts and they are (a) the uttering of words (morphemes,
sentences); (b) referring and predicating; (c) stating, commanding, promising, etc
(1969:23). In addition, the utterance becomes meaningful too when the sentence is
What meaningful components mean here is that the deeper layers of a sentence such
as deep syntactic structure and the stress and contour of the intonation bear the overall
meaning of the sentence (Searle, 1968). Hence, a crucial element of the speech act
content dimension (Austin, 1962: Searle, 1969, 1979 cited in Holtgraves, 2005)
2.8 Conclusion
Based on the discussion of Brown and Levinsons theory and previous studies
conducted on the Malay language, it can be concluded that there is still ample room
for investigations into the Malay language, specifically the Malay dialects. The next
48
CHAPTER THREE
RESEARCH METHODOLOGY
3.0 Introduction
This chapter provides a description of the theoretical framework that was used
to investigate the politeness phenomena between speakers of two Malay dialects for
this study. It also describes the participants involved and the instruments used to
The theoretical framework used in this study is Brown and Levinsons (1984)
politeness theory. Brown and Levinsons theory has been applied in many local
politeness studies. The current research aims to see if the theory is applicable in the
Brown and Levinson (1987) proposed that when speakers are engaged in a
conversation there is a tendency for people to cooperate (and assume each others
mutual vulnerability of face (p.61) as face is the public self image that every
member wants to claim for himself (Brown and Levinson, 1987: 61). In relation to
the current study, Brown and Levinsons theory is used to investigate the politeness
phenomena in the context of inter-dialects that is the Standard Malay and Kelantanese
dialects. Other studies on the Malay language have incorporated Brown and
49
Levinsons theory. The current study differs from those in that it intends to
investigate how and what politeness strategies are used when speakers of two Malay
speakers repair or compensate in some way the threat to positive and negative public
Brown and Levinson (1978) have presented a framework for how face-threatening
acts are performed. Before interlocutors decide on utilizing any of the strategies, the
act of threatening ones face is acted upon. When this act has been performed, it can
(Maginnis, 2011). When one chooses to respond with redress, one will cater to the
others positive and negative face by means of positive politeness (Maginnis, 2011).
Responding by going bald on, on the other hand, is being intentionally face-
threatening (Brown and Levinson, 1978). According to Maginnis (2011), people opt
to go bald on because they prioritize efficiency over face saving actions and will state
the comment explicitly (p.17). However, though the comment is terse and perhaps in
there will be times when judgements are trivial and bald on comments are welcomed.
As Brown and Levinson (1978, 1978) say, there will be times when comments are
made bald on record and there will be people who may not like what they hear, but
(Shigemitsu, 2003: 32). Redressing to the hearers positive face is done by letting
ones own desires be known but at the same time acknowledging the other persons
desires too by satisfying ones desire and making it similar to the other persons
Negative politeness occurs when there is an attack on the negative face and it
is often accompanied with an apology (Maginnis, 2011:18). The reason behind the
use of apologies when in a negative face threat is because negative politeness deals
speaker to maintain his/her face and avoid conflicts by distancing the speaker from
Fialova, 2010:19). The apology is also to show that one is aware of the other persons
If one goes off record, then one does a face-threatening act that is worded in a
way that does not hold a person accountable (Brown and Levinson, 1978). Hinting or
joking sarcastically are some of the ways to go off record. One can also convey ones
(Shigemitsu, 2003). One of the reasons for going off record is to avoid a
(Maginnis, 2011:17).
51
The current study will look into the said politeness strategies between speakers
of two Malay dialects and will identify what politeness strategies were used alongside
A face threatening act is the opposite of a face saving act. If a face threatening
act involves saying something that will damage another persons self image, then a
face saving act is an act that is the direct opposite (Yule, 2008). Speakers are entitled
to select whichever way they choose to respond, bearing in mind their intention of
wanting to make the other person feel bad or not feel bad about himself or herself. An
example to show the difference between a face threatening act and a face saving act is
as follows:
A neighbor is playing music very loudly. One can choose to respond to the
(Yule, 2008:61)
In the first example given, that is Im going to tell him to stop that awful
noise right now!, the speaker is indifferent towards the hearers (the neighbor) face.
This act is then classified as a face threatening act. This is a face threatening act that
goes bald on record. A face threatening act that goes bald on displays the speakers
On the other hand, the second example; perhaps you could just ask him if he
is going to stop soon because its getting a bit late and people need to get to sleep is
an example that shows the speakers intention to address the hearers negative face.
In contrast to the previous example, the negative face is the notion of imposing on
the hearer, and intruding on their space (Lean, 2008:67). Addressing a persons
negative face is being aware of the other persons personal space (Lean, 2008). Thus,
using conditions such as the adverb perhaps, the modal could and the indirect
sentence form if he is going to stop soon will help to minimize the demand of the
request and show deference towards the hearer on the speakers part.
When one employs an FTA (face threatening act), one has the choice to
This is because as speakers, we want to show the hearer that we respect the hearers
need to be excluded from any person or group so as to be free from any imposition
(Cutting, 2008) At the same time, one can also redress the threat with positive
politeness. Positive politeness is different from negative politeness because when one
redresses with positive politeness, one has a desire to belong and be included with the
3.2 Participants
The participants used for this study were six female students who were
data collection, the participants were all aged 19. The participants for this study were
former students of the researcher and had all volunteered to participate in the study.
53
Out of six of the participants, two participants were from Kelantan. These
participants have spoken the Kelantanese dialect all their lives. One of the participants
resides in Kota Bharu and the other resides in Gua Musang. Adi Yasran (2005) writes
that although Kota Bharu and Gua Musang share the same parent dialect, that is the
Kelantanese dialect, some variations of the sub dialects in both districts do exist.
Some of the differences are based on the manners of articulation for certain phonemes
when uttering certain words (Asmah, 1988). The difference that exists lies in the
accent. Accent refers to the way in which a speaker pronounces, and therefore refers
(Asmah Omar, 1980:5). The participant from Gua Musang had a thicker Kelantanese
accent and the participant from Kota Bharu had a weaker Kelantanese accent.The
other four participants came from Putrajaya, Johor, Pahang and Kuala Lumpur and the
dialect they speak at home and when with family and friends is Standard Malay.
The Kelantanese dialect spoken by someone who comes from Kota Bharu is
Malay is more obvious because more external forces such as social media, formal
education, migration between states and many other factors affect the dialect in Kota
Bharu which is the states capital city. Consequently, the Kelantanese dialect spoken
by individuals who come from districts further away from the state capital would have
a thicker accent and may cause some difficulty for other speakers to comprehend due
to the differences in the articulation of certain words, intonation and word choice.
The participants from Kelantan use the dialect at home when conversing with
family members. They also converse in the Kelantanese dialect when in school with
54
their teachers and friends with a few exceptions to subjects such as English. When in
their hometown, these participants would also use the Kelantanese dialect when going
about their daily routine such as when shopping or requesting for information. When
in campus, these participants are also inclined to converse in the Kelantanese dialect
when they are among peers who are from the same state.
3.3 Procedure
Once all six volunteers had been identified, the researcher met the participants
personally to explain the purpose of their involvement and also the nature of the
current study. The researcher then instructed the participants to schedule time slots to
The presence of the researcher during data collection has implications on the
data collection procedure. In a study such as this where data is collected via
recording, subjects need to be encouraged to be natural and not watch what they say.
The presence of the researcher could possibly make the subjects feel self-conscious
and result in their editing their thoughts and verbal output. To remove apprehension
and allow the subjects to feel as comfortable and behave as normally as possible
during the recording, it was decided that the researcher would not be present during
the recordings. This was made known to the subjects. To ensure that the recording
process was clearly understood, the researcher explained the steps involved and how
55
For the first recording, no specific tasks were given to the subjects. The
engage in such as chatting or gossiping. For the first recording, the researcher wanted
the data to be more reflective of their personalities and preferences. The researcher
also wanted the students to feel comfortable while they carried out the recording.
