Schellenberg SimplifyingTheIRModel
Schellenberg SimplifyingTheIRModel
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://about.jstor.org/terms
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted
digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about
JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
University of California Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Music Perception 1 997 by the regents of the
Spring 1997, Vol. 14, No. 3, 295-318 university of California
E. GLENN SCHELLENBERG
University of Windsor
Narmour known
cognition (1990, 1992)
as the proposes a theory of melodic
implication-realization perception
(I-R) model. and
Narmour's
model is based on the idea that when listeners hear a melody they typically
form expectancies about its continuation. These expectancies are presumed
to result from a combination of innate and learned factors (see also Jones,
1990). The inclusion and specification of innate factors in the model is,
arguably, one of its most important contributions to the psychology of
music. The influence of these factors on melodic expectancies has been
shown to generalize across listeners from different musical cultures tested
in a wide variety of musical and nonmusical contexts (Cuddy & Lunney,
1995; Schellenberg, 1996), consistent with their proposed universality.
Nonetheless, the I-R model appears to be overspecified and more complex
than necessary (Schellenberg, 1996). The present report examines the de-
gree to which the model can be simplified while retaining its predictive
accuracy. Simplicity is one of the main criteria by which psychological models
295
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
296 E. Glenn Schellenberg
are evaluated (e.g., Cutting, Bruno, Brady, & Moore, 1992). Indeed, the
principle of parsimony (Occam's razor) is a scientific canon stating that a
simpler explanation of any phenomenon is preferable to a more complex
explanation. Simpler models, with fewer parameters, also "stand a better
chance of being scientifically replicable" (Bentler &c Mooijaart, 1989, p.
315). Hence, a simplified model of melodic expectancy is more likely to
describe a universal phenomenon (i.e., one that could be replicated across
all listeners in all musical contexts).
Learning is obviously involved in the expectancies that arise while listen-
ing to melodies. For example, a listener familiar with Western music (i.e.,
music from the common practice period, or from traditional or popular
idioms) would generally expect a Western melody to stay "in key." The
claim of innate components in melodic expectancy is more contentious.
According to the I-R model, listeners, regardless of cultural background or
exposure, form common expectancies that stem from innate psychological
principles of perception and cognition. Any melodic interval that is not
perceived as complete (or closed) is considered an implicative interval, such
intervals imply that some tones are more likely than others to follow. (For
a detailed account of factors causing closure, see Narmour, 1990.) The
interval between the second tone of an implicative interval and the follow-
ing tone is considered a realized interval. At its most basic (i.e., tone-to-
tone) level, the I-R model describes how implicative intervals imply real-
ized intervals and how some realized intervals are more implied than others.
Narmour (1990, 1992) claims that these implications result from five per-
ceptual predispositions (acting in combination with learned factors): registral
direction, intervallic difference, registral return, proximity, and closure. A
cursory description follows (for a more detailed description, see Schellenberg,
1996).
The first two predispositions (registral direction and intervallic differ-
ence) form the core of the I-R model; from these, the basic melodic struc-
tures are derived. Both principles depend on whether the implicative inter-
val is small (five semitones or fewer) or large (seven semitones or more),
because small intervals are considered to have different implications than
large intervals. (The tritone [six semitones] is considered a threshold inter-
val that is neither small nor large.) The principle of registral direction states
that small intervals imply a continuation of pitch direction (e.g., a small
upward implicative interval implies an upward realized interval), whereas
large intervals imply a change of direction (e.g., a large upward implicative
interval implies a downward realized interval or a unison). The principle of
intervallic difference states that small implicative intervals imply similarly-
sized realized intervals (defined as the same size two semitones if the
realized interval changes registral direction, the same size three semitones
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Simplifying the Implication-Realization Model of Melodic Expectancy 297
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
298 E. Glenn Schellenberg
-.s2g
"I fail*
rt > ^ m
'C f^ JS o x ti
lii-ii
s -s I H
s s i e^ &
j> I '6 ?<
> J3 v^^S e
u -2- - ^
Ill Id 3
s S -8 sU<
uiiii
iiiiii
s 2 ^ > e^
g o 5 S * -.
