Sistema Atencional de Reorientacion de La Atencion
Sistema Atencional de Reorientacion de La Atencion
Author Manuscript
Neuron. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 June 30.
Published in final edited form as:
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Abstract
Survival can depend on the ability to change a current course of action to respond to potentially
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
advantageous or threatening stimuli. This reorienting response involves the coordinated action of
a right hemisphere dominant ventral frontoparietal network that interrupts and resets ongoing activity
and a dorsal frontoparietal network specialized for selecting and linking stimuli and responses. At
rest, each network is distinct and internally correlated, but when attention is focused, the ventral
network is suppressed to prevent reorienting to distracting events. These different patterns of
recruitment may reflect inputs to the ventral attention network from the locus coeruleus/
norepinephrine system. While originally conceptualized as a system for redirecting attention from
one object to another, recent evidence suggests a more general role in switching between networks,
which may explain recent evidence of its involvement in functions such as social cognition.
Introduction
To safely navigate the environment, survive, and reproduce, animals and people must rapidly
select sensory information that is relevant to their goals (e.g., routes, food, mates). They must
also quickly redirect their attention and change their course of action when faced with novel,
potentially threatening, or rewarding stimuli. The complex set of adjustments in response to
novel and unexpected stimuli is defined here as a reorienting response. Reorienting may occur
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
between two environmental stimuli, such as when we orient to the siren of an ambulance while
reading a newspaper, or between an internally directed activity and the environment, as when
the same siren interrupts a train of thought. While several autonomic and motor responses can
be triggered by novel sensory stimuli through subcortical reflexes that are largely automatic
and unconscious (the orienting reflex; Sokolov, 1963), more recent work indicates that this
adaptive behavior involves a complex interaction between cortical systems specialized for the
selection of sensory information. A dorsal frontoparietal (or dorsal attention) network enables
the selection of sensory stimuli based on internal goals or expectations (goal-driven attention)
and links them to appropriate motor responses. A ventral frontoparietal (or ventral attention)
network detects salient and behaviorally relevant stimuli in the environment, especially when
unattended (stimulus-driven attention). These systems dynamically interact during normal
perception to determine where and what we attend to. In this paper, we review evidence from
neuroimaging, neuropsychology, and neurophysiology on the role of these two networks,
particularly the ventral network, in the reorienting response.
finding an object with particular features (Treisman and Gelade, 1980; Wolfe, 1994) or at a
particular location (Eriksen and Hoffman, 1974; Posner, 1980) and responding to it in an
appropriate manner (Hommel, 2000). This form of selection is labeled goal-driven or
endogenous to emphasize the internal or top-down signals that guide perception through a
dynamic interaction with sensory or bottom-up information. The biased-competition model of
attention, for example, proposes that objects in a visual scene compete for access to visual
short-term memory and that the competition is biased by top-down signals that promote access
of behaviorally relevant objects (Desimone and Duncan, 1995). These top-down signals,
characterized as working memory (e.g., Downing, 2000; but see Woodman and Luck, 2007),
long-term memory (Moores et al., 2003), or action related (Craighero et al., 2002; Rosenbaum,
1991), interact with sensory (bottom-up) signals produced by objects in the visual scene,
enabling the desired object to be selectively perceived and entered into memory at the expense
of unimportant objects (Bundesen, 1990; Wolfe, 1994). For instance, Figure 1A shows a
student who focuses on his computer desktop while writing his thesis and ignores surrounding
objects and people.
Adaptive behavior, however, also requires that we respond to objects that are outside the current
focus of attention, i.e., that do not match current settings for selecting stimuli and responses.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
The object we are looking for may appear with different features than we expected or at a
different location. More importantly, a new object may appear that requires a completely
different course of action. While the student looks at the computer screen, a colleague may ask
a question (Figure 1B), or while a monkey searches for food, a predator may appear. Moreover,
we may be presented with new events requiring a response while we are engaged in internally
directed activities that do not involve an interaction with the environment. Someone may
distract us while we are considering the meaning of a sentence in the thesis we are writing, or
a monkey may quickly react to the appearance of a predator while grooming or eating.
Reorienting to new objects may occur reflexively, based on their high sensory salience (Jonides
and Yantis, 1988), particularly when we do not have a specific task to do (Pashler and Harris,
2001), but distinctive objects attract attention more effectively when they are also behaviorally
relevant (Yantis and Egeth, 1999), either because they match our current goals or because of
long-term memory associations that signal their importance, as when we hear the phone ringing
or the siren of an ambulance. In fact, the degree to which a distinctive but entirely irrelevant
object can attract our attention, so-called exogenous attention, is controversial (Folk et al.,
1992; Gibson and Kelsey, 1998; Jonides, 1981; Posner and Cohen, 1984; Theeuwes and
Burger, 1998; Yantis and Egeth, 1999). In some cases, shifts of attention to a distinctive
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
stimulus can be part of a task goal (Bacon and Egeth, 1994), as when someone tries to detect
any salient object appearing in a visual scene. In other cases, distinctive but irrelevant objects
may share a specific feature with our current goal, as when we notice someone wearing a red
sweater while looking for a friend with a red hat (Folk et al., 1992; Gibson and Kelsey,
1998).
mechanism proposed by biased competition and related theories (Bundesen, 1990; Desimone
and Duncan, 1995; Wolfe, 1994). The dorsal system generates and maintains endogenous
signals based on current goals and preexisting information about likely contingencies and sends
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
out top-down signals that bias the processing of appropriate stimulus features and locations in
sensory cortex. This conclusion is based on evidence that the dorsal network is preactivated
by the expectation of seeing an object at a particular location or with certain features (e.g.,
movement in a specific direction) (Corbetta et al., 2000; Hopfinger et al., 2000; Kastner et al.,
1999; Shulman et al., 1999), by the preparation of a specific response (Astafiev et al., 2003;
Connolly et al., 2002), or by the short-term memory of a visual scene (LaBar et al., 1999;
Pessoa et al., 2002). The dorsal system is also involved in linking relevant stimuli to responses,
as it is modulated when people change their motor plan for an object (Rushworth et al.,
2001). Under some conditions, the preparatory activation of the dorsal frontoparietal network
extends to visual cortex, presumably reflecting the top-down modulation of sensory
representations (Giesbrecht et al., 2006; Hopfinger et al., 2000; Kastner et al., 1999; Serences
et al., 2004; Silver et al., 2007; Sylvester et al., 2007) (Figure 1A). Accordingly, anticipatory
activity may predict performance to subsequent targets (Giesbrecht et al., 2006; Pessoa and
Padmala, 2005; Sapir et al., 2005; Sylvester et al., 2007). Finally, recent studies show that
electrical or magnetic stimulation of FEF or IPS leads to a retinotopically specific modulation
of visual areas and parallel improvement of perception at corresponding locations of the visual
field (Moore and Armstrong, 2003; Ruff et al., 2006, 2007).
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
A second system, the ventral frontoparietal network, is not activated by expectations or task
preparation but responds along with the dorsal network when behaviorally relevant objects (or
targets) are detected (Corbetta et al., 2000). Both dorsal and ventral networks are also activated
during reorienting, with enhanced responses during the detection of targets that appear at
unattended locations. For example, enhanced responses are observed when subjects are cued
to expect a target at one location but it unexpectedly appears at another (i.e., invalid targets
in the Posner spatial cueing paradigm) (Arrington et al., 2000; Corbetta et al., 2000; Kincade
et al., 2005; Macaluso et al., 2002; Vossel et al., 2006) or when a target appears infrequently,
as in oddball paradigms (Bledowski et al., 2004; Braver et al., 2001; Linden et al., 1999;
Marois et al., 2000; McCarthy et al., 1997; Stevens et al., 2005) (Figure 1B). Core regions of
the ventral network include temporoparietal junction (TPJ) cortex (anatomically, TPJ is more
strictly defined as the cortex at the intersection of the posterior end of the STS, the inferior
parietal lobule, and the lateral occipital cortex), defined as the posterior sector of the superior
temporal sulcus (STS) and gyrus (STG) and the ventral part of the supramarginal gyrus (SMG)
and ventral frontal cortex (VFC), including parts of middle frontal gyrus (MFG), inferior frontal
gyrus (IFG), frontal operculum, and anterior insula (Figure 2A, orange regions). An early
theory of how the two networks interact (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002) proposed that when
attention is reoriented to a new source of information (stimulus-driven reorienting), output
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
from the ventral network interrupts (as a circuit breaker) ongoing selection in the dorsal
network, which in turn shifts attention toward the novel object of interest.
Although both attentional networks have been most extensively investigated in vision, the
available evidence indicates a supramodal function (Driver and Spence, 1998; Macaluso et al.,
2002). The ventral network (right TPJ, right IFG) registers salient events in the environment
not only in the visual but also in the auditory and tactile modalities (Downar et al., 2000), and
similar dorsal and ventral parietal and frontal regions are modulated by reorienting to invalid
targets (Arrington et al., 2000; Corbetta et al., 2000; Giessing et al., 2006; Kincade et al.,
2005; Macaluso et al., 2002; Mayer et al., 2006; Vossel et al., 2006) or by oddballs (Braver et
al., 2001; Kiehl et al., 2001; Linden et al., 1999; Marois et al., 2000) in different modalities.