After listening and deliberating over the first recording, the researcher decided
task was assigned. Therefore, for the subsequent recordings, the researcher assigned a
focus for the conversations. The researcher wanted to also see if there would be
involved deciding on the location, the setting up of a committee, the selection of the
committee members and suggesting types of activities for the event. The third
recording was also an assigned task. The participants had to come up with an event or
The researcher was mindful that the participants should feel at ease when
doing this recording as recording sessions can be time consuming and students are
already occupied with their studies and academic matters. As all the subjects were
staying in the students hostel, it was decided that all the recordings would be done in
one of their rooms. The details of the recording are summarized in the following
table.
56
One of the participants was responsible for the recording for each session.
This participant was instructed by the researcher to place the recorder where there
would be no obstruction so that the recording would be clear. The recorder was
noticeable to the other participants. The participant in charge of the recorder also
timed the conversation. After each recording session, the recorder was returned to the
researcher who then downloaded the data into her computer. The researcher then
listened to the recording to assess the quality of the recordings and transcribe the
conversation.
As can be seen from Table 3.1 above, the number of participants involved in
each of the recording varies. The number of participants present at each recording
was subject to the availability of the participants. The length of each recording also
varies. The first recording was close to an hour long. However, the recording
revealed that the conversation lacked focus. Hence, the researcher decided to set a
shorter time limit for the next recordings. The two later recordings were around thirty
minutes long. Both these shorter recordings had more focus as tasks were given.
57
To answer the first research question which is What are the politeness
strategies employed by the speakers? the researcher analyzed the transcribed data
and then categorized the utterances of the participants based on speech acts. These
speech acts were then categorized under the relevant sub strategies for the politeness
frequency counts to justify the preferred politeness strategies and sub strategies
the framework used for analysis in this study which is derived from Brown and
positive face. Positive face refers to a persons need to feel included and is about
forming relationships or solidifying current ones. The sub strategies within the
positive politeness strategy intend to make the speaker (S) and the hearer (H) feel
58
Intensify interest to H
Seek agreement
Be optimistic
Avoid disagreement
jokes
Assert or presuppose Ss knowledge of and concern
for Hs wants.
Offer, promise.
shows the listener that one notices or approves of him or her. For example:
When one employs this strategy, aspects of prosody such as intonation and
stress are exaggerated. One would also intensify modifiers. For example:
(Akbari, 2002:2)
3.4.1.3 Intensify interest to H
To communicate with another person would also require one to intensify the
Hearers interest in a particular issue at hand. The Speaker can engage the Hearer by
making a good story as he pulls the listening right into the middle of the events
(Akbari, 2002:2)
60
interlocutors. French and Arabic are among the languages that have honorific
for informally addressing another person and also used by those who do not have
much social distance from each other (Akbari, 2002:3). The pronoun vous, on the
other hand, reflects social distance between interlocutors especially if the other
Other ways of signalling in-group identity markers are by using generic names
and terms of address such as dear, love or honey (Brown and Levinson, 1987:107).
In the context of Malaysia, similar terms of address are such as kakak, sayang, and
adik. Brown and Levinson (1987) provide the following example: Help me with
speech that interlocutors utilize. Akbari (2002) writes that to show solidarity with his
relatives, a man may speak in Bakhtiari. Meanwhile, the language used with his wife
would be Persian.
speakers to share associations and attitudes that he and the Hearer may have towards
61
In the case of contraction and ellipsis, both the speaker and the hearer share
mutual knowledge that makes the contraction and ellipsis intelligible to both of them
and not necessarily to others. In the local context, the word bodoh which means
stupid or foolish can be derogatory. However, the contracted form doh (commonly
spelt dowh or doe) is used regularly in the speech of many young Malaysians and
does not at all carry derogative connotations. This expression can be roughly
fools only. The examples below show that the utterances share the same meaning,
Sejuk la bodoh!
Sejuk la dowh/ doe!
Its cold fools!
(own example)
62
speaker will show agreement with the opponents opinion and show understanding
example:
(Shigemitsu, 2003:33)
Utilizing this sub strategy also requires the speaker to stick to safe topics
which allows S to stress his agreement with H and therefore to satisfys Hs desire to
be right (Brown and Levinson, 1987: 112). This sub strategy also can be manifested
by repeating part or all of what the preceeding speaker has said (Brown and
63
The first way is token agreement. In the instance of trying to agree or appear
Yes, but
(Brown and Levinson, 1987:112)
Another example is:
3.4.1.6.2 Pseudo-agreement
conclusion to a line of reasoning carried out cooperatively with the listener (Brown
White lies are used to save the listeners positive face (Akbari, 2002: 6).
Brown and Levinson (1987) writes that a person is obliged to make a white lie when
he is confronted with a situation where the truth is unfavorable and is best dealt by
64
responding with a comment that makes the other person feel good although the reality
You go to someones house for dinner, but they dont know what you
like. By chance, they have cooked something that you dont like. So
you eat it without much of an appetite. She asks you, Dont you like
my cooking? You answer, Just before I came here, I ate something
that spoiled my appetite.
(Akbari, 2002:6)
Hedging is also made by choosing words at the extremes of the relevant value
system (Akbari, 2002:6). This occurs when one wants to appear polite and extremely
accommodating.
65
initiate the FTA by first engaging in small talk or gossip (Brown and Levinson, 1987).
This is an example:
S: Assalmualaikum Hakim. Sibuk ke? Boleh cakap ke? Kau kat mana?
() Rumah siape tu? Kau buat ape kat situ? ()Kerja kau kat Kapar
dah siap? Semua OK? ohh. Tak, aku nak Tanya kau ni.Kau ada tak tool
bag tu dengan sekarang ni? Aku kene bawa ke KK esok
The above request begins by S asking Hakim (the other person on the line) his
whereabouts and how his work in Kapar is progressing. This is considered small talk
as both interlocutors work in the same company and understand the reference to
Kapar. The issue is the tool bag which S urgently needs to get hold of. However, the
request is initiated with small talk because if Hakim does have the tool bag with him,
it would require S to intrude on Hakims personal time as the conversation took place
on a Friday evening. Hence, to soften the request, S begins the conversation with
small talk.
66
To reduce the social distance between two speakers, Akbari says, the speaker
talks with the listener as if their knowledge about the topic were equal (2002:6). This
A cup of tea in this cold weather hits the spot, doesnt it?
(Akbari, 2002:6)
To show that a speaker knows what the hearer wants, the speaker may
and Levinson, 1987). Most of the time, presupposing other peoples wants and
attitudes is to just show that both the speaker and hearer share the same opinions on
topics, concepts and objects. Interlocutors can sometimes address one another with
the use of terms of address such as honey or love even with strangers to soften the
3.4.1.8 Jokes
Jokes are able to minimize the demand of an FTA as jokes are able to put the
hearer at ease and are usually based on mutual shared background (Akbari, 2002: 7).
For example:
67
How about lending me this old heap of junk? (Hs new Cadillac)
When insisting on a speakers need, the speaker is able to pressure the hearer
I know you cant bear parties, but this will be really good- do come!
I know you have class, but I will only take a minute of your time.
(Akbari, 2002:7)
get cooperation from the hearer if the speaker asserts that whatever the hearer wants,
the speaker wants too. Giving offers or promises regardless of whether or not it gets
fulfilled shows the speakers awareness of the need to satisfy the hearers positive
3.4.1.11 Be optimistic
Being optimistic can help to minimize the size of a face threat. Shigemitsu
included the effect of smiling and laughing in this strategy because it shows that the
speaker does not sound serious (Shigemitsu, 2003:33). The participants involved in
the conversation are able to laugh or scoff at one another despite being attacked.