<H1J,
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Simplifying the Implication-Realization Model of Melodic Expectancy 299
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
300 E. Glenn Schellenberg
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Simplifying the Implication-Realization Model of Melodic Expectancy 301
Table 1
Multiple Regression Results from Data of Schellenberg
(1996, Appendixes A, B, and C)
Degrees of Freedom
R R2 F Model Residual
Experiment 1 (N=120)*
Model 1: Implication-realization (I-R) model .826 .683 40.49 6 113
Model 2: Revised model .871 .759 90.65 4 115
Model 3: Principal-components model (I-R) .818 .669 58.03 4 115
Model 4: Principal-components model (revised) .871 .759 121.62 3 116
Model 5: Two-factor model .871 .759 121.88 3 116
Experiment 2 (N = 200)
Model 1: I-R model .679 .461 33.18 5 194
Model 2: Revised model .726 .527 72.73 3 196
Model 3: Principal-components model (I-R) .669 .447 52.86 3 196
Model 4: Principal-components model (revised) .724 .524 108.38 2 197
Model 5: Two-factor model .721 .520 106.80 2 197
Experiment 3 (N= 132)*
Model 1: I-R model .830 .690 46.27 6 125
Model 2: Revised model .869- .755 97.66 4 127
Model 3: Principal-components model (I-R) .829 .688 70.00 4 127
Model 4: Principal-components model (revised) .868 .754 130.62 3 128
Model 5: Two-factor model .866 .750 127.89 3 128
Note. All ps < .0001. For each regression model, the number of predictor variables equals
the degrees of freedom for the model.
* These models included a covariate to control for effects of tonality.
intervals. Although Carlsen was not attempting to test the I-R model, his
stimuli can be considered implicative intervals. Intervals between the sec-
ond tone of his stimuli and the first tone of responses can be considered
realized intervals. Reanalyses of Carlsen's data revealed a good fit to the I-
R model (Cuddy & Lunney, 1995; Schellenberg, 1996) and no differences
as a function of cultural background (Schellenberg, 1996). A reanalysis of
Unyk and Carlsen's (1987) replication of the study by Carlsen (1981) with
American music students also revealed that response patterns were consis-
tent with predictions of the I-R model (Schellenberg, 1996).
Nonetheless, strong correlations among some of the quantified principles
of the I-R model raise the possibility of redundant predictors. Pairwise cor-
relations between predictors are provided in Table 2, calculated for the 263
combinations of implicative and realized intervals shown in Figure 1. The
correlations reveal that intervallic difference, proximity, and closure form
a highly collinear set of predictors. Thus, inclusion of all three principles in
the model may be unnecessary.
Schellenberg (1996)'s revised model (Table 1, Model 2), which contained
two fewer principles than the original I-R model (Table 1, Model 1), in fact
predicted his listeners' response patterns with greater accuracy. Quantified
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
302 E. Glenn Schellenberg
Table 2
Pairwise Correlations between Principles of the Implication-Realization
Model and the Revised Model (N= 263)
Intervallic Registral
Implication-Realization Model Difference Return Proximity Closure
**p<.0001.
values for the three revised principles are provided in Figure 3. Because of
intercorrelations among the original principles of intervallic difference, prox-
imity, and closure, Schellenberg omitted intervallic difference and closure
and revised proximity, forming a new predictor variable that he designated
proximity-revised. Proximity-revised simply represents the size of the real-
ized interval (in semitones); intervals are less implied as they become larger.
The remaining two principles were also modified. Registral direction was
revised so that it applies only to large intervals, with large implicative inter-
vals implying a realized interval in a different direction. Finally, Schellenberg
revised registral return as an all-or-none (dummy) variable, eliminating the
distinction between symmetric and approximately symmetric pitch patterns.
Although Schellenberg derived his revised principles on the basis of the
data from the first of his three experiments, the revised model proved supe-
rior to the original at predicting listeners' responses across all three experi-
ments.