The sections below review in more detail recent work on these networks, particularly the ventral
network, including: (1) the functional-anatomical independence of each network, (2) the
importance of behavioral relevance rather than sensory salience in driving the ventral network,
(3) whether the output of the ventral network initiates a reorienting response and how the dorsal
and ventral networks interact, (4) how the functions of the ventral network may generalize
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
beyond perception and action to include memory and social cognition, and finally (5) the
emerging link between activity in the ventral network and the output of the locus coeruleus-
norepinephrine system (LC-NE), as recently outlined by neurocomputational theories (Aston-
Jones and Cohen, 2005; Bouret and Sara, 2005; Dayan and Yu, 2006; Yu and Dayan, 2005).
We do not consider in this discussion the relationship between cortical and subcortical regions
involved in the control of attention. There is strong evidence that subcortical structures like
the superior colliculus are involved in stimulus-driven but also goal-driven attention (Bell et
al., 2004; Fecteau et al., 2004; Rafal et al., 1988; Sapir et al., 1999). The pulvinar nucleus of
the thalamus has been proposed as a gateway structure that funnels top-down biases from
parietal areas into visual cortex (Petersen et al., 1987; Shipp, 2004).
Regions that putatively belong to the dorsal and ventral attention systems, based on their
consistent activation in the Posner cueing paradigm to spatial cues and unattended targets,
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
respectively, also show significant interregional correlation at rest (Fox et al., 2006b) or during
an active task with the mean task signal removed (He et al., 2007a) (see Figure 3). There is a
remarkable similarity between the dorsal parietal and frontal regions identified by a meta-
analysis of task-evoked activation studies (Figure 2) and those showing high resting-state
correlations (Figure 3). Similar results are found for ventral frontoparietal regions coactivated
during stimulus-driven orienting (Fox et al., 2006a; He et al., 2007a). Moreover, the right
hemispheric bias observed in the ventral attention network in several activation studies
(Arrington et al., 2000; Corbetta et al., 2000; Downar et al., 2000) is mirrored in fcMRI (Fox
et al., 2006a; He et al., 2007a) (compare ventral network in Figures 2 and 3).
While segregation between dorsal and ventral attention networks is nearly complete,
spontaneous activity in right posterior MFG correlates with both networks (Figure 3),
indicating that right MFG may contain intermixed neuronal populations respectively connected
with dorsal or ventral regions (Fox et al., 2006a). This result raises the possibility that ventral
and dorsal networks do not directly interact but are principally linked through prefrontal cortex
(Fox et al., 2006a). This link is also supported by results obtained in neglect subjects showing
that the functional disconnection of MFG with dorsal parietal cortex is responsible for abnormal
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
stimulus selection (see below and He et al., 2007a) (wire diagram, Figure 2B). The functional
segregation of the two networks in the absence of a task may allow their flexible recruitment
during active behavior. For example, while dorsal regions are active following the presentation
of an instructive cue, ventral regions are not recruited or are even suppressed (Shulman et al.,
2003;Todd et al., 2005). However, following the presentation of a target, both ventral and
dorsal regions respond briskly (Corbetta et al., 2000;Hampshire et al., 2007;Shulman et al.,
1999,2003). In summary, the correspondence between activation and connectivity analyses
provides strong evidence for separate dorsal and ventral attention networks forming distinct
functional systems.
(Kincade et al., 2005). Kincade and colleagues separated the BOLD activity produced by an
uninformative but salient peripheral cue, a red square in an array of green squares, from the
activity produced by discriminating a subsequent rotated T or L (Figure 4A). In control
conditions, subjects were presented with a neutral display of randomly intermixed color squares
or a foveal cue that oriented attention voluntarily. Exogenous cues (the red square) did not
activate the ventral network (Figure 4A), even though performance was better at that location,
indicating that these cues were effective in generating a shift of attention. In contrast, the dorsal
network (IPS/SPL and FEF) showed stronger activation for exogenous than neutral cues
(Figure 4A), although the strongest recruitment was recorded for endogenous cues (data not
shown). Many other studies have measured activations in exogenous orienting paradigms that
have combined activations during the cue and target periods (Kim et al., 1999; Lepsien and
Pollmann, 2002; Mayer et al., 2006; Peelen et al., 2004; Rosen et al., 1999). Although these
studies are more difficult to interpret, they indicate that the ventral network is not recruited by
orienting to uninformative but salient cues presented before a target appears (see Peelen et al.,
2004, for an exception). Similarly, de Fockert and colleagues showed that uninformative but
salient distracters that attract attention did not activate the ventral system (de Fockert et al.,
2004) (Figure 4B), although they did activate the dorsal system. The overall conclusion is that
exogenous orienting recruits the same dorsal frontoparietal network that is responsible for
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Conversely, the ventral network is well activated by stimuli that are important, even if they are
not very distinctive. Indovina and Macaluso (2007), for example, showed that unattended
targets of low salience activated regions in both dorsal (FEF, precuneus) and ventral (IFG and
anterior insula) attention networks, in line with previous results (Arrington et al., 2000;
Corbetta et al., 2000; Macaluso et al., 2002), to a much greater degree than highly salient but
irrelevant distracters (see Figure 4C). Finally, the ventral network is activated by irrelevant
objects when they are similar to a target object. Serences et al. (2005) asked subjects to
categorize red foveal letters interspersed among a rapid, successive series of colored foveal
letters (rapid serial visual presentation, or RSVP) while peripheral distracter letters were
occasionally presented in the target color (red) or in a nontarget color (green) (Figure 4D). This
situation is analogous to when we look in a crowd for a friend wearing a red sweater and notice
people wearing red but not green clothes (contingent orienting; Folk et al., 1992). TPJ
activation was only observed for the red distracters (Figure 4D), consistent with the hypothesis
that the ventral network responds mainly to stimuli thought to be behaviorally relevant (see
also Downar et al., 2001).
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
In summary, the ventral network is not activated by orienting to distinctive but unimportant
stimuli (exogenous orienting), except perhaps in the special case where subjects do not have
an ongoing task, but does underlie reorienting to environmental stimuli based on their task
relevance. An important conclusion from these neuroimaging studies is that the psychological
distinction between exogenous and endogenous orienting (Jonides, 1981) may not map onto
different neural systems. Rather, a more fundamental distinction appears to be between systems
involved in orienting, both exogenous and goal-driven, i.e., the dorsal attention system, and
those involved in stimulus-driven reorienting, i.e., the ventral and dorsal attention systems.
In one study, subjects saw a rapid stream of letters (RSVP) and were instructed to look for an
occasional digit (Figure 5A) (Shulman et al., 2003). Prior to the point at which the digit was
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
detected, while subjects were still searching the letter displays, the ventral network (bilateral
TPJ, R MFG, and R IFG) showed a sustained deactivation (Figure 5C, green-blue voxels).
These deactivated regions overlapped regions that showed increased positive responses to
unattended targets in a separate experiment, indicating that the deactivation occurred within
ventral regions involved in stimulus-driven reorienting. Shulman and colleagues (Shulman et
al., 2003) suggested that the suppression of activity prevented an inappropriate response to
irrelevant stimuli. Because targets still triggered a robust positive response, however, activity
in the ventral network appeared to have been gated by task relevance or filtered, with only
targets passing the filter. Stronger filtering appeared to correlate with better performance,
because the average deactivation in right TPJ was significantly larger on trials in which the
subsequent target was detected than missed (Shulman et al., 2007) (Figure 5A).
In a second study (Todd et al., 2005), subjects remembered a set of objects in a visual display,
and following a blank retention interval, decided whether any of the objects were present in a
new display (Figure 5B). The larger the number of objects the subject had to remember (the
memory load), the more R TPJ was deactivated during the retention interval. The authors
separately showed that higher memory loads resulted in poorer detection of a novel unattended
stimulus, suggesting that high memory loads suppress activity in R TPJ and prevent stimulus-
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
driven reorienting (Todd et al., 2005). Together, these studies indicate that when subjects focus
on a task, signals for task relevance (filtering in Figure 2B) deactivate TPJ, preventing
reorienting to unimportant objects.
Another possible source of top-down signals is prefrontal cortex (Desimone and Duncan,
1995; Miller and Cohen, 2001). Resting-state analyses suggest that R MFG may link dorsal
and ventral networks (Fox et al., 2006a), possibly funneling top-down biases from the dorsal
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
network onto the ventral network (Figure 5C and 2B). R MFG is probably not the source
because it showed sustained deactivation along with R TPJ and R IFG (Figure 5C). However,
sustained increases were observed in anterior cingulate and anterior insula (Shulman et al.,
2003), which have been postulated to form the core of a network for cognitive control
(Dosenbach et al., 2006) (orange arrows in Figure 5C). The influence from these cortical
regions may be direct through cortico-cortical interactions or indirectly via subcortical loops.
In the last section, we relate the pattern of activity in TPJ, including filtering signals, to the
output of the LC-NE, which receives input from the anterior cingulate and the anterior insula
(Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005; Ongur et al., 2003).
If prefrontal cortex is the source or the conduit of these modulations onto TPJ, then poor top-
down control of stimulus-driven reorienting should be evident after prefrontal lesions. Chao
and Knight (1995) reported that patients with unilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC)
lesions showed markedly decreased performance in an auditory match-to-sample task due to
irrelevant distracter tones presented during the retention interval. Loss of prefrontal inputs may
have decreased top-down control over TPJ, resulting in inappropriate reorienting to distracting
stimuli (see also Ro et al., 1998; Snow and Mattingley, 2006).
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
In summary, only environmental stimuli that are behaviorally relevant trigger the ventral
network. The ventral network response is suppressed when irrelevant stimuli are presented,
even if they are distinctive, reflecting a filtering signal that gates sensory responses by
behavioral relevance. The source of the filtering signal may be the dorsal network or other
parts of prefrontal cortex, either directly or indirectly via subcortical loops.