I cant seem to find your book. Give me a few more days to find it.
(Yamazaki, 2001:24)
A request or any other speech act can sometimes easily be achieved when the
inclusive pronoun we is used. The example below implies the pronoun we.
(Akbari, 2002:8)
Brown and Levinson (1987) say that for a speaker to get what he wants, the
speaker may include the hearer in the activity. This way, the hearer is somehow lead
69
into the reasonableness of the speakers FTA (Brown and Levinson, 1987). The
example below shows the speakers intention of requesting the hearer to bring her out.
The speaker does this by rationalizing to the hearer why today would be a profitable
Apa kata sayang bawa kita jalan jalan hari ni? Boleh tengok movie tu
sekali.
Why not take me out today dear? We could catch that movie you want
to watch.
(own example)
example:
Ill go get the book for you, and you type this part for me.
I lent you my car last week, then lend me your computer for today.
(Akbari, 2002:8)
speaker shows to the hearer that the speaker intends to form a trustworthy
all individuals, giving gifts to the hearer satisfies the hearers (and also the
speakers) positive face that is, the desire to be liked, admired and so on. In Persian,
speakers also include religious beliefs in order to bring about a kind of mental relief
addressees negative face: his want to have his freedom of action unobstructed and his
attention unrestricted (Brown and Levinson, 1987:129). The sub strategies within
71
Be conventionally indirect
Question, hedge
Be pessimistic
Minimize imposition
Give deference
Apologize
Impersonalize S and H
Nominalize
72
Being conventionally indirect is the desire to give the hearer an out by being
2009:27). This can pose an inner conflict for the speaker, hence this conflict is dealt
by giving contextually unambiguous meanings which are different from the literal
Requests are sometimes very clear with only a small indication of respect to
another persons negative face. One way of doing this is by including hedges into the
request. Hedges usually consist of particles, a word, or phrase that modifies the
membership of a predicate or noun phrase in a set (Brown and Levinson, 1987: 145).
For example:
3.4.2.3 Be pessimistic
Being pessimistic draws attention to the speakers doubt about the success of
Excuse me, if you were in class yesterday, could I use your notes?
(Akbari, 2002:10)
One may also minimize the imposition of a request (or any utterance) by
indicating that the weight of the act is not so big, leaving only distance and power as
74
Excuse me, professor, can I take a few minutes of your precious time?
(Akbari, 2002:10)
3.4.2.6 Apologize
on the other persons negative face (Akbari, 2002:11). Conclusively, this can
I know this is a bore to you but Id like to ask you a big favour.
(Simpson, 1995:175)
This sub strategy occurs with speakers dissociating themselves from the FTA
(Simpson, 1995). Most of the time, impersonalizing the FTA can be done by
75
dropping pronouns such as I and you (Brown and Levinson, 1987). An example of
dissociation is:
This strategy aims to distance the speaker and the hearer from the imposition
by not using pronouns (Fialova, 2010). It is no wonder that this strategy is closely
there is a general social rule behind the FTA (Van Der Wijst and Uljin, 1995:329).
Therefore, because of this rule, both speakers are able to dissociate from the FTA
Passengers will please refrain from flushing the toilets on the train.
We dont sit on tables, we sit on chairs, Johnny.
3.4.2.9 Nominalize
According to Van Der Wijst & Uljin (1995), in nominalizing, the actor is
detached from the action. This detachment is also known as a masking mechanism
which inadvertently enhances indirectness (Van Der Wijst and Uljin, 1995). For
example:
(Akbari,2002:12)
76
This strategy is meant to express debt or gratitude to the hearer. On the other
hand, if the need arises, this strategy is also to disclaim any indebtedness to the
(Akbari, 2002:12)
Going off record implies the opposite of responding on record. Going off
questions and so on (Brown and Levinson, 1987). Basically, going off record is
can:
- mention a trigger that serves as a notice to the addressee and that some
said.
77
The following table shows the sub strategies that speakers use to make off
record FTAs.
Give hints
Presuppose
Understate
Overstate
Tautologies
Contradictions
Be ironic
Use metaphors
Be ambiguous
Be vague
Over-generalize
Displace H
78
When giving hints, the speaker is saying something that is not explicitly
relevant and requires some inferencing on the hearers part. Indirect speech acts can
be made by raising the issue of some desired act by stating motives or reasons for
doing it (Akbari, 2002: 13). The following scenario describes the speakers indirect
request to the hearer to pick up his things that were lying on the floor.
with the act required of the listener, either by precedence in the speaker/ listener
(Brown and Levinson, 1987:215. The following scenario describes the strategy.
(Akbari, 2002:13)
79
3.4.3.3 Presuppose
to Brown and Levinson, when the speaker says this, he presupposes that he has done
it before sometime in the week (Brown and Levinson, 1987:217). This then allows
the hearer to look for any relevant information related to the utterance mentioned
previously.
3.4.3.4 Understating
Understatements are FTAs that are made by saying less than is required
(Brown and Levinson. 1987:217). Participants are required to share the same
3.4.3.5 Overstate
80
a point of scale that is higher than the usual state of affairs (Brown and Levinson,
Levinson, 1987: 220). The hearer is expected to define the non-informative utterance.
For example:
A: War is war.
(Brown and Levinson, 1987: 220)
3.4.3.7 Contradictions
When making FTAs based on contradictions, the speaker intends to make the
truth or the actual information hazy. Therefore, it is up to the hearer to look for an
3.4.3.8 Be ironic
The sub strategy be ironic means saying the opposite of what [one] means
(Brown and Levinson, 1987: 221). The condition for the irony to be successfully
conveyed is that there are clues that the intended meaning is being conveyed
indirectly (Brown and Levinson, 1987: 221). The clues can be prosodic, kinesic or
Some metaphors are made with hedging particles (sort of) and euphemisms
Rhetorical questions are made with no intention of getting a response from the
other interlocutors. What the speaker wants is for the hearer to provide him the
82
information. Most of the time the questions can be uttered and left in the air (Brown
3.4.3.11 Be ambiguous
The example given by Brown and Levinson shows that not always a clear
connotation can be made for ambigious FTAs (Brown and Levinson, 1987:225).
3.4.3.12 Be vague
intention of being vague is to not be too forthright especially when criticizing (Brown
83
Or in a euphemism:
3.4.3.13 Over-generalize
Over-generalizing leaves the hearer to decide whether or not the rule applies
to him. Over-generalizing can be made given in rule instantiation and the use of
proverbs (Brown and Levinson, 1987). When this sub strategy is utilized, the degree
3.4.3.14 Displace H
Displacing the hearer gives the option for the hearer to choose to react on the
FTA (Brown and Levinson, 1987). The example given by Ervin-Tripp (1972:247)
exemplifies this sub strategy: A secretary asks another to pass the stapler. The
84
exactly obliged to respond to the request. Hence, if the FTA was or was not
successful, nobodys face was threatened nonetheless (Brown and Levinson, 1987).
This sub strategy allows the speaker to leave the implicature hanging in the
Bald on record is an FTA done in the most direct, clear, unambiguous and
concise way possible (Brown and Levinson, 1987:69). Utterances made bald on
85
dont do the FTA is labeled as silence (Fukushima, 2002: 43). The speaker can
choose to perform or not perform the speech act (Bonikowska, 1988 cited in
Fukushima, 2002:43). Hence, dont do the FTA is considered as the most polite
To identify the politeness sub strategies, the researcher identified speech acts
found on the transcriptions. Speech acts are the utterance of sounds or the making of
marks (Searle, 1965:143). These mere utterances of sounds or making of marks bear
meaning and are significant because they allow an utterance to transfer meanings
speech act is in an act of marriage where a priest or any religious representative that
has been vested by law can legalize a marriage by uttering, I hereby pronounce you
man and wife (Finch, 2003: 160). This union is also sealed when the man and
After identifying all the speech acts, the researcher transferred the speech acts
onto tables and counted the occurrences of any of the politeness sub strategies found
in the data. An example of the table designed for the data analysis is provided.