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Simplifying the Implication-Realization Model of Melodic Expectancy 303
Fig. 3. Schellenberg's (1996) quantification of the three principles of his revised model.
Reprinted from Cognition, 55(1), E. G. Schellenberg, "Expectancy in melody: Tests of the
implication-realization model," 75-125, 1996, with kind permission of Elsevier Science-
NL, Sara Burgerhartstraat 25, 1055 KV Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
an octave from the second tone of the implicative interval (total of 200
ratings). The first tone of each interval was a dotted quarter note, the sec-
ond tone was an eighth note, and the test tone was a quarter note, such
that durations stood in a ratio of 3:1:2. Again, standard multiple regres-
sion analyses revealed that the I-R model successfully predicted response
patterns well above chance levels even though the stimulus contexts were
musically impoverished.
On the basis of their findings, Cuddy and Lunney (1995) revised registral
direction so that it applied only to large intervals, although their method of
requantification was different from that of Schellenberg (1996). They also
used the revised version of registral return as coded in Figure 3. Their most
complete explanation of response patterns was provided by a multiple re-
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
304 E. Glenn Schellenberg
gression model that contained a revised version of the I-R model (closure
was excluded, registral direction and registral return were revised) and three
co varites (one controlling for variation in ratings due to pitch height, the
other two controlling for influences of tonality).
Because Cuddy and Lunney (1995) did not use the revised model as
coded here, an initial analysis provided a direct comparison of the revised
and original I-R models. The results are summarized in Table 3 (Models 1
and 2). As with Schellenberg's (1996) data, the revised model did not result
in a loss of predictive accuracy compared with the original I-R model. Rather,
72.5% of the variance was explained by the revised model (plus the three
covariates) compared with 64.0% for the I-R model (plus covariates).
To summarize, the I-R model can successfully predict ratings in melodic
expectancy tasks well above chance levels regardless of differences in musi-
cal styles and groups of listeners. Nonetheless, considered in combination
with Schellenberg's (1996) findings, the reanalysis of Cuddy and Lunney's
(1995) data confirms that the I-R model can be substantially simplified
without loss of predictive power.
The objective of the present report was to derive the simplest possible
model of expectancy in melody while retaining the predictive power of
more complex models. Specifically, principal-components analysis was used
to measure the number of nonredundant (i.e., orthogonal, or uncorrelated)
dimensions embodied in the I-R model. A "principal-components" model
consisting solely of these unique dimensions was evaluated for relative effi-
Table 3
Multiple Regression Results from Data of Cuddy and Lunney
(1995, Appendix)
Degrees of Freedom
R R2 F Model Residual
All ps < .0001 (N = 200). For each regression model, the number of predictor variables
equals the degrees of freedom for the model. Each model included three covariates. Two of
these held constant differences in responding due to the tonal implications of the stimulus pat-
terns; the third controlled for influences of the pitch height of the test tones.
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Simplifying the Implication-Realization Model of Melodic Expectancy 305
cacy in predicting the data of Cuddy and Lunney (1995) and those of
Schellenberg (1996); the principal-components model was also compared
with the original I-R model and Schellenberg's revised model. Finally, a
separate principal-components analysis was conducted on the principles of
the revised model; from this analysis, a further simplified model of melodic
expectancy was derived.