The dorsal network contains the neural machinery for directing attention and the eyes to sensory
stimuli appearing at unexpected locations, with spatially selective responses to contralateral
stimuli and responses to movements of attention or the eyes (Beauchamp et al., 2001; Corbetta
et al., 1998, 1993; Nobre et al., 1997; Schluppeck et al., 2005; Sereno et al., 2001; Sweeney et
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
al., 1996; Sylvester et al., 2007). In contrast, group-averaged studies of ventral regions (TPJ,
VFC) have not found spatially selective responses during reorienting (Corbetta et al., 2002;
Macaluso et al., 2002; Macaluso and Patria, 2007). Similarly, mapping studies in individuals
have only reported weak spatially selective responses near or within the ventral network in
parts of MFG (Hagler and Sereno, 2006; Jack et al., 2007) and superior temporal gyrus (STG)
(Jack et al., 2007). The weak evidence for spatial selectivity in the ventral network suggests
that spatial reorienting is not mediated solely by that network but involves joint activation of
dorsal and ventral regions.
There is little evidence, however, that short-latency responses in the ventral attention network
precede those in dorsal areas and trigger a reorienting response. Within dorsal parietal and
frontal sites, EEG- or MEG-based estimates of visual response latency to targets for an eye
movement vary between 130 and 170 ms (Evdokimidis et al., 2001; McDowell et al., 2005;
Sestieri et al., 2008). Within ventral sites in TPJ and IFG, the response to targets is thought to
be indexed by the P300 potential, with a latency of 300400 ms, considerably longer than the
dorsal latencies (Bledowski et al., 2004; Daffner et al., 2003; Knight et al., 1989; Menon et al.,
1997; Yamaguchi and Knight, 1991a). Unfortunately, P300 and eye movement paradigms are
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
difficult to compare. There have been a number of ERP/MEG studies of spatial reorienting,
but the results are ambiguous in relation to the relative latency of dorsal and ventral parietal
regions (Luck et al., 1994; Mangun and Hillyard, 1991). Invalid targets that follow a voluntary
cue to shift attention increase a late-positive deflection (230400 ms) at central, parietal, and
occipital sites that might correspond to P300 (Mangun and Hillyard, 1991). At temporal
electrodes ipsilateral to the target (Hopfinger and Ries, 2005), invalid targets that follow an
uninformative (exogenous) cue produce a negative-going deflection in the range of 200250
ms, preceding a separate P300. Although this latter paradigm involved noninformative cues,
the ERP component was sensitive to several task-contingent factors, reflecting top-down
signals (Hopfinger and Ries, 2005).
Overall, the latency of visual responses to salient behaviorally relevant visual stimuli is, if
anything, shorter in dorsal parietal than in ventral parietal areas, but definitive studies have not
been conducted. In awake behaving monkeys, neural responses to visual stimuli in lateral
intraparietal area (LIP), the putative homolog of human IPS/SPL, show a very rapid
nonselective volley (~50 ms) followed by slower oscillations (100200 ms) that are modulated
by spatial attention (Bisley et al., 2004). In more ventral parietal cortex, in correspondence
with area 7A, which shows modulation by unattended stimuli (Constantinidis and Steinmetz,
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
2001; Robinson et al., 1995) and salient oddball stimuli during simple fixation (Constantinidis
and Steinmetz, 2005), similar to the ventral attention network, average response are about 100
ms (typical range 70200 ms; Constandidinis personal communication). No direct comparison
on the same task has been carried out, however. Reorienting of attention to a behaviorally
relevant and salient stimulus outside of the current focus is probably initiated in dorsal
frontoparietal cortex in conjunction with subcortical structures (e.g., superior colliculus).
Ventral system activity during reorienting may reflect slower adjustments necessary to
complete or carry out a complex reorienting response that involves shifts in task sets,
expectations, reward contingencies, and arousal.
Do Signals from the Ventral Network Influence Reorienting and the Dorsal
Network?
While the latency data from electrophysiological studies are ambiguous on whether ventral
network activity triggers dorsal activity during reorienting, transcranial magnetic stimulation
studies (TMS) nonetheless support a key role for ventral regions in reorienting attention and
detecting targets in conjunction with dorsal frontoparietal regions (IPS, FEF). An extensive
discussion of TMS studies of visuospatial attention is beyond the scope of this review, but
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
some conclusions can be drawn from the extant literature. First, interference with regions in
inferior parietal cortex (TPJ, SMG, AG) disrupts visual target detection and reorienting
(Chambers et al., 2004a; Ellison et al., 2004; Meister et al., 2006). Second, disruption has been
demonstrated for stimulation latencies ranging from 90120 ms (Chambers et al., 2004a) to
200300 ms following target onset (Chambers et al., 2004a; Ellison et al., 2004; Meister et al.,
2006). Early interference effects may reflect disruption of a signal that disengages attention
from its current location and initiates reorienting (Chambers et al., 2004a). Third, the regions
in inferior parietal cortex that show effects of TMS depend on the task: R TPJ during detection
of bilateral stimuli (Meister et al., 2006), angular but not supra-marginal gyrus (SMG) during
reorienting in an exogenous cueing paradigm (Chambers et al., 2004a), SMG during reorienting
in an endogenous cueing paradigm (Chambers et al., 2004b), and STG during visual search
(Ellison et al., 2004). Fourth, a larger set of studies has reported effects of TMS in FEF or
posterior parietal cortex (PPC) on detection, search, and orienting (Fuggetta et al., 2006;
Grosbras and Paus, 2002; Muggleton et al., 2003; OShea et al., 2004; Taylor et al., 2007; Thut
et al., 2005). Overall, in agreement with the imaging evidence showing that dorsal and ventral
networks are coactivated during target detection and stimulus-driven reorienting (Corbetta et
al., 2002; Giessing et al., 2006; Kincade et al., 2005; Marois et al., 2000), TMS of both ventral
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
We have reported direct evidence for an interaction between the two networks in fMRI studies
of stroke patients with spatial neglect. Spatial neglect is a syndrome characterized by a bias to
attend and respond to objects on the contralesional side and is observed more frequently after
right than left hemisphere strokes (Heilman et al., 1987b; Mesulam, 1999). Lesions that cause
neglect are typically localized in ventral frontal or temporoparietal cortex and underlying white
matter (Husain and Kennard, 1996; Karnath et al., 2004; Mort et al., 2003; Vallar and Perani,
1987). We recently demonstrated that the spatial bias of neglect depends on a physiological
imbalance between left and right dorsal parietal cortex (IPS/SPL), which is caused by structural
and physiological abnormalities in the ventral attention network (Corbetta et al., 2005; He et
al., 2007a). The inter-hemispheric imbalance in IPS/SPL is evident both during spatial attention
tasks, with a significant relationship between left-side neglect and hyperactivation of left
parietal cortex, and in measures of functional connectivity at rest. For instance, Figure 6A
shows BOLD time series collected from a stroke patient who suffered extensive damage to
inferior frontal, perisylvian, and TPJ cortex and showed severe left-side neglect at the acute
stage. The time series clearly show abnormal correlation of the resting BOLD signal between
left and right IPS, which is not structurally damaged. This deficit correlates across subjects
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
with the severity of neglect and recovers over 9 months as neglect improves (He et al.,
2007a). Interestingly, the degree of functional impairment in dorsal parietal cortex correlates
with the degree of impaired functional connectivity in the structurally damaged ventral
network, hence demonstrating the interaction between the two networks. Notably, this
interaction involved right MFG and the white matter fibers connecting this region to dorsal
parietal cortex (He et al., 2007a), providing more support for the hypothesis that right MFG
links ventral and dorsal systems (Figure 2B).
Finally, a recent paper used a Granger Causality analysis to show an influence of ventral activity
on dorsal activity when healthy subjects passively listened to a movement from a symphony
(Sridharan et al., 2007) (Figure 6B), consistent with an interaction between the networks.
Completion of the movement activated both networks, but the ventral activation preceded the
dorsal activation (Sridharan et al., 2007). The authors suggested that the ventral network
activity marked an event boundary and influenced dorsal activity during a subsequent updating
of working memory.
In summary, TMS, neuroimaging, and lesion evidence support the hypothesis that ventral and
dorsal networks are both necessary and interact when attention is reoriented to behaviorally
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
different feature(s) than the standard, rather than by a different location (see Marois et al.,
2000, for a comparison of the two cases), the enhancement to the oddball is not related to a
spatial shift of attention.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
But the oddball paradigm combines a range of processes, making the fMRI activations difficult
to interpret. For example, a spatial or feature cue in a typical visual attention task may indicate
what object should be attended (e.g., attend to the red letter) (Broadbent, 1971; Bundesen,
1990) but not how the object should be categorized or responded to (e.g., if the letter is a
vowel, press the left key), restricting the relevant processes to those involved in stimulus
selection (Logan and Gordon, 2001). In the oddball paradigm, however, the oddball/standard
distinction indicates what response should be made, adding processes involved in categorizing
the oddball, selecting a response (whether overt or covert, go or no-go) based on the current
stimulus-response mapping, making the response, and generating signals related to
performance monitoring.