86
Table 3.9: Example of table used to record occurrences and sub strategies
GivinggiftstoH
AttendingtoH
Usingingroup
Includingboth
Giving(orask)
Exaggeration
interesttoH
Offeringand
Assumingor
Intensifying
speechacts
reciprocity
SandHin
promising
asserting
Totalper
Noticing,
markers
reasons
identity
activity
joking
Totease
Torequest
Toapologize
Tocompliment
Toadvice
Totalpersub
strategies
After tabulating the occurrences, the researcher added up all the occurrences
and transfered the data onto bar graphs for further analysis and interpretation.
A brief description of the speech acts found in the data are as follows:
87
contain the basic condition, that is the speaker should lack some information
(pp.2029).
negative act for which the speaker is responsible (Holtgraves, 2005: 2029).
To tease: teasing are mockful but playful jibes against someone (Drew,
1987:219)
To request: Leech and Svartvik (1975) define request as to ask your hearer
were available.
88
3.6 Conclusion
This chapter has provided the theoretical background for the current study.
The method of obtaining data and analyzing data is also discussed in detail. The next
89
CHAPTER FOUR
DATA ANALYSIS
4.0 Introduction
This chapter provides the analysis obtained from the data collected. The study
aims to identify the politeness strategies employed by the participants who spoke
either the Standard Malay dialect or the Kelantanese Dialect. The data collected
Malay dialects?
speech acts found in the recording. The strategies were then calculated based on the
frequencies of occurrence of the politeness strategies and the data was converted into
percentages.
Each section will discuss the strategies and sub strategies (refer to Chapter 3)
with data from the recordings and summarize the findings based on the strategies
employed. The FTAs that were found to be manifested in a particular sub strategy will
90
bald on record and off record) have been drawn up. The tables outline the speech acts
toendconversation
Speechacts/
tocompliment
substrategies
Totalpersub
toapologize
tocomplain
torequest
tosuggest
tocorrect
toadvice
toclarify
toagree
strategy
totease
toask
Givinggiftsto 1 1
H
Assumingor
asserting
reciprocity
Giving(orask) 1 1 3 5
reasons
Includingboth 2 1 1 14 18
SandHin
activity
Offeringand 1 1
promising
Joking 6 1 7
Being 1 1
Optimistic
Presupposition/
Raise/assert
Commonground
Avoiding
disagreement
Seeking 1 1 3 1 6
agreement
Usingingroup 4 2 3 1 10
identitymarkers
Intensifying 1 1 1 3
interesttoH
Exaggeration 1 1 1 3
Noticing, 2 1 1 4
AttendingtoH
Totalper
T 11 2 2 5 3 3 7 1 20 3 1 1 59
speechacts
91
Table 4.1 shows the speech acts that utilized the positive politeness (PP)
strategy. It shows that the sub strategy Including S and H activity (18) and Using
identity markers (9) obtained the highest number of occurrences. It can also be seen
that the speech act that utilized the most types of PP sub strategies is to give
suggestions and the sub strategies that only utilized one PP sub strategy are to end
conversation and to ask. This finding is rather significant because the number of
occurrences of each speech act throws light on the dynamics of the relationship
As can be seen, PP sub strategies were favored among the participants when it
positive politeness as defined by Brown and Levinson (1987) that is, redress directed
partially satisfying that desire by communicating that ones wants are in some
respects similar to the addressees wants (Brown and Levinson, 1987:101). Based on
the recordings, the participants had discussed about having a reunion and starting a
small business. Naturally, the discussion would revolve around giving suggestions.
The participants were found to be supportive of one another as the chosen strategy is
the positive politeness strategy. The examples below show how each sub strategy
Being optimistic:
Sample 4.1
D: I think itd be fun if we mix. You can check out for any
potential partners.
B: [sounds good]
Sample 4.2
D: semuo ah. Jap jap. Set set tino ke set set jatey? Ke nok
campur? Campur sedap sikit!
D: all together. Hang on. Just girls or boys? Or do you want to
mix?Mixed sounds better!
Sample 4.3
Sample 4.4
B: dah kito nok buat gapo wei? Hah! Kita buat gotu nok?
A: apa?
B: Kite buat bean porridge
B: so what else can we do? Hah! Why dont we do this?
A: what?
B: we sell bubur kachey (kacang)
including everyone in the decision. This can be seen with the use of the pronouns
suggestions with any PP strategies, one is able to put forth his/her ideas without
excluding the others which indirectly shows that one is concerned about the other
persons feelings and wants, and above all, insists on maintaining solidarity.
The next sub strategy noticing the aspects of the H can be seen in one of the
Sample 4.5
quality of D that is she is hard working and would not have any trouble studying if
she was at home, unlike the other participants. This highlighted quality addresses Ds
positive face as it makes her (although, in reality, it is the contrary) feel good about
herself.
Next, there were two occurrences for the sub strategy offering and
promising is:
Sample 4.6
94
C: I forgot. You guys are getting married later eh. Sorryy. Next
time, if you guys are getting married, come look for me ok?
Here, C apologizes for her carelessness in talking about private matters related
to husband and wife relationships to her friends. (C is about 19 years old and was to
be married soon at the time of recording). She apologizes by offering marital tips to
The data for the speech act to tease had mostly come under the joking sub
strategy. Having a dual nature, teasing can be face saving as it promotes bonding and
friendliness but it also has an opposite effect that is, teasing can be face threatening
too (Lampert and Ervin-Tripp, 2006). Joking allows the teasing to be considered as
Sample 4.7
prejudice towards the dialect. However, this is not applicable to participants A and C
who are from Kelantan and B who speak Standard Malay. Instead, what the tease
95
Sample 4.8
attractive quality (sweet) she possess being a Kelantanese. Bs responding this way
An example from the data of to tease that employed using in-group identity
markers is as follows:
Sample 4.9
C: eee how fun. SPICE program. I went to the SPICE program the
other day. There was this guy who tried to approach me. god
he was flirting with me. I just ignored him. But kept on ignoring
him
A: [fuyoooo Amazing]
96
The expression fuyooo syok molek is directed at Cs positive face that is,
her perennial desire that her wants should be thought of as desirable (Brown and
Levinson, 1987: 101). This expression is shared among the participants where
thejeering of fuyoo syok molek is not meant to humiliate C but is only meant to
show As appreciation for the so-called extraordinary event that had happened to C.
The next example shows how A clarifies to B what Mek means. Initially, A
had said something and called B Mek. B was unsure of what Mek means and
and H in the activity to sort of pacify B who is slightly unamused at being addressed
as Mek.
Sample 4.10
97
Goingonrecordandincurring
Beingconventionallyindirect
adebt,orasnotindebtingH
Speechact/substrategy
StatingtheFTAasageneral
Minimizingtheimposition
ImpersonalizingSandH
Totalperspeechacts
Questioning,Hedge
Beingpessimistic
Givingdeference
Nominalization
Apologizing
rule
Toask 1 1 2
Toadvice 5 5
Todisagree 1 4 3 8
Tosuggest 1 1 1 2 5
To 2 1 1 4
complain
Todecline 5 5
Toclarify 1 1
Totease 1 1
Totalper 5 1 10 1 2 12 31
sub
strategies
8 speech acts were found to utilize the negative politeness (NP) strategy. In
Table 4.2 above, it can be seen that a total of 12 occurrences were found to employ
the stating the FTA as a general rule and this is the most favored NP sub strategy.