PRINCIPAL-COMPONENT ANALYSIS
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
306 E. Glenn Schellenberg
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Simplifying the Implication-Realization Model of Melodic Expectancy 307
Table 4
Results from Principal-Components Analysis of the Implication-
Realization Model
Standardized Correlation
Principle Coefficient Coefficient with Factor
Factor 1
Registral direction -.068 -.034 .037
Intervallic difference .876 .436 .835***
Registral return -.055 -.043 .001
Proximity .242 .472 .876***
Closure .495 .342 55***
Constant -1.079
Factor 2
Registral direction .024 .012 .032
Intervallic difference -.157 -.078 -.041
Registral return 1.200 .925 .962***
Proximity -.072 -.140 -.107
Closure .412 .284 .322***
Constant -.554
Factor 3
Registral direction 1.922 .962 .983***
Intervallic difference .308 .153 .201 * *
Registral return .032 .024 .042
Proximity -.031 -.061 -.016
Closure -.244 -.168 -.130*
Constant -.828
*p < .05
**p<.005
***p<.0001
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
308 E. Glenn Schellenberg
direction principle (r = .59, p < .0001). These correlations reveal that the
principles of the revised model, which was derived from listeners' expect-
ancy ratings, are associated with those of the mathematically derived prin-
cipal components (I-R) model. Nonetheless, the revised registral direction
principle was also weakly correlated with the first and second factors (r =
.23, p < .0005, and r = .44, p < .0001, respectively). Moreover, the revised
model (Model 2) consistently outperformed the principal-components (I-
R) model (Model 3) across the analyses reported in Tables 1 and 3, ex-
plaining between 5.3% and 9.5% more variance in each case. Hence, the
revised model is not simply a reflection of the latent structure of the origi-
nal I-R model. Rather, the changes incorporated into the revised model
actually improve its predictive power relative to the original I-R model.
Subsequent analyses focused on potential simplification and improvement
of the revised model.
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Simplifying the Implication-Realization Model of Melodic Expectancy 309
Table 5
Results from Principal-Components Analysis of the Revised Model
Standardized Correlation
Principle Coefficient Coefficient with Factor
Factor 1
Registral direction-revised .880 .608 .814*
Registral return-revised 1.618 .616 .820*
Proximity-revised .004 .013 -.013
Constant -.323
Factor 2
Registral direction-revised -.099 -.068 -.087
Registral return-revised .219 .083 .065
Proximity-revised .274 .993 .996*
Constant -1.740
*p < .0001.
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
310 E. Glenn Schellenberg
tions between the principle and expectancy data are expected. Although
the principle is specified here using semitones, any logarithmic pitch scale
could be substituted. Thus, the pitch-proximity principle can be used with
scales whose intervals are not multiples of semitones. With the Thai scale,
for example, where consecutive tones of the seven-tone scale are equidis-
tant or approximately equidistant (Ellingson, 1992; Morton, 1980), one
would code a unison as 0, an interval of one scale step as 1, an interval of
two scale steps as 2, and so on. Moreover, the pitch-proximity principle
does not suffer from the arbitrary assumption of Narmour's (1990, 1992)
original concept of proximity, in which all realized intervals larger than
five semitones are considered to be equally nonproximate (see Figure 2).
Expectancies are typically considered to represent learned schmas,
whereas perceptual grouping based on proximity is considered to reflect a
"primitive" (unlearned) process (Bregman, 1990). Nonetheless, because
proximity tends to predict grouping in vision (Koffka, 1935; Kohler, 1947)
and audition (Bregman, 1990), the pitch-proximity principle could stem
from a hard-wired perceptual predisposition that also influences expectan-
cies when listening to melodies, as Narmour claims. For example, listeners
perceive tones that are proximate in pitch to be similar (Kallman, 1982).
Moreover, research on "auditory scene analysis" (Bregman, 1990) indi-
cates that proximity is a critical factor in the perception of auditory pat-
terns that appears to be independent of learning. Tones proximate in pitch
tend to be heard as originating from the same source or object; nonproximate
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Simplifying the Implication-Realization Model of Melodic Expectancy 311
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
312 E. Glenn Schellenberg
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Simplifying the Implication-Realization Model of Melodic Expectancy 313
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
314 E. Glenn Schellenberg
References
Aldwell, E., c Schachter, C. (1989). Harmony and voice leading (2nd d.). San Diego:
Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Bentler, P. M., & Mooijaart, A. (1989). Choice of structural model via parsimony: A ratio-
nale based on precision. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 315-317.
Bharucha, J. J., & Pryor, J. H. (1986). Disrupting the isochrony underlying rhythm: An
asymmetry in discrimination. Perception & Psychophysics, 40, 137-141.
boltz, M. (lyyl). borne structural determinants or melody recall. Memory cr Cognition,
19,239-251.