Several other studies suggest that the ventral network marks transitions when one behavior is
interrupted or terminated and a new behavior begins, including transitions at event boundaries
(for a general discussion of event boundaries, see Zacks et al., 2007). A similar phenomenon
appears to occur during the transition between a period of rest and a task block involving many
trials (task onset) or the transition from the task block to rest (task offset). Both block onsets
and offsets robustly and transiently activate R TPJ and VFC, but also other regions, including
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
dorsal prefrontal cortex and the dorsal attention network (Dosenbach et al., 2006; Fox et al.,
2005a; Konishi et al., 2001) (Figure 7A). Even within a single trial, coactivation of dorsal and
ventral frontoparietal areas at task offset may index a readjustment or interruption of ongoing
task sets (Shulman et al., 2002). Interestingly, in this latter study, the transient signal at task
transition occurred both in dorsal frontoparietal areas that were engaged prior to the transition,
but only at the transition point in the ventral network. One interpretation is that the ventral
network signals the task transition and/or provides a reset signal. As discussed below, it is
possible that these cortical reset signals are related to similar signals identified in the LC-NE
system, which putatively allows for a shift of cortical architecture at task boundaries (Bouret
and Sara, 2005).
Overall, the above studies suggest that, whenever environmental stimuli call for a change in a
maintained task, ventral (and dorsal) attention networks are modulated at the transition point.
Interestingly, the ventral network is not recruited when people regularly switch from one task
to another over short time periods (e.g., task-switching paradigms). This form of task control
appears to involve a separate set of dorsal parietal and frontal regions (Brass and von Cramon,
2004; Braver et al., 2003; Kimberg et al., 2000; Rushworth et al., 2002).
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
mediate a number of default processes to which the brain returns in the absence of a task
(Raichle et al., 2001). A similar set of regions show high temporal correlation in resting-state
fcMRI (Fox et al., 2005b; Greicius et al., 2003).
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Some authors have proposed that default and dorsal attention networks represent two
fundamental axes of functional organization in the brain, with the dorsal attention network
controlling environmentally directed processes (e.g., perception and action) and the default
network controlling internally directed processes (e.g., memory, introspection) (Fox et al.,
2005b; Golland et al., 2007). This hypothesis is based on the observation that goal-directed
tasks activate the dorsal attention network and deactivate the default network. Moreover,
several fcMRI studies have reported that default activity is negatively correlated with the dorsal
network (see top panel of Figure 3; (Fox et al., 2006a; Fransson, 2005; but see Golland et al.,
2007; Nir et al., 2006). Finally, during natural vision, the posterior part of the brain is entirely
occupied either by regions that are positively correlated with the dorsal attention network or
the default network (Golland et al., 2007; Nir et al., 2006).
The hypothesis that the ventral network may function as a system to switch (reorient) between
internally and externally directed activities is based on two sets of observations. First, the
ventral network is largely segregated in terms of functional connectivity from both dorsal
attention and default networks (see Figure 3; Fox et al., 2006a). The independence in the resting
state of the ventral network from both dorsal and default networks may allow a flexible
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
interaction during externally or internally directed behavior. Second, although both ventral
attention and default systems may deactivate during goal-oriented behavior, the deactivations
depend on different factors. During a perceptual task, in which subjects monitored an RSVP
stream for a single target (Shulman et al., 2003), the TPJ component of the ventral network
was deactivated only while subjects searched the stream for the target (Figure 5A), but the
angular gyrus component of the default network was deactivated as long as the RSVP stream
remained on the screen. In other words, TPJ was deactivated by the attentional component,
while the angular gyrus was deactivated by the sensory component of the task. Moreover, the
presentation of the attended target activated TPJ but not angular gyrus (see Golland et al.,
2007, for a different dissociation between anterior and posterior portions of IPL). In contrast,
when subjects searched their episodic memory for an item (an internally directed task), both
sets of regions were still deactivated, but only the angular gyrus was then activated by a positive
match in memory (Shannon and Buckner, 2004;Wheeler and Buckner, 2004). The similarity
of response profile when looking for a target in the environment or in memory raises the
possibility that the ventral attention network plays a similar role in both processes. In both
cases, filtering of the ventral attention network is necessary to protect the system from
involuntarily reorienting to environmental stimuli when resources are allocated to perceptual,
memory, or self-referential processing.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
A recent study reported that ToM activations, measured by comparing responses to false-belief
stories and control stories involving outdated photographs, colocalized with activations from
reorienting to invalid targets in a Posner cueing task (Mitchell, 2007) (Figure 7C). Figure 7B
shows the close correspondence between activations during attentional reorienting and social
cognition in R TPJ from a recent meta-analysis (Decety and Lamm, 2007), although there was
a tendency for the social cognition activations to extend slightly more posteriorly, perhaps into
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Colocalization of activations from ToM and reorienting paradigms does not necessarily imply
a common process. First, the colocalization, while impressive, is only approximate. In addition
to the fact that fMRI activity averages over large cell populations, there may be a slightly more
posterior distribution for ToM activations. To our knowledge, the VFC component of the
ventral attention network has not been reported in studies of social cognition. Instead, social
cognition paradigms often activate, in addition to TPJ, foci in posterior cingulate and medial
prefrontal cortex that belong to the default network, which we argued above is distinct from
the ventral attention network. Perhaps a slightly more posterior location for the TPJ focus in
some ToM paradigms reflects connectivity with these default regions. Second, colocalization
may mask subtle but systematic differences in the voxelwise distributions of the activations
(Downing et al., 2007). Demonstrating that two voxelwise patterns or distributions are not
identical, however, begs the question of why both patterns occur in the same cortical tissue.
Although in principle the two distributions could reflect completely unrelated functions that
are juxtaposed, i.e., a specialized ToM module (Saxe and Powell, 2006) and a node within a
reorienting network (Corbetta and Shulman, 2002), the close anatomical correspondence may
suggest a less arbitrary relationship.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
If activations from reorienting and ToM are not completely unrelated, why might they be
linked? First, colocalization might reflect factors that are poorly controlled in either or both
paradigms. For example, ToM paradigms generally involve blocks or trials in which subjects
comprehend animations, movie sequences, or stories over an extended period. The cognitive
or working memory loads of the experimental and control stories in these ToM paradigms have
not been explicitly controlled. In several studies, the selective activation of right TPJ during
ToM conditions as compared to control conditions actually reflected a lesser deactivation (e.g.,
Figure 7C from Mitchell, 2007). Because greater memory loads produce stronger TPJ
deactivations (Todd et al., 2005), differential TPJ activity in experimental and control
conditions of ToM paradigms could reflect overall differences in memory load or task
complexity. The consistency of R TPJ activations in ToM and reorienting experiments across
very different paradigms, however, suggests that any single methodological factor may not
explain the colocalization.
Second, colocalization might reflect cognitive processes that are present in both paradigms.
For example, both reorienting and ToM paradigms often involve breaches of expectation (e.g.,
invalid cues [Arrington et al., 2000; Corbetta et al., 2000; Macaluso et al., 2002] or false-belief
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
stories [Gallagher and Frith, 2003; Vogeley et al., 2001]), which appear to modulate the ventral
network. Decety and Lamm (2007) suggest that many aspects of social cognition involve a
comparison of internal predictions with actual external events, explaining the ubiquitous
presence of R TPJ activity. However, some ToM studies have included controls for this factor
(Saxe et al., 2004), and some ToM and reorienting studies have not involved manipulations of
expectation (Saxe and Powell, 2006; Serences et al., 2005).
Another possibility along these lines is that TPJ activity during ToM tasks reflects signals
linked to shifts in eye gaze or for perception or imagery of gaze. Several studies have shown
that posterior STS is activated during the perception of gaze shifts (Allison et al., 2000;
Pelphrey et al., 2003, 2004). Within a social context, activation from viewing-gaze shifts are
larger when they occur toward the viewer (mutual gaze) than when they occur away from the
viewer (averted gaze) (Figure 7D). This error signal may reflect a mismatch between our
expectation and the observed direction of another persons gaze (similar to an invalidly cued
target) or an error signal in the inferred state of mind of the other person (a ToM signal). In
general, strong evolutionary reasons link mechanisms for shifting attention to the development
of social mechanisms for conspecific interactions in old and new world monkeys to
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
mechanisms to infer others intention or ToM in higher apes and humans (Tomasello et al.,
2001). Gaze-related activations in STS, however, may not colocalize with those for reorienting
or ToM (these functions have not been assessed within the same experiment) (see Gobbini et
al., 2007, for a recent meta-analysis).
the relationship between reorienting signals in the ventral attention network and sense of body
remains to be explored, an intriguing hypothesis is that similar environmental and bodily
representations and their comparison may be co-opted for ToM interactions and that attention
signals in TPJ may be important to switch between internal, bodily, or self-perspective and
external, environmental, or others viewpoint, a key ingredient of ToM.
frequency (e.g., oddball paradigms), or produce error signals that drive learning, reward, or
affective mechanisms. While, in some cases, violations of expectation may be an essential
feature of the process that drives ventral network activation, it will also be important in future
work to explicitly manipulate stimulus-driven reorienting independently from expectation.