Stating the FTA as a general rule is followed by the sub strategy being pessimistic
with a total number of 10 occurrences. The other sub strategies only managed to
98
The sub strategies that were not favored are probably because of the very
nature of the sub strategies themselves. Firstly, the exchanges between the
participants did not require the participants to commit FTAs by means of giving
oneself alongside carrying out the FTA. In the example cited from Simpson
(1995), it can be seen that for the participants involved in the present study to make a
(Simpson, 1995:175)
However, the next example would seem more fitting if the context was directed to
Excuse me, professor, can I take a few minutes of your precious time?
(Akbari, 2002:10)
The same justification applies to why the sub strategy apologizing was not
favored by the participants. The participants have no need to coat their FTAs by being
they share is mutual enough to allow the participants to commit FTAs without
negative redress.
and incurring a debt, or not indebting H were not favored because the participants
did not need to detach themselves from the FTA. Additionally, the participants were
99
not obliged to redress their FTAs with expressions of gratitude or even to express debt
to the other speaker. These would be more applicable if it was made to a stranger
such as when one desperately needs to make an emergency phone call but ones
mobile phone is either disconnected or missing. Ones request may be made in this
way: Excuse me, Miss, I lost my hand phone. Could you lend me your phone? I
would really appreciate it. Thank you so much. May God bless you
Based on Table 4.2, it can be seen that one of the most favored NP sub
negative face by explicitly expressing doubt that the conditions for the
appropriateness of Ss speech act obtain (Brown and Levinson, 1987: 173). Five of
the speech acts had employed this sub strategy which shows that the participants were
aware of the other participants need of space. Although the relationship between the
participants is mutual and there is no immediate need to redress FTAs with NP or PP,
there are times when the participants realize that the negative face of others had to be
respected. The next example shows this when a participant wanted to decline being
Sample 4.11
D declines the offer to become the PIC despite the other participants
insistence. However, she does not decline baldly. Instead, she tries to deflect the
100
other participants insistence while protecting her space by describing the worst
possible scenario that will occur if she were to become the PIC. The following is
another example of the sub strategy being pessimistic used to commit the
FTA to disagree.
Sample 4.12
this response implies that A does not agree with the suggestion. A disagrees by
questioning B as to whether or not the security guards and the hostel wardens would
allow students to be out of the centers grounds at such an hour. This rationalization
is a pessimistic one because it curtails Bs effort to sell their wares, thus not opening
being pessimistic.
Sample 4.13
B had not stated her disagreement directly or bald on record such as by saying
no we dont need to sell drinks. Instead, she conjures a condition that makes the idea
101
of selling drinks as troublesome and unprofitable for the both of them. This can be
seen in the expression buat gapo (why for?: literal translation) that some how
Another speech act that was manifested through the NP sub strategy of being
pessimistic is to complain. In the example from the data given below, the
participants were about to discuss the activities for the reunion. D complains about
the proposal:
Sample 4. 14
pessimistic. She intends to have the other participants agree with her but does it in a
way that does not seem too bold. The reason for this is probably because D is aware
that games are popular during reunions but does not want to really show her
disagreement on it.
H an out by being indirect and the desire to go on record (Brown and Levinson,
1987:132).
102
Sample 4.15
The above example is extracted from the context of A and B talking about not
receiving an allowance during the short semester. They talk about how they have to
restrict certain food choices due to their limited budget. However, B (the other
participant) mentioned that she had indulged herself by eating in a particular cafeteria
teasing B as seen in the extract above. The intention of A was to go on record about
Bs ability to afford expensive meals. On the other hand, A did not want to sound too
of jealousy (if there was such an inclination). Thus, the teasing is made
conventionally indirect that is, between a bald on record (tapi makan mewah kot!
but you eat so lavishly!) and coating it with an element of indirectness (duit negatif
so little cash).
Sample 4.16
103
disagrees with Ds choice of movie but cares for Bs negative face by not directly
saying no to her which is in fact terse and maximally efficient (Brown and
Levinson, 1987: 95). Her response of saying hah in the earlier parts of the extract
affirms that she acknowledges Bs suggestion but at the end of the extract, B
rationalizes her disagreement by saying that the selected movie was funny and a
current hit but it would not be so in five years time (the time of the reunion) when
there would be even better movies released. Thus, B maintains Ds negative face by
agreeing that the chosen movie is a good choice but at the same time implying a no
The other NP sub strategy that is most favored is stating the FTA as a general
rule. Stating the FTA as a general rule is to almost completely remove ones self
from the imposition (Brown and Levinson, 1987). When an FTA is structured in the
imperative form, the FTA becomes some general social rule, regulation, or
obligation (Brown and Levinson, 1987: 206), leaving one minimal choice but to
adhere to it. The following examples from the data show how this sub strategy was
employed:
To clarify:
104
Sample 4.17
The other participants in the recording had asked what gelenya meant. After
gelenya as flirty. The response provided clarification and was not directed as an
attack but is a statement that requires the other participants to adhere to the meaning.
This had saved Ds negative face as it clarified the meaning and this had also saved
the other participants face by not making them feel that the correct meaning for the
The next example shows how a participant disagrees by stating the FTA as a
general rule.
Sample 4.18
A: oooh. Eh! kenapa kau panggil aku mek? Aku mude ah cheehh
((laugh))
B: alaa mu ni rilek ah bahase kelatey key panggil mek panggil
timoh
A: oooh. Eh! why do you call me mek? I am still young
ah..Cheehh ((laugh))
B: alaa come on relax its kelantanese call mek call timoh
mude ah (I am still young ah). This claim is a general rule and that basically sums up
her attitude towards the idea of being addressed as mek. A did not have any
105
intention to embarrass the other participant. A had maintained the other participants
negative face by laughing at the end of the utterance. Nonetheless, she asserts her
disagreement by implying that mek is reserved for older ladies, and she is still too
FTA as a general rule. The exchange between the participants was about making
estimations on the profit the participants would get from their small business.
Sample 4.19
The general idea is that some simple calculations could be done effortlessly.
However, B had become a little bit confused with some simple calculations which
was why she had exclaimed as such in the extract. A disagreed with Bs exclamation
on the difficulty of the calculations by asserting that eh math biase je! Matematik
moden kot! (eh its just simple math. Modern mathematics in fact!) and this is an
FTA uttered as a general rule. Thus, Bs negative face is maintained with a mere
The next example is an advice made in the state the FTA as a general rule
form.
Sample 4.20
commuter train coach for both male and female passengers. Unfortunately, this
particular participant had been harassed. A then responded by advising her as shown
in the extract above. To respect this particular participants negative face, when
expressing her concern, A made this advice general as if she was also addressing the
rest of the participants. Thus, the negative face of this particular participant is
maintained (that it is her decision to board any coach but at the same time, she should
make sure she boards the coach for women only for her own sake) because this advice
The next advice made as state the FTA as a general rule is a continuation of
the previous extract. After dwelling on the experience of being harassed inside the
Sample 4. 21
Here, C has shared her experience and thus wants to advise her friends to not
make the same mistake again. Because the advice applies to all, C would naturally
manifest this advice by using this sub strategy. To follow or not to follow this advice
107
The next two examples from the data show the sub strategy state the FTA as a
general rule.
Sample 4.22
B: tapi kalau kalau kau nak balik bawa balik dengan orang
lain la. sorang sorang bahaya kot
A/D: mmm
B: ye ah sebab macam macam kalau nak kat KL ni. Hah. Nak
drive sorang sorang ni pun bahaya
Sample 4.23
So far, four examples of giving advice made by stating the FTA as a general
rule have been shown. It can be concluded that the participants involved in this study
had chosen this sub strategy as a means to give advice to one another. The two
examples given previously may show that B could have addressed the advice to
somebody specific. But clearly this is not the intention. The intention is definitely to
advice the other participants as well. Although the pronoun kau (you) was used, it is
also meant to be directed at the other participants as well. Drawing in parallels like
the reality of driving in KL helps minimize the prescriptiveness of the advice, thus
allowing the other participants to make their own conclusions on the above matter.
108
The sub strategy Impersonalizing S and H can be seen in the speech act to
Sample 4.24
D employed the sub strategy impersonalizing S and H to ask about the eating
habits of the Kelantanese. Since there is no relation to the participants from Kelantan,
it is natural for the speakers to disregard any association of the question to the
109
Table 4.3 Total occurrences of off record sub strategies based on speech acts
Beingincomplete:usingellipsis
Usingrhetoricalquestions
GivingAssociationclues
Totalperspeechacts
Usingcontradictions
Overgeneralization
UsingTautologies
Beingambiguous
Usingmetaphors
Presupposition
Understating
DisplacingH
Beingvague
GivingHints
Beingironic
Overstating
Totease 1 1 2
Toend 6 1 7
conversation
Todisagree 1 1 2
Tocomplain 4 1 5
Torequest 1 1
Totalper 12 1 1 1 2 17
sub
strategies
Giving hints (12 occurrences) was the most favored off record sub strategy
of the participants. Giving association clues, using metaphors and being vague
recorded only one occurrence compared to the other sub strategies. This makes these
sub strategies the least utilized. There is a rather significant imbalance in the use of
sub strategies, where giving hints is clearly the more favored one. To end the
conversation is seen to utilize the most off record sub strategy that is, giving hints.
Off record FTAs allow the speaker an out. What an out means is the
speaker will not be held accountable for whatever it is he says (Brown and Levinson,
1987). Giving hints means to raise the issue of some desired act (Brown and
110
Levinson, 1987: 215). The following example shows how D hints to the other
participants that she is unhappy with the suggestion of sleeping in tents during the
reunion. The desired act is to disagree with the suggestion. However, D invites the
hearer to search for an interpretation of the possible relevance (Brown and Levinson,
Sample 4.25
C: khemoh
D: siye ko anok aku. Tidor daley khemoh!
C: tent
D: poor baby. Sleeping in a tent!
D gave hints (about her unborn childs discomfort) when complaining instead
of being overt about her disinclination towards camping and in this way, she is not
In the following examples, the participants employ the give hints sub
Sample 4.26
In fact, D has no obligation to return home and fix dinner as she was actually
not married yet at the time of recording and she does not have a proper kitchen to
111
cook dinner. Her exclamation is really to provide a leeway for her to leave the
conversation. She may have grabbed the attention of the other participants but what
she has done is really expecting the other participants to comply with her desire to end
the conversation. D has left inferences for the other participants to interpret by hinting
The next example from the data also shows ways to end conversation by
Sample 4.27
Sample 4.28
A: ngantuknyee
B: wei! Janganlah! ((Laugh)) jangan la tidor lagi Syikin
A: Im sleepy
B: wei! Dont! ((laugh)) dont fall asleep yet Syikin
The above examples clearly spell what Brown and Levinson describe as a
communicative act done in such a way that it is not possible to attribute only one clear
communicative intention to the act (Brown and Levinson, 1987: 211). One can
interpret many things, such as A wanting to express how tired she was or just a mere
exclamation of emotions. But in the extracts given, A repeatedly gives hints about
ending the conversation by expressing how sleepy she is. In fact, her being sleepy has
intention of wanting to end the conversation so that she can take a nap.
112
The sub strategy give association clues is also found in the speech act to end
conversation. What is slightly different between give association clues and giving
hints is that when making association clues, both interlocutors involved in the
the communicative act (Brown and Levinson, 1987:215). One example found in the
Sample 4.29
D had exclaimed boh ah puji aku! letih doh! (Enough with the compliments!
Im tired!) which signaled to the other participants that D did not want the other
discussion. This implies that D wants to quickly end the conversation. How her
participants have finalized their discussion on the reunion and realize Ds potential as
a leader. Because they have run out of points to discuss, the participants immediately
113
The sub strategy use metaphors had been manifested through the speech act
to tease. Using metaphors can be marked with euphemism and hedging particles
Sample 4.30
C: number. Aku pun letok. Die talipon duo tigo kali. Aku tok
angkat angkat last skali die pun senyap gitu. Aku ke baguh dok
laye oghee ((laugh))
B: sempat lagi angkat bakul
C: number. I hung up. He called again two or three times. I didnt
pick up and finally the phone calls just stopped. I am a good
girl arent I not entertaining people of that sort ((laugh))
B: manages to lift her own basket (literal translation)
Manages to blow her own horn (metaphorical translation)
Observing Bs response, the association of the basket (or horn) is non existent
to the conversation that took place. This expression is metaphorical and allows a sense
of detachment for B if ever the tease threatens Cs face. However, the act of teasing
is actually intentional and one which Brown and Levinson would deem as made bald
on record (Brown and Levinson, 1987). On the other hand, what is made off record
114
The bar graph shows that altogether 23 speech acts were expressed baldly on
record. Based on the table, the speech act to complain was made 63 times. The other
two speech acts that were significant are to disagree and to tease with occurrences
of 39 and 30 respectively. The speech act that was least used and obtained only one
occurrence is to encourage.
Interestingly, the data shows that speech acts that were theoretically face
occurrences compared to other speech acts that were more gentle such as to
115
apologize, to suggest and a few others. This suggests that an FTA such as
ODriscoll (1997). As ODriscoll states, the claim that some acts threaten face is self
more face saving strategy can be employed by the speakers such as negative
least soften the complaint. However, as the analysis shows, this claim does not
always hold true. The participants in this study had made complaints baldly yet did
not offend anyone. The following data to illustrate this will be shown in both
Standard Malay and the Kelantanese dialect and is followed by the English
translation.
Sample 4.31
Here, D complains to the other participants that the medical course takes a
long time to complete. This FTA is threatening because two of the other participants
are actually medical students. However, D still went ahead by highlighting this baldly
on record.
116
some of the participants. D does not hesitate to attack the faces of the two
participants by exclaiming eee pening aku kecek dengan mu (eee such a headache
talking to you) and eee mu nih key! eee Ya Allah! Aku gurau ja sat ni! (eee oh you
really! Eee my god! I was just joking just now). The attack is direct and immediate
with the use of the pronoun mu which addresses the other person directly. But
because the participants are well adjusted with one another, there is no risk to the
relationship.
Sample 4.32
Sample 4.33
Sample 4.34
117
so. Ideally, to encourage someone who is learning or attempting something new, one
politeness, that is by saying things to make the other person feel good about himself
pleasant at all in her attempt to speak Kelantanese and in fact, she sounds like she is
speaking Thai. This is a grave attack on the other speaker. However, B does not seem
conversants try to keep their own positions by opposing each other. Keeping ones
own position means asserting ones own opinions over those of others. To avoid
friction, the participants should have employed the positive politeness strategy or the
negative politeness strategy. But, clearly bald on record was no threat at all to the
participants share does not require the participants to coat their disagreements in a
more discreet manner. The camaraderie they share can be seen in the excerpt below
118
Sample 4.35
relationship between the two participants was not close, this interjection would have
resulted in some friction as B had imposed her opinions even before D said anything.
This is a threat to ones negative face. However, D had not been threatened and B did
not intend to belittle Ds response as their relationship was based on mutual grounds
as they have been friends for almost two years and have grown accustomed to one
disagreeing baldly.
Sample 4.36
dialect would improve. If the relationship between the participants had not been close,
119
this bold disagreement could have reflected As negative attitude towards the
Kelantanese people and also the Kelantanese dialect. However, this was not the
intention of A. A disagreed purely out of her disinterest in the idea of looking for a
Next , the data showed that teasing among the participants, something typical
of female speech, was common and was mainly made baldly on record. The teasing
was found to be mocking but playful jibes against someone (Drew, 1987:219).
Figure 4.1 shows that teasing on record was made as many as 34 times. This shows
that the participants are comfortable at mocking playfully at one another and are not
offended even if the teasing was made baldly on record. Some of the examples from
Sample 4.37
The scenario above revolves around the participants A and C sharing their
experience visiting a chocolate fair and trying out some of the chocolates that were
exhibited. They had described the flavors and tastes of the chocolates. C had used
some of the common Kelantanese expressions when describing flavor and D (Khai)
had a go at trying to say those expressions. According to B, Khai should not try
120
saying them because it would obviously sound awkward. This discouragement was
not intentionally made to put D (Khai) down; it was only to tease her.
In the next example, A teases C for (purposely) forgetting about her own
wedding.
Sample 4.38
The above examples show that the participants have no qualms about running
down the other participants by means of teasing. Both examples of teasing can be
likened to criticizing. However, the nature of the exaggerated teasing alongside the
laughter that the participants shared softens the attack and makes it good natured
criticism.
To agree, to apologize, and to interrupt are among the other speech acts
that obtained the least number of occurrence made bald on record. This is also typical
when the participants involved in a particular conversation are very familiar with one
another and there is no need to be nice. Participants who are comfortable with one
another are aware of the dynamics involved when they converse with each other.
121
They know what topics or issues can be discussed and how to go about discussing
them. Hence, the occurrences of the said speech acts are minimal and also not
To apologize:
Sample 4.39
To agree:
Sample 4.40
implies the meaning of exactly to the claim that afternoons are for napping.
To interrupt:
Sample 4.41
122
To threaten:
Sample 4.42
simple and clear as possible. However, it is undeniable that even though this threat
was made baldly on record, the laughter and the use of the emphatic marker amboi
123
160
142
140
120
100
83
80
60
40
32
27
20 18
13 12
5
0
baldon positivepoliteness negativepoliteness offrecord
Standard
Malay
Kelantanese
Dialect
Figure 4.2 above shows the overall occurrence of the politeness strategy in the
two Malay dialects between the participants. Overall, the speakers that come from
Kelantan have collectively utilized more politeness strategies (bald on record, positive
124
Malay. The most widely utilized politeness strategy is the bald on record strategy
and the least is the off record strategy. This applies to both Malay dialects.
The data shows the use of more bald on record strategies by the participants
when conversing with one another with the Kelantanese speakers dominating the
strategy (142). The speakers of Standard Malay account for 83 occurrences of the
strategy. Although the difference is quite large, the numbers show that clearly, the
speakers are comfortable conversing using the bald on record strategy. Some may
say that the bald on record strategy can potentially damage a persons face as it has
no redress. However, this is not the case for the participants involved in this study.
They utilized the strategy as it shows the level of comfort they have with one another,
thus not necessitating the need to coat their utterances or intentions with hedges,
A further indication of the level of their relationship is the fact that the second
most utilized strategy is the positive politeness (PP) strategy. PP is a strategy that
values solidarity and good feelings to oneself and to the other self (Brown and
Levinson, 1987). The to give suggestions and to tease speech acts that were
employed by the speakers reflected this strategy. From the speech acts that utilized
PP, it is possible to describe the type of relationship the participants had. The
participants were sensitive to the type of speech acts that were more suitably
conversations, they were obligated to satisfy the need for ones positive face.
125
frequency for both dialects. This is probably due to the participants cautionary take
on the concept of negative politeness that is, respecting ones personal space and not
imposing opinions on others (Brown and Levinson, 1987). Based on the number of
occurrences, the participants did not really see the need to be too formal with one
another. Based on Figure 4.2 above, the speech acts that employed most of the NP
and disagreeing are speech acts that are threatening to ones face as complaining
and disagreeing suggests that we are in contrary to the other persons opinions or
belief. Moreover, asking, in the context of the recordings, involves the participant
asking about matters that would intrude into personal space, such as asking whether
or not the other participants would be willing to travel to Johor or Kelantan to attend
a wedding reception. Clearly, questions that bear imposition on another persons time,
The off record strategy recorded a marked imbalance between the two
dialects. The Kelantanese speakers had initiated more detachment in their utterances
In conclusion, what can be seen based on Figure 4.2 is that the speakers of the
speakers of Standard Malay. The speakers of the Kelantanese dialect also employed a
more significant number of bald on record utterances. There was some balance in the
occurrences. However, there is a big difference in the employment of the off record
126
strategy between the two Malay dialects. One explanation for this imbalance is
probably that there were more utterances produced by the speakers of the Kelantanese
dialect. This would certainly affect the total number of politeness strategies between
the two Malay dialects. Besides that, it reflects that the speakers from Kelantan had
initiated more discussion and could have also contributed more ideas into the
conversation.
4.6 Conclusion
This chapter shows the results that have been obtained from the analysis.
Generally, the most preferred politeness strategy utilized by the participants are
strategies that reflected the dynamics of their relationship which revolves around
clarity and maintaining solidarity. These values are reflected on their preference for
the politeness strategies going bald on record and positive politeness. The other
two strategies, negative politeness and off record, were used sparingly with great
consideration whenever the FTA that was bound to be committed had a bigger threat
127
CHAPTER FIVE
5.0 Introduction
This chapter is dedicated to answering the research questions that guide this
study. The discussion will summarize and conclude from the findings of this
Research Question 1:
Research Question 2:
utterances baldly during conversations between people who are familiar with each
other. The least favoured strategy is the off record strategy as the nature of the
conversations shows that there is no need for the participants to engage in indirect
speech as they are around people they are familiar with. Additionally, the strategy
Dont do the FTA is not utilised at all because the participants were found to not
completely ignore utterances made by each other. The discussion which follows will
5.1.1 Research Question 1: What are the politeness strategies employed by the
speakers?
The study shows that the participants in the study had employed four of the
politeness strategies proposed by Brown and Levinson (1987). The strategies are
when doing an FTA are to either go off record or go on record with the two
going bald on record. Going bald on record is then provided with two options
The study shows that FTAs that were intrinsically face threatening such as
complaining, disagreeing, and teasing had been employed on more bald on record
strategy than the other three strategies. Bald on record is a strategy that values
maximum efficiency (Brown and Levinson, 1987). Because the relationship between
the participants is already established, the conversations that take place are more
immediate and upfront. This would not be the case if the relationship between the
participants was not mutual. Although the explanation given by Brown and Levinson
that whenever S wants to do the FTA with maximum efficiency more than he wants
to satisfy Hs face, even to any degree, he will choose the bald on record strategy
(1987:95) shows a rather bold move on the speakers part, this is not found to be
applicable in the context of the present study. Brown and Levinson also mention
that face is a universal concept, it follows that each culture values the importance of
face (cited in Srisuruk, 2011:32). Clearly, in the context of the participants who come
from two different regions in Malaysia, regional cultural differences also do not seem
129
to affect the conversations even when the bald on record strategy is employed by
them.
After the bald on record strategy, positive politeness was the second most
favoured strategy used by the participants. The employment of this strategy has
somehow validated the dynamics of the relationship that is shared by the participants.
Positive politeness refers to a persons need to make others feel good as the speaker
himself wants to feel good too. Apart from the differences in the dialects spoken, the
participants provided the assurance that every speaker involved in the conversations
would feel equally important. This is in line with Brown and Levinsons (1987)
means both the speakers are the same (p.70). This can be seen in how the
participants utilize the strategy on certain speech acts such as when teasing and
giving suggestions. What this suggests is the participants know when to be direct or
when to be attentive to the other persons positive face. Not only that, the participants
were found to use the sub strategy include S and H up to 14 times, making it the
most utilized sub strategy. This concretely validates the type of relationship the
participants share because they have deliberately selected this strategy when giving
suggestions. The concern is ultimately in making everyone happy with whatever the
decision has been made because they have included both the speaker and the hearer in
the speech act. This has certainly lessened the impact of imposing ones suggestion on
the others.
employed significantly lower (11 occurrences) than when it is made bald on record
130
(30). This is a rather obvious imbalance in the manifestation of teasing but this
suggests that the participants were aware of the kinds of teasing that can be made
carrying out this FTA had only strengthened the group camaraderie despite bald on
record being committed more than the positive politeness strategy which is all about
The same phenomenon can be seen for the speech act to complain.
politeness (3). Once again, the numbers show the level of comfort the participants
have with one another as the numbers can suggest that the manifestation of
complaints is made baldly with no inhibitions towards the other participants face. On
The negative politeness strategy is a strategy that shows respect to the other
persons awareness that one is not entirely entitled to impose ones opinion on others
especially if the relationship is in its infancy. Even the number of speech acts that
utilized the negative politeness strategy in the data is less than the two previous
strategies.
The most favoured sub strategy of positive politeness is the state the FTA as a
general rule. It may be concluded that this sub strategy was the most favoured
strategy because the participants in the study want to appear unforceful and polite and
participants in the study show a preference for strategies that mark solidarity and
transparency such as the positive politeness strategy and the bald on record
strategy.
their individual opinions onto another individual, the participants would opt for the
negative politeness strategy that gives them the upper hand to be clear (or bald on
record) but at the same time ensure no one gets hurt (or positive politeness) by
transforming the FTA as some general social rule, regulation or obligation (Brown
The study also found that the sub strategy being pessimistic was one of the
two more frequently utilized sub strategies manifested on a few of the speech acts.
expressing doubt (Brown and Levinson, 1987: 173). By explicitly expressing doubt,
the speaker is showing the hearer that he allows the hearer to decline or disregard the
speakers FTA but at the same time, when rejected, the negative face of the speaker
remains (slightly) intact. Being pessimistic can be seen in the use of the negative
(with a tag), the use of the subjunctive, and the use of remote-possibility markers
(Brown and Levinson, 1987: 175). These sub elements of pessimism are then used
together with the strategy Question, Hedge, thus laying the foundation for the
construction of polite indirect speech acts (Brown and Levinson, 1987: 175). Hence,
the participants were careful enough to give advice, show disagreement and decline
132
The least utilized politeness strategy is the off record strategy. The
participants in this study did not resort to this politeness strategy probably because
this strategy theoretically revolves around the idea that off record utterances are
essentially indirect uses of language (Brown and Levinson, 1987: 211). Considering
the relationship that the participants share, it is unlikely for them to make many off
record FTAs as the utterances can potentially jeopardize their relationship and the
rapport they already have. Based on the findings, the participants had a higher
tendency to perform FTAs by utilizing the sub strategy giving hints. Their
awareness of maintaining the face of others can be seen in the lengths the participants
go to in order to prevent as much friction as possible when they try to end the
conversation by giving hints. This sub strategy is the only strategy that was used for
six times and is enough to show that the other participants did not want to openly
express their boredom, their fatigue and restlessness during the recording to the
participant who had initiated the recording session. This would certainly offend one
particular participant as she was given the responsibility to carry out the recording by
the researcher. Therefore, by giving hints, the other participants could avoid the
responsibility for doing it (FTA) and leave it up to the addressee to decide how to
133
social structure (cited in Al-Ahmad and Wardat, 2010:2). This claim can be seen in
The findings show collectively that the most employed politeness strategy is
the bald on record strategy with 225 occurrences. This is followed by the positive
politeness strategy (59), the negative politeness strategy (31) and lastly the off record
strategy (17). Based on the findings from this study, the conclusion that can be made
is relationships where one can be more direct and clear in ones utterances are
relationships where one is closer with the other person. In addition, when a sense of
comfort and familiarity has been established, one would be able to employ other
politeness strategies such as the negative and positive politeness strategies and off
134
Table 5.2 above shows that generally, the participants that come from
Kelantan had performed more politeness strategies compared to the speakers of the
Standard Malay dialect. However, there is only one exception for the negative
politeness strategy as it shows the participants who used Standard Malay had
be due to the fact that most of the talking was done by the Kelantanese speakers.
5.1.3 Conclusion
consolidated solidarity and clarity rather than those that appeared falsely polite and
indirect as there was no need for such pretence. The participants dialectal differences
did not appear to be a hindrance that forced them to communicate superficially and to
appear falsely polite. It is clear that the participants valued clarity because they
went about performing the FTAs with disregard for ambiguity. The researcher
contends that the flow of the conversations found on the recordings showed that the
135
amicably with one another. In addition, the participants also softened some of the
The data also shows that there are certain instances where the participants
consciously employed the politeness strategies that took into account their friends
personal space and feelings. These were the instances where the participants tried
hard to not be so direct as they were consciously aware that it could threaten the
rapport of the conversation and offend the other persons too. The politeness strategies
that were used to address this were the negative politeness and off record strategies.
However, on the whole, the participants maneuvered their conversations around two
very important principles i.e. valuing solidarity and clarity. As such, although they
conversed in two different dialects, they had no trouble maintaining rapport with one
another.
5.2 Recommendations
Future studies can benefit from this study as there are so many areas that
researchers can pursue. One area future research can look into is comparing the
these FTAs. This exploration would not only investigate an area so often taken for
136
granted (performance of FTAs across dialects) , it would also bring Brown and
Next, an aspect that was not taken into consideration in this study and can be
made into an area of research is exploring Malay dialects and the employment of
Brown and Levinsons politeness theory between genders. Clearly, gender studies is a
research area that has been going on for a very long time already. However, research
on Malay dialects is lacking in this aspect especially with regard to the extralinguistic
speak the Malay dialects perform FTAs. Thus, exploring Brown and Levinsons
worthwhile.
Dialects have been available in languages all over the world including
Malaysia ever since language and speech came into existence. This is significant as
particular Malay dialect. Future research can identify whether or not there exists a
different ages make an FTA and redress them with any of Brown and Levinsons
politeness strategies.
Not limited to intergenerational discourse, the Malay dialects and Brown and
Levinsons theory can be elevated further by utilizing the framework on rural and
urban Malay dialects. This of course would require one to first identify what
137
differentiates the two. Nonetheless, this is another worthy area because with rapid
migration from rural areas into the cities (irrespective of age, gender, social
changes (or none) in how the speakers of Malay dialects employ FTAs so as to blend
(or not) into their new surroundings. The politeness strategies can also be investigated
by making comparisons between the rural dialects and urban dialects based on
specific FTAs.
sub strategy in Malay speech. Perhaps future research can look into how the sub
strategy being pessimistic for negative politeness manifests itself and investigate
who uses this sub strategy (or any other sub strategies) more; males or females, the
how politeness strategies are being utilised in these ceremonies. Perhaps there are
differences or similarities.
blogs and social networking websites, and with an increasing number of computer
literate individuals, the Internet is becoming a platform for everyone to voice out their
opinions. Future research can look into these sites, especially the ones written in any
138
religious authorities give their sermons. Lately, there has been a resurgence among
Apparently, these authorities would give their sermons in their native dialects. The
researcher sees this as authentic and also a gold mine for identifying politeness
strategies whenever these authorities perform FTAs such as when giving advice,
clarifying information and when dwelling into the sanctity of the religion. Future
Future research can also improvise on the current research by involving more
participants or including a gender variable. The current research can also serve as a
guide for other researchers to study other Malay dialects and make comparisons with
the results obtained from this study. The researcher believes that Malay dialects and
the indigenous dialects in Malaysia too should be explored further especially at the
extralinguistic level. The researcher believes that Pragmatics has so much potential in
dialects that is fast declining today as there is so much focus on gearing the nation to
the English language. Moreover, as the lives of Malay families continue to progress
(or not), the preference to speak Standard Malay or even colloquial Malay has
encouragement and research on Pragmatics and the Malay and indigenous dialects in
Malaysia.
139