Boltz, M. (1993). The generation of temporal and melodic expectancies during musical
listening. Perception & Psychophysics, 53, 585-600.
Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Carlsen, J. C. (1981). Some factors which influence melodic expectancy. Psychomusicology,
1, 12-29.
Cuddy, L. L., 6c Lunney, C. A. (1995). Expectancies generated by melodic intervals: Percep-
tual judgments of melodic continuity. Perception & Psychophysics, 57, 451-462.
Cutting, J. E., Bruno, N., Brady, N. P., & Moore, C. (1992). Selectivity, scope, and simplic-
ity of models: A lesson from fitting judgments of perceived depth. Journal of Experimen-
tal Psychology: General, 121, 364-381.
Diekhoff, G. (1992). Statistics for the social and behavioral sciences: Unwartate, bwanate,
multivariate. Dubuque, IA: Wm. C. Brown.
Deutsch, D. (1978). Delayed pitch comparisons and the principle of proximity. Perception
& Psychophysics, 23, 227-230.
Dowling, W. J., 6c Harwood, D. L. (1986). Music cognition. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Ellingson, T. (1992). Transcription. In H. Myers (Ed.), Ethnomusicology: An introduction
(pp. 110-152). New York: Norton.
2. Funding for this research was provided by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Re-
search Council of Canada. Emmanuel Bigand, Katy Dunham, Mari Jones, Bill Thompson,
and Sandra Trehub provided helpful comments on earlier versions of the manuscript.
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Simplifying the Implication-Realization Model of Melodic Expectancy 315
Jones, M. R. (1981). Music as a stimulus for psychological motion: Part I. Some determi-
nants of expectancies. Vsychomusicolozy, 1 (2), 34-51.
Jones, M. R. (982). Music as a stimulus for psychological motion: Part II. An expectancy
model. Vsychomusicology, 2 (1), 1-13.
Jones, M. R. (1987). Dynamic pattern structure in music: Recent theory and research. Per-
ception & Psychophysics, 41, 621-634.
Jones, M. R. (1990). Learning and the development of expectancies: An interactionist ap-
proach. Psychomusicology, 9, 193-228.
Jones, M. R., c Boltz, M. (1989). Dynamic attending and responses to time. Psychological
Review, 96, 459-491.
Jones, M. R., Boltz, M., & Kidd, G. (1982). Controlled attending as a function of melodic
and temporal context. Perception & Psychophysics, 32, 211-218.
Jones, M. R., Summerell, L., & Marshburn, E. (1987). Recognizing melodies: A dynamic
interpretation. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39 A, 89-121.
Kallman, H. J. (1982). Octave equivalence as measured by similarity ratings. Perception &
Psychophysics, 32, 37-49.
Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt psychology. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Kohler, W. (1947). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts of modern psy-
chology. New York: Liveright.
Krumhansl, C. L. (1995). Music psychology: Influences from music theory. Music Theory
Spectrum, 17, 53-80.
Meyer, L. B. (1973). Explaining music: Essays and explorations. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
Morton, D. (1980). Thailand. In S. Sadie (Ed.), The new Grove dictionary of music and
musicians (Vol. 18, pp. 712-722). London: Macmillan.
Narmour, E. (1989). The "genetic code" of melody: Cognitive structures generated by the
implication-realization model. Contemporary Music Review, 1989,4, 45-64.
Narmour, E. (1990). The analysis and cognition of basic melodic structures: The implica-
tion-realization model. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Narmour, E. (1992). The analysis and cognition of melodic complexity: The implication-
realization model. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Rosner, B. S., & Meyer, L. B. (1982). Melodic processes and the perception of music. In D.
Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of music (pp. 317-341). New York: Academic Press.
Rosner, B. S., & Meyer, L. B. (1986). The perceptual roles of melodic process, contour and
form. Music Perception, 4, 1-40.
Schellenberg, E. G. (1996). Expectancy in melody: Tests of the implication-realization model.
Cognition, 58, 75-125.
Schellenberg, E. G., & Trehub, S. E. (1996). Children's discrimination of melodic intervals.