Several neuromodulators have been linked to the detection of unexpected events, including
dopamine and norepinephrine (NE) (Dayan and Yu, 2006). Although dopaminergic responses
to unexpected stimuli are often discussed in the context of reward (Schultz, 1998; Schultz et
al., 1997) some authors have proposed that they more generally facilitate a shift of attention
to unexpected and behaviorally important stimuli (Horvitz, 2000; Redgrave et al., 1999; Zink
et al., 2003). This putative function is very similar to that proposed for the ventral attention
network, but there is no evidence of a significant dopaminergic projection to TPJ. In contrast,
there is evidence in monkey for a strong noradrenergic innervation of inferior parietal cortex
and superior temporal gyrus, possible homologs of human TPJ (Foote and Morrison, 1987;
Morrison and Foote, 1986). Therefore, we next consider the functional relationship between
the ventral attention network and activity in the locus coeruleus (LC), the primary source of
NE.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
LC neurons exhibit both tonic and phasic activity modes. Tonic activity is low in an unaroused
state that facilitates sleep and disengagement from the environment (Aston-Jones and Bloom,
1981; Rajkowski et al., 1994), moderate when the organism is engaged in a focused task of
high utility and filters out irrelevant stimuli (Usher et al., 1999), and high when the organism
is not committed to a task, is exploring the environment, and there is uncertainty concerning
the proper relationship between stimuli and responses (Aston-Jones et al., 1997) (i.e.,
unexpected uncertainty). Although these transitions in tonic firing of LC neurons occur over
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
seconds or minutes, decrements of tonic LC activity have been observed on a shorter timescale
in the period between a warning cue instructing the onset of a trial and a rewarded target
stimulus (Bouret and Sara, 2005). Aston-Jones and Cohen have proposed that LC-NE tonic
signals enable transitions between behavioral states (sleep, focused alert, exploratory) and that
the decrement of tonic activity from an exploratory state to a specific task state reflects the
higher utility associated with the detection of upcoming target stimuli. Accordingly, transitions
between different tonic levels are enabled by cortical inputs from prefrontal regions (anterior
cingulate, orbitofrontal cortex) that heavily project to LC and are sensitive to task context and
reward information.
The second component of LC discharge is the phasic response observed to target stimuli, which
is most strongly generated in the moderate tonic task-focused mode. Interestingly, phasic
responses of LC neurons share many similarities with the P300 target-related cortical evoked
potential, which was previously discussed in relation to the timing of the response in the ventral
attention network (Aston-Jones and Cohen, 2005; Nieuwenhuis et al., 2005). Two different yet
related theories have been proposed to explain the putative function of the LC phasic response
to targets. According to Aston-Jones and Cohen, the phasic response enhances the gain of
neural responses in the complex neural matrix involving sensory, decision, and motor regions
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
and therefore speeds up behavioral responses. Importantly, the LC phasic response is thought
to be triggered by pre-frontal inputs only after the sensory evidence for a target has exceeded
a decision threshold in the relevant cortical network, i.e., it is a relatively late postdecision
signal that restricts LC activity to target stimuli (Clayton et al., 2004; Rajkowski et al., 2004),
consistent with the relatively late P300 response to target detection. Alternatively, the phasic
signal has been conceptualized as an interrupt signal (Dayan and Yu, 2006) or as a network
reset signal (Bouret and Sara, 2005) that allows the flexible configuration of a target network
once a target is detected. Bouret and Sara note that this interpretation is consistent with the role
that norepinephrine plays in much simpler organisms. For instance, in the stomatogastric
nervous system of crustacea, synchronized activity from a small number of neuromodulatory
cells can construct ex novo a functional network from neurons otherwise belonging to a
different functional network (Marder and Thirumalai, 2002; Meyrand et al., 1994; Simmers et
al., 1995). The phylogenetic stability of norepinephrine systems from crustaceans to humans
is a powerful argument for stability of function. The Aston-Jones/Cohen theory of the phasic
LC-NE signal is not necessarily inconsistent with this idea, because the authors note that the
phasic signal effectively reconfigures the target cortical network from a multilayer to a single-
layer network following a decision phase but does not capture the network reset idea.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
We propose a functional relationship between signals of the LC/NE system and activity in the
ventral attention network, both in relation to behavioral transitions (tonic signals) and target
detection (phasic response). The decrease in tonic LC activity during the transition from an
exploratory state to a task-focused state may parallel the deactivation of TPJ, relative to rest,
when subjects engage in a demanding task (Shulman et al., 2003; Todd et al., 2005) (Figure
8). The Aston-Jones/Cohen theory maintains that a decrease in the tonic level of LC activity
promotes engagement on the current task and filtering of distracters, similar to the hypothesis
that the ventral attention network is deactivated under demanding conditions, reflecting a top-
down filtering signal that restricts the network response to a narrow range of task-relevant
stimuli (targets or contingent distracters). Conversely, the hypothesized broad sensitivity to
environmental stimuli during the high tonic activity/exploratory LC mode in the Aston-Jones/
Cohen model may correspond to the ability of any salient stimulus to activate TPJ during
passive viewing or no-task states (Downar et al., 2000). We reviewed evidence above that the
sources of top-down filtering signals into the ventral network are either the dorsal network
through prefrontal cortex (Figures 5C and 2B) or prefrontal regions (anterior cingulate, frontal
operculum; Figure 5C) directly or indirectly through their projection via LC. On the output
side, LC-NE neurons densely projects to inferior parietal cortex and superior temporal gyrus,
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
possible homologs of human TPJ (Foote and Morrison, 1987; Morrison and Foote, 1986).
Therefore, as shown in Figure 8, the deactivation of the ventral attention network during
focused attention may be partly caused by a decrement of tonic activity in the LC/NE system.
There is also a striking similarity between the target-related response in the ventral network,
P300 potentials, and the phasic response in the LC (Table 1). All three (ventral network, P300,
LC neurons) show enhanced responses to behaviorally relevant stimuli (targets) in multiple
modalities, relative to distracters, and an enhanced response to low-frequency targets.
Detection of unattended targets (i.e., invalid targets in the Posner cueing paradigm) enhances
both TPJ activity and the amplitude of a late positive potential that may correspond to P300
(Mangun and Hillyard, 1991), while stimuli of high emotional valence modulate P300 and LC
activity. On the response output side, TPJ activity, P300, and LC activity are relatively
independent of response parameters (Astafiev et al., 2006;Clayton et al., 2004;McCarthy and
Donchin, 1981). Finally, both P300 and LC activity can be anatomically linked to TPJ. Lesions
of different parts of the ventral attention network affect different components of P300, with
TPJ damage decreasing both target- and novel-evoked P300 components and prefrontal lesions
affecting the novelty response (Yamaguchi and Knight, 1991b;Verleger et al., 1994;Daffner
et al., 2000). A recent study showed that oddball target responses in TPJ and prefrontal cortex
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
were abolished by propranolol, a -adrenergic blocker drug (Strange and Dolan, 2007).
These physiological similarities point to similar functions. The hypothesis that the ventral
attention network is involved in reorienting from one task state to another, either in the
environment or between internally and externally directed activities, is very close to the
network-reset hypothesis of Bouret and Sarah. A network reset or interrupt hypothesis captures
the sensitivity of the ventral attention network to task transitions or unexpected events that may
require the dorsal network to be reconfigured (as in Figures 6B and 7A). Under these conditions,
activity in the dorsal network reflects the reconfiguration of task processes (stimulus and motor
representations) in response to the new contingency, while activity in the ventral network
facilitates rather than initiates this reset or reconfiguration process. We have already discussed
that activity in the ventral network, as indexed by the P300, may not be sufficiently fast to
initiate a reorienting response. A similar argument applies to the LC-NE system, which has a
relatively long latency to a stimulus (~100150 ms) and a slow transmission of its output to
the cortex (~50100 ms). In the context of a nonlinear dynamic system, the highly synchronized
LC-NE activation of the ventral network may allow the dorsal network to switch to or settle
into another state more appropriate for the new environmental situation (Serences and Yantis,
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
2006).
While the adaptive-gain theory of Aston-Jones and Cohen is concerned with the role of LC/
NE activity in categorization and responding to attended targets, an interrupt/reset/reorienting
framework includes other situations discussed above, such as stimulus-driven shifts of
attention, transitions between rest and an extended task period, and detection of event
boundaries.
The disruption of a reset signal may impair shifting between objects or events in the
environment and thus underlie nonlateralized attentional impairments after damage of ventral
frontal and temporoparietal cortex (Husain and Rorden, 2003), such as poorer detection or
identification of targets in both visual fields (Duncan et al., 1999; He et al., 2007a; Peers et al.,
2005), problems with vigilance (Heilman et al., 1987b; Robertson, 2001; Wilkins et al.,
1987), and an extended attentional blink (Husain et al., 1997; Shapiro et al., 2002). Moreveor,
impaired interactions between the ventral and dorsal attention network (Corbetta et al., 2005;
He et al., 2007a) produce activity imbalances in parietal spatial maps that result in a tonic
attentional bias toward the ipsilesional field. Transient increases in vigilance improve spatial
attention and perception (Robertson, 2001; Robertson et al., 1998), presumably through an
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
augmentation of LC-NE output that leads to a more normal interaction between the two
networks.
Future Directions
This review of the function of the ventral attention network suggests several novel avenues for
future investigation. It is important to know the timing of the activation of ventral and dorsal
networks on timescales that are closer to the underlying neural signals and whether temporal
codes such as synchronization and coherence link widely separate neuronal populations during
selection and behavioral reorienting. The recent combination of fMRI and EEG/MEG methods,
as well as the integration of TMS/fMRI and EEG, should provide important information on
timing and causal interactions between areas. Also, the evolutionary precursors of the ventral
attention network and its right hemisphere lateralization could be uncovered by neuroimaging
and single-unit studies of primates. An ongoing and critical issue is the relationship between
different attentional functions and neuromodulatory systems, especially noradrenaline,
acetylcholine, and dopamine, for which there is already strong evidence of a role in attention
and learning. Finally, further exploration into human pathologies, both focal (e.g., stroke) and
nonfocal (e.g., traumatic brain injuries, attention-deficit disorders), using cognitive
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
neuroscience models of attention, may lead to a better theory of these debilitating conditions.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the J.S. McDonnell Foundation, the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and
Stroke (R01 NS48013), the National Institute of Mental Health (R01 MH71920-06), and a Marie Curie Chair European
Union (MEXC-CT-2004-006783). We thank Jan De Fockert, Emiliano Macaluso, John Serences, Ren Marois, Vinod
Menon, Devarajan Sridharan, Nikos Dosenbach, Steve Petersen, Jean Decety, Jason Mitchell, Kevin A. Pelphrey, and
Gary Aston-Jones for generously providing illustration of their results. We would like also to thank James Bisley,
Christos Constantidinis, Ron Mangun, and Joe Hopfinger for helpful discussions.
References
Allison T, Puce A, McCarthy G. Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends Cogn
Sci 2000;4:267278. [PubMed: 10859571]
Arrington CM, Carr TH, Mayer AR, Rao SM. Neural mechanisms of visual attention: object-based
selection of a region in space. J Cogn Neurosci 2000;12:106117. [PubMed: 11506651]
Arzy S, Thut G, Mohr C, Michel CM, Blanke O. Neural basis of embodiment: distinct contributions of
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Beauchamp MS, Petit L, Ellmore TM, Ingeholm J, Haxby JV. A parametric fMRI study of overt and
covert shifts of visuospatial attention. Neuroimage 2001;14:310321. [PubMed: 11467905]
Bell AH, Fecteau JH, Munoz DP. Using auditory and visual stimuli to investigate the behavioral and
neuronal consequences of reflexive covert orienting. J Neurophysiol 2004;91:21722184. [PubMed:
14702335]
Binder JR, Frost JA, Hammeke TA, Bellgowan PS, Rao SM, Cox RW. Conceptual processing during
the conscious resting state. A functional MRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 1999;11:8095. [PubMed:
9950716]
Bisley JW, Krishna BS, Goldberg ME. A rapid and precise on-response in posterior parietal cortex. J
Neurosci 2004;24:18331838. [PubMed: 14985423]
Biswal B, Yetkin F, Haughton V, Hyde J. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human
brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med 1995;34:537541. [PubMed: 8524021]
Blanke O, Arzy S. The out-of-body experience: disturbed self-processing at the temporoparietal junction.
Neuroscientist 2005;11:1624. [PubMed: 15632275]
Bledowski C, Prvulovic D, Goebel R, Zanella FE, Linden DE. Attentional systems in target and distractor
processing: a combined ERP and fMRI study. Neuroimage 2004;22:530540. [PubMed: 15193581]
Bouret S, Sara SJ. Network reset: a simplified overarching theory of locus coeruleus noradrenaline
function. Trends Neurosci 2005;28:574582. [PubMed: 16165227]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Brass M, von Cramon DY. Decomposing components of task preparation with functional magnetic
resonance imaging. J Cogn Neurosci 2004;16:609620. [PubMed: 15165351]
Braver TS, Barch DM, Gray JR, Molfese DL, Snyder A. Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict:
effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. Cereb Cortex 2001;11:825836. [PubMed: 11532888]
Braver TS, Reynolds JR, Donaldson DI. Neural mechanisms of transient and sustained cognitive control
during task switching. Neuron 2003;39:713726. [PubMed: 12925284]
Broadbent, DE. Decision and Stress. London: Academic Press; 1971.
Buckner RL, Carroll DC. Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn Sci 2007;11:4957. [PubMed:
17188554]
Bundesen C. A theory of visual attention. Psychol Rev 1990;97:523547. [PubMed: 2247540]
Chambers CD, Payne JM, Stokes MG, Mattingley JB. Fast and slow parietal pathways mediate spatial
attention. Nat Neurosci 2004a;7:217218. [PubMed: 14983182]
Chambers CD, Stokes MG, Mattingley JB. Modality-specific control of strategic spatial attention in
parietal cortex. Neuron 2004b;44:925930. [PubMed: 15603736]
Chao LL, Knight RT. Human prefrontal lesions increase distractibility to irrelevant sensory inputs.
Neuroreport 1995;6:16051610. [PubMed: 8527724]
Clayton EC, Rajkowski J, Cohen JD, Aston-Jones G. Phasic activation of monkey locus ceruleus neurons
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Desimone R, Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci
1995;18:193222. [PubMed: 7605061]
Dosenbach NU, Visscher KM, Palmer ED, Miezin FM, Wenger KK, Kang HC, Burgund ED, Grimes
AL, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. A core system for the implementation of task sets. Neuron
2006;50:799812. [PubMed: 16731517]
Downar J, Crawley AP, Mikulis DJ, Davis KD. A multimodal cortical network for the detection of
changes in the sensory environment. Nat Neurosci 2000;3:277283. [PubMed: 10700261]
Downar J, Crawley AP, Mikulis DJ, Davis KD. The effect of task relevance on the cortical response to
changes in visual and auditory stimuli: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 2001;14:1256
1267. [PubMed: 11707082]
Downing PE. Interactions between visual working memory and selective attention. Psychol Sci
2000;11:467473. [PubMed: 11202491]
Downing PE, Wiggett AJ, Peelen MV. Functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of
overlapping lateral occipitotemporal activations using multi-voxel pattern analysis. J Neurosci
2007;27:226233. [PubMed: 17202490]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Driver J, Spence C. Cross-modal links in spatial attention. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
1998;353:13191331. [PubMed: 9770225]
Duncan J, Bundesen C, Olson A, Humphreys G, Chavda S, Shibuya H. Systematic analysis of deficits
in visual attention. J Exp Psychol Gen 1999;128:450478. [PubMed: 10650583]
Ellison A, Schindler I, Pattison LL, Milner AD. An exploration of the role of the superior temporal gyrus
in visual search and spatial perception using TMS. Brain 2004;127:23072315. [PubMed: 15292055]
Eriksen CW, Hoffman JE. Selective attention: noise suppression or signal enhancement? Bull Psychon
Soc 1974;4:587589.
Evdokimidis I, Smyrnis N, Constantinidis TS, Gourtzelidis P, Papageorgiou C. Frontal-parietal activation
differences observed before the execution of remembered saccades: an event-related potentials study.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2001;12:8999. [PubMed: 11489612]
Fecteau JH, Bell AH, Munoz DP. Neural correlates of the automatic and goal-driven biases in orienting
spatial attention. J Neurophysiol 2004;92:17281737. [PubMed: 15115792]
Fletcher PC, Happe F, Frith U, Baker SC, Dolan RJ, Frackowiak RS, Frith CD. Other minds in the brain:
a functional imaging study of theory of mind in story comprehension. Cognition 1995;57:109
128. [PubMed: 8556839]
Folk CL, Remington RW, Johnston JC. Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control
settings. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 1992;18:10301044. [PubMed: 1431742]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Foote SL, Morrison JH. Extrathalamic modulation of cortical function. Annu Rev Neurosci 1987;10:67
96. [PubMed: 3551766]
Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Barch DM, Gusnard DA, Raichle ME. Transient BOLD responses at block
transitions. Neuroimage 2005a;28:956966. [PubMed: 16043368]
Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is
intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
2005b;102:96739678. [PubMed: 15976020]
Fox MD, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME. Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes
human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2006a;103:1004610051.
[PubMed: 16788060]
Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Zacks JM, Raichle ME. Coherent spontaneous activity accounts for trial-to-trial
variability in human evoked brain responses. Nat Neurosci 2006b;9:2325. [PubMed: 16341210]
Fransson P. Spontaneous low-frequency BOLD signal fluctuations: an fMRI investigation of the resting-
state default mode of brain function hypothesis. Hum Brain Mapp 2005;26:1529. [PubMed:
15852468]
Fuggetta G, Pavone EF, Walsh V, Kiss M, Eimer M. Cortico-cortical interactions in spatial attention: A
combined ERP/TMS study. J Neurophysiol 2006;95:32773280. [PubMed: 16436477]
Gallagher HL, Frith CD. Functional imaging of theory of mind. Trends Cogn Sci 2003;7:7783.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
[PubMed: 12584026]
Gibson BS, Kelsey EM. Stimulus-driven attentional capture is contingent on attentional set for
displaywide visual features. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 1998;24:699706. [PubMed:
9627409]
Giesbrecht B, Weissman DH, Woldorff MG, Mangun GR. Pre-target activity in visual cortex predicts
behavioral performance on spatial and feature attention tasks. Brain Res 2006;1080:6372. [PubMed:
16412994]
Giessing C, Thiel CM, Rosler F, Fink GR. The modulatory effects of nicotine on parietal cortex activity
in a cued target detection task depend on cue reliability. Neuroscience 2006;137:853864. [PubMed:
16309846]
Gobbini MI, Koralek AC, Bryan RE, Montgomery KJ, Haxby JV. Two takes on the social brain: a
comparison of theory of mind tasks. J Cogn Neurosci 2007;19:18031814. [PubMed: 17958483]
Golland Y, Bentin S, Gelbard H, Benjamini Y, Heller R, Nir Y, Hasson U, Malach R. Extrinsic and
intrinsic systems in the posterior cortex of the human brain revealed during natural sensory
stimulation. Cereb Cortex 2007;17:766777. [PubMed: 16699080]
Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a nework
analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2003;100:253258. [PubMed:
12506194]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Grosbras MH, Paus T. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field: effects on visual
perception and attention. J Cogn Neurosci 2002;14:11091120. [PubMed: 12419133]
Hagler DJ Jr, Sereno MI. Spatial maps in frontal and prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 2006;29:567577.
[PubMed: 16289928]
Hampshire A, Duncan J, Owen AM. Selective tuning of the blood oxygenation level-dependent response
during simple target detection dissociates human frontoparietal subregions. J Neurosci
2007;27:62196223. [PubMed: 17553994]
Hampson M, Peterson BS, Skudlarski P, Gatenby JC, Gore JC. Detection of functional connectivity using
temporal correlations in MR images. Hum Brain Mapp 2002;15:247262. [PubMed: 11835612]
Hampson M, Tokoglu F, Sun Z, Schafer RJ, Skudlarski P, Gore JC, Constable RT. Connectivity-behavior
analysis reveals that functional connectivity between left BA39 and Brocas area varies with reading
ability. Neuroimage 2006;31:513519. [PubMed: 16497520]
He BJ, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Epstein A, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. Breakdown of functional
connectivity in frontoparietal networks underlies behavioral deficits in spatial neglect. Neuron 2007a;
53:905918. [PubMed: 17359924]
He BJ, Shulman GL, Snyder AZ, Corbetta M. The role of impaired neuronal communication in
neurological disorders. Curr Opin Neurol 2007b;20:655660. [PubMed: 17992085]
Heilman, KM.; Watson, RT.; Valenstein, E.; Goldberg, ME. Attention: behaviour and neural
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
mechanisms. In: Plum, F.; Mountcastle, VB.; Geiger, ST., editors. The Handbook of Physiology
Section 1: The Nervous System Volume V Higher Functions of the Brain Part 2. Bethesda, MD:
American Physiological Society; 1987b. p. 461-481.
Hommel, B. The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation. In: Monsell,
S.; Driver, J., editors. Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance 18. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press; 2000. p. 247-273.
Hopfinger JB, Ries AJ. Automatic versus contingent mechanisms of sensory-driven neural biasing and
reflexive attention. J Cogn Neurosci 2005;17:13411352. [PubMed: 16197688]
Hopfinger JB, Buonocore MH, Mangun GR. The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control.
Nat Neurosci 2000;3:284291. [PubMed: 10700262]
Horvitz JC. Mesolimbocortical and nigrostriatal dopamine responses to salient non-reward events.
Neuroscience 2000;96:651656. [PubMed: 10727783]
Husain M, Kennard C. Visual neglect associated with frontal lobe infarction. J Neurol 1996;243:652
657. [PubMed: 8892067]
Husain M, Rorden C. Non-spatially lateralized mechanisms in hemispatial neglect. Nat Rev Neurosci
2003;4:2636. [PubMed: 12511859]
Husain M, Shapiro K, Martin J, Kennard C. Abnormal temporal dynamics of visual attention in spatial
neglect patients. Nature 1997;385:154156. [PubMed: 8990117]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Indovina I, Macaluso E. Dissociation of stimulus relevance and saliency factors during shifts of
visuospatial attention. Cereb Cortex 2007;17:17011711. [PubMed: 17003078]
Jack AI, Patel GH, Astafiev SV, Snyder AZ, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. Changing human
visual field organization from early visual to extra-occipital cortex. PLoS ONE 2007;2:e452.
[PubMed: 17505546]
Jonides, J. Voluntary vs. automatic control over the minds eyes movement. In: Posner, MI.; Marin, O.,
editors. Attention and Performance. XI. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers;
1981. p. 187-205.
Jonides J, Yantis S. Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention. Perception and
Psychophysics 1988;43:346354. [PubMed: 3362663]
Karnath HO, Fruhmann Berger M, Kuker W, Rorden C. The anatomy of spatial neglect based on
voxelwise statistical analysis: a study of 140 patients. Cereb Cortex 2004;14:11641172. [PubMed:
15142954]
Kastner S, Pinsk MA, De Weerd P, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG. Increased activity in human visual
cortex during directed attention in the absence of visual stimulation. Neuron 1999;22:751761.
[PubMed: 10230795]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Kiehl KA, Laurens KR, Duty TL, Forster BB, Liddle PF. Neural sources involved in auditory target
detection and novelty processing: an event-related fMRI study. Psychophysiology 2001;38:133142.
[PubMed: 11321614]
Kim YH, Gitelman DR, Nobre AC, Parrish TB, LaBar KS, Mesulam MM. The large-scale neural network
for spatial attention displays multifunctional overlap but differential asymmetry. Neuroimage
1999;9:269277. [PubMed: 10075897]
Kimberg DY, Aguirre GK, DEsposito M. Modulation of task-related neural activity in task-switching:
an fMRI study. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2000;10:189196. [PubMed: 10978708]
Kincade JM, Abrams RA, Astafiev SV, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. An event-related functional magnetic
resonance imaging study of voluntary and stimulus-driven orienting of attention. J Neurosci
2005;25:45934604. [PubMed: 15872107]
Knight RT, Scabini D, Woods DL, Clayworth CC. Contributions of temporal-parietal junction to the
human auditory P3. Brain Res 1989;502:109116. [PubMed: 2819449]
Konishi S, Donaldson DI, Buckner RL. Transient activation during block transition. Neuroimage
2001;13:364374. [PubMed: 11162276]
LaBar KS, Gitelman DR, Parrish TB, Mesulam M. Neuroan-atomic overlap of working memory and
spatial attention networks: a functional MRI comparison within subjects. Neuroimage 1999;10:695
704. [PubMed: 10600415]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Lenggenhager B, Tadi T, Metzinger T, Blanke O. Video ergo sum: manipulating bodily self-
consciousness. Science 2007;317:10961099. [PubMed: 17717189]
Lepsien J, Pollmann S. Covert reorienting and inhibition of return: an event-related fMRI study. J Cogn
Neurosci 2002;14:127144. [PubMed: 11970781]
Linden DEJ, Prvulovic D, Formisano E, Vollinger M, Zanella FE, Goebel R, Dierks T. The functional
neuroanatomy of target detection: an fMRI study of visual and auditory oddball tasks. Cereb Cortex
1999;9:815823. [PubMed: 10601000]
Logan GD, Gordon RD. Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychol Rev
2001;108:393434. [PubMed: 11381835]
Luck SJ, Hillyard SA, Mouloua M, Woldorff MG, Clark VP, Hawkins HL. Effects of spatial cuing on
luminance detectability: psychophysical and electrophysiological evidence for early selection. J
Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 1994;20:887904. [PubMed: 8083642]
Macaluso E, Patria F. Spatial re-orienting of visual attention along the horizontal or the vertical axis. Exp
Brain Res 2007;180:2334. [PubMed: 17262217]
Macaluso E, Frith CD, Driver J. Supramodal effects of covert spatial orienting triggered by visual or
tactile events. J Cogn Neurosci 2002;14:389401. [PubMed: 11970799]
Mangun GR, Hillyard SA. Modulations of sensory-evoked brain potentials indicate changes in perceptual
processing during visual-spatial priming. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 1991;17:10571074.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
[PubMed: 1837297]
Mantini D, Perrucci MG, Del Gratta C, Romani GL, Corbetta M. Electrophysiological signatures of
resting state networks in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2007;104:1317013175.
[PubMed: 17670949]
Marder E, Thirumalai V. Cellular, synaptic and network effects of neuromodulation. Neural Netw
2002;15:479493. [PubMed: 12371506]
Marois R, Leung HC, Gore JC. A stimulus-driven approach to object identity and location processing in
the human brain. Neuron 2000;25:717728. [PubMed: 10774738]
Mayer AR, Harrington D, Adair JC, Lee R. The neural networks underlying endogenous auditory covert
orienting and reorienting. Neuroimage 2006;30:938949. [PubMed: 16388970]
Mazoyer B, Zago L, Mellet E, Bricogne S, Etard O, Houde O, Crivello F, Joliot M, Petit L, Tzourio-
Mazoyer N. Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious
resting state in man. Brain Res Bull 2001;54:287298. [PubMed: 11287133]
McCarthy G, Donchin E. A metric for thought: a comparison of P300 latency and reaction time. Science
1981;211:7780. [PubMed: 7444452]
McCarthy G, Luby M, Gore J, Goldman-Rakic P. Infrequent events transiently activate human prefrontal
and parietal cortex as measured by functional MRI. J Neurophysiol 1997;77:16301634. [PubMed:
9084626]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
McDowell JE, Kissler JM, Berg P, Dyckman KA, Gao Y, Rockstroh B, Clementz BA.
Electroencephalography/magnetoencephalography study of cortical activities preceding
prosaccades and antisaccades. Neuroreport 2005;16:663668. [PubMed: 15858402]
Meister IG, Wienemann M, Buelte D, Grunewald C, Sparing R, Dambeck N, Boroojerdi B.
Hemiextinction induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation over the right temporoparietal
junction. Neuroscience 2006;142:119123. [PubMed: 16876326]
Menon V, Ford JM, Lim KO, Glover GH, Pfefferbaum A. Combined event-related fMRI and EEG
evidence for temporal-parietal cortex activation during target detection. Neuroreport 1997;8:3029
3037. [PubMed: 9331910]
Mesulam MM. Spatial attention and neglect: parietal, frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental
representation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonal events. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B
Biol Sci 1999;354:13251346. [PubMed: 10466154]
Meyrand P, Simmers J, Moulins M. Dynamic construction of a neural network from multiple pattern
generators in the lobster stomatogastric nervous system. J Neurosci 1994;14:630644. [PubMed:
7507982]
Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci
2001;24:167202. [PubMed: 11283309]
Mitchell JP. Activity in right temporoparietal junction is not selective for theory-of-mind. Cereb Cortex
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
localization of the system for visuo-spatial attention using positron emission tomography. Brain
1997;120:515533. [PubMed: 9126062]
OShea J, Muggleton NG, Cowey A, Walsh V. Timing of target discrimination in human frontal eye
fields. J Cogn Neurosci 2004;16:10601067. [PubMed: 15298792]
Ongur D, Ferry AT, Price JL. Architectonic subdivision of the human orbital and medial prefrontal cortex.
J Comp Neurol 2003;460:425449. [PubMed: 12692859]
Pashler H, Harris CR. Spontaneous allocation of visual attention: Dominant role of uniqueness. Psychon
Bull Rev 2001;8:747752. [PubMed: 11848595]
Peelen MV, Heslenfeld DJ, Theeuwes J. Endogenous and exogenous attention shifts are mediated by the
same large-scale neural network. Neuroimage 2004;22:822830. [PubMed: 15193611]
Peers PV, Ludwig CJ, Rorden C, Cusack R, Bonfiglioli C, Bundesen C, Driver J, Antoun N, Duncan J.
Attentional functions of parietal and frontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 2005;5:14691484. [PubMed:
15689522]
Pelphrey KA, Mitchell TV, McKeown MJ, Goldstein J, Allison T, McCarthy G. Brain activity evoked
by the perception of human walking: controlling for meaningful coherent motion. J Neurosci
2003;23:68196825. [PubMed: 12890776]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Pelphrey KA, Morris JP, McCarthy G. Grasping the intentions of others: the perceived intentionality of
an action influences activity in the superior temporal sulcus during social perception. J Cogn
Neurosci 2004;16:17061716. [PubMed: 15701223]
Pessoa L, Padmala S. Quantitative prediction of perceptual decisions during near-threshold fear detection.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2005;102:56125617. [PubMed: 15800041]
Pessoa L, Gutierrez E, Bandettini P, Ungerleider L. Neural correlates of visual working memory: fMRI
amplitude predicts task performance. Neuron 2002;35:975987. [PubMed: 12372290]
Petersen SE, Robinson DL, Morris JD. Contributions of the pulvinar to visual spatial attention.
Neuropsychologia 1987;25:97105. [PubMed: 3574654]
Posner MI. Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 1980;32:325. [PubMed: 7367577]
Posner, MI.; Cohen, Y. Components of visual orienting. In: Bouma, H.; Bowhuis, D., editors. Attention
and Performance X. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1984. p. 531-556.
Rafal RD, Posner MI, Friedman JH, Inhoff AW, Bernstein E. Orienting of visual attention in progressive
supranuclear palsy. Brain 1988;111:267280. [PubMed: 3378136]
Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. Inaugural Article: A
default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2001;98:676682. [PubMed: 11209064]
Rajkowski J, Kubiak P, Aston-Jones G. Locus coeruleus activity in monkey: phasic and tonic changes
are associated with altered vigilance. Brain Res Bull 1994;35:607616. [PubMed: 7859118]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Ruff CC, Blankenburg F, Bjoertomt O, Bestmann S, Freeman E, Haynes JD, Rees G, Josephs O,
Deichmann R, Driver J. Concurrent TMS-fMRI and psychophysics reveal frontal influences on
human retinotopic visual cortex. Curr Biol 2006;16:14791488. [PubMed: 16890523]
Ruff CC, Bestmann S, Blankenburg F, Bjoertomt O, Josephs O, Weiskopf N, Deichmann R, Driver J.
Distinct causal influences of parietal versus frontal areas on human visual cortex: evidence from
concurrent TMS fMRI. Cereb Cortex 2007;18:817827. [PubMed: 17652468]
Rushworth MF, Paus T, Sipila PK. Attention systems and the organization of the human parietal cortex.
J Neurosci 2001;21:52625271. [PubMed: 11438601]
Rushworth MF, Hadland KA, Paus T, Sipila PK. Role of the human medial frontal cortex in task
switching: a combined fMRI and TMS study. J Neurophysiol 2002;87:25772592. [PubMed:
11976394]
Sapir A, Soroker N, Berger A, Henik A. Inhibition of return in spatial attention: direct evidence for
collicular generation. Nat Neurosci 1999;2:10531054. [PubMed: 10570480]
Sapir A, dAvossa G, McAvoy M, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. Brain signals for spatial attention predict
performance in a motion discrimination task. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2005;102:1781017815.
[PubMed: 16306268]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Saxe R, Powell LJ. Its the thought that counts: specific brain regions for one component of theory of
mind. Psychol Sci 2006;17:692699. [PubMed: 16913952]
Saxe R, Xiao DK, Kovacs G, Perrett DI, Kanwisher N. A region of right posterior superior temporal
sulcus responds to observed intentional actions. Neuropsychologia 2004;42:14351446. [PubMed:
15246282]
Schluppeck D, Glimcher P, Heeger DJ. Topographic organization for delayed saccades in human
posterior parietal cortex. J Neurophysiol 2005;94:13721384. [PubMed: 15817644]
Schultz W. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J Neurophysiol 1998;80:127. [PubMed:
9658025]
Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science 1997;275:1593
1599. [PubMed: 9054347]
Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, Reiss AL, Greicius MD.
Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J
Neurosci 2007;27:23492356. [PubMed: 17329432]
Serences JT, Yantis S. Selective visual attention and perceptual coherence. Trends Cogn Sci 2006;10:38
45. [PubMed: 16318922]
Serences JT, Yantis S, Culberson A, Awh E. Preparatory activity in visual cortex indexes distractor
suppression during covert spatial orienting. J Neurophysiol 2004;92:35383545. [PubMed:
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
15254075]
Serences JT, Shomstein S, Leber AB, Golay X, Egeth HE, Yantis S. Coordination of voluntary and
stimulus-driven attentional control in human cortex. Psychol Sci 2005;16:114122. [PubMed:
15686577]
Sereno MI, Pitzalis S, Martinez A. Mapping of contralateral space in retinotopic coordinates by a parietal
cortical area in humans. Science 2001;294:13501354. [PubMed: 11701930]
Sestieri C, Pizzella V, Cianflone F, Romani GL, Corbetta M. Sequential activation of human oculomotor
centers during planning of visually guided eye movements: a combined fMRI-MEG study. Frontiers
in Human Neuroscience 2008;110.3389/neuro.09/001.2007
Shannon BJ, Buckner RL. Functional-anatomic correlates of memory retrieval that suggest nontraditional
processing roles for multiple distinct regions within posterior parietal cortex. J Neurosci
2004;24:1008410092. [PubMed: 15537877]
Shapiro K, Hillstrom AP, Husain M. Control of visuotemporal attention by inferior parietal and superior
temporal cortex. Curr Biol 2002;12:13201325. [PubMed: 12176361]
Shipp S. The brain circuitry of attention. Trends Cogn Sci 2004;8:223230. [PubMed: 15120681]
Shulman GL, Fiez JA, Corbetta M, Buckner RL, Miezin FM, Raichle ME, Petersen SE. Common blood
flow changes across visual tasks: II Decreases in cerebral cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 1997;9:648663.
Shulman GL, Ollinger JM, Akbudak E, Conturo TE, Snyder AZ, Petersen SE, Corbetta M. Areas involved
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Snow JC, Mattingley JB. Goal-driven selective attention in patients with right hemisphere lesions: how
intact is the ipsilesional field? Brain 2006;129:168181. [PubMed: 16317021]
Sokolov, EN. Perception and the Conditioned Reflex. New York: McMillan; 1963.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Sridharan D, Levitin DJ, Chafe CH, Berger J, Menon V. Neural dynamics of event segmentation in music:
converging evidence for dissociable ventral and dorsal networks. Neuron 2007;55:521532.
[PubMed: 17678862]
Stevens MC, Calhoun VD, Kiehl KA. Hemispheric differences in hemodynamics elicited by auditory
oddball stimuli. Neuroimage 2005;26:782792. [PubMed: 15955488]
Strange BA, Dolan RJ. Beta-adrenergic modulation of oddball responses in humans. Behav Brain Funct
2007;3:29. [PubMed: 17567916]
Sweeney JA, Mintum MA, Kwee S, Wiseman MB, Brown DL, Rosenberg DR, Carl JR. Positron emission
tomography dtudy of voluntary saccadic eye movement and spatial working memory. J
Neurophysiol 1996;75:454468. [PubMed: 8822570]
Sylvester CM, Shulman GL, Jack AI, Corbetta M. Asymmetry of anticipatory activity in visual cortex
predicts the locus of attention and perception. J Neurosci 2007;27:1442414433. [PubMed:
18160650]
Taylor PC, Nobre AC, Rushworth MF. FEF TMS affects visual cortical activity. Cereb Cortex
2007;17:391399. [PubMed: 16525126]
Theeuwes J, Burger R. Attentional control during visual search: the effect of irrelevant singletons. J Exp
Psychol Hum Percept Perform 1998;24:13421353. [PubMed: 9778827]
Thut G, Nietzel A, Pascual-Leone A. Dorsal posterior parietal rTMS affects voluntary orienting of
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Yamaguchi S, Knight RT. P300 generation by novel somato-sensory stimuli. Electroencephalogr Clin
Neurophysiol 1991b;78:5055. [PubMed: 1701715]
Yantis S, Egeth HE. On the distinction betweeen visual salience and stimulus-driven attentional capture.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Figure 2. Definition of Dorsal and Ventral Networks from Activation Data and Putative
Interactions
(Top panel) Results from a meta-analysis of activation data. Regions in blue are consistently
activated by central cues, indicating where a peripheral object will subsequently appear or what
is the feature