Developmental Psychology, 32, 1039-1050.
Schmuckler, M. A. (1989). Expectation in music: Investigation of melodic and harmonic
processes. Music Perception, 7, 109-150.
Schmuckler, M. A. (lyyu). lhe performance or global expectancies, fsychomusicology, y,
122-147.
Tabachnick, B. G., 6c Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (3rd d.). New York:
Harper Collins.
Unyk, A. M., & Carlsen, J. C. (1987). The influence of expectancy on melodic perception.
Psychomusicology, 7, 3-23.
Appendix
The tables in this appendix provide detailed statistics from the multiple regression analy-
ses. For each predictor variable (or set of predictors) in each model, the squared semipartial
correlation (sr2 or SR2), F ratio, and p value are reported. The squared semipartial correla-
tion represents the proportion of variance in the outcome variable that is uniquely ex-
plained by each predictor (or set of predictors).
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
316 E. Glenn Schellenberg
Table Al
Analyses of Data from Schellenberg (1996)
sr> F df p
Experiment 2 (N = 200)
Model 1: Implication-Realization (I-R) Model
model R2 = .461
Registral direction .093 33.63 1,194 <.0001
Intervallic difference .017 5.93 1,194 <.O5
Registral return .035 12.42 1,194 <.001
Proximity .071 25.69 1,194 <.0001
Closure .016 5.60 1,194 <.O5
Model 2: Revised Model
model R2 = .527
Registral direction-revised .094 38.90 1,196 <.0001
Registral return-revised .016 6.50 1,196 <.O5
Proximity-revised .358 148.16 1,196 <.0001
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
Simplifying the Implication-Realization Model of Melodic Expectancy 317
Table Al (Continued)
st2 F df p
Model 3: Principal-Components Model (I-R)
model JR2 = .447
Factor 1 .311 110.24 1,196 <.0001
Factor 2 .036 12.94 1,196 <.0005
Factor 3 .103 36.61 1,196 <.0001
Model 4: Principal-Components Model (Revised)
model R2 = .524
Factor 1 .144 59.44 1,197 <.0001
Factor 2 .381 157.53 1,197 <.0001
Model 5: Two-Factor Model
model R2 = .520
Pitch proximity .364 149.38 1,197 <.0001
Pitch reversal .144 59.26 1,197 <.0001
Experiment 3 (N=132)
Model 1: Implication-Realization (I-R) Model
model R2 = .690
Registral direction .010 3.97 1,125 <.O5
Intervallic difference .065 25.85 1,125 <.0001
Registral return .031 12.42 1,125 <.001
Proximity .067 26.76 1,125 <.0001
Closure .048 19.31 1,125 <.0001
Tonality covariate .009 3.26 1,125 <.l
Model 2: Revised Model
model R2 = .755
Registral direction-revised .077 39.73 1, 127 <.0001
Registral return-revised .015 7.75 1, 127 <.01
Proximity-revised .570 295.10 1,127 <.0001
Tonality covariate .010 4.96 1,127 <.O5
Model 3: Principal-Components Model (I-R)
model R2 = .688
Factor 1 .615 250.42 1,127 <.0001
Factor 2 .043 17.52 1,127 <.0001
Factor 3 .017 6.86 1,127 <.01
Tonality covariate .008 3.41 1,127 <.l
Model 4: Principal-Components Model (Revised)
model R2 = .754
Factor 1 .122 63.10 1,128 <.0001
Factor 2 .599 311.29 1,128 <.0001
Tonality covariate .010 5.09 1,128 <.O5
Model 5: Two-Factor Model
model R2 = .750
Pitch proximity .579 296.22 1,128 <.0001
Pitch reversal .120 61.41 1,128 <.0001
Tonality covariate .010 5.15 1,128 <.O5
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
318 E. Glenn Schellenberg
Table A2
Analyses of Data from Cuddy and Lunney (1995, Appendix) N= 200
st2 F df p
This content downloaded from 130.132.173.56 on Mon, 16 May 2016 12:00:17 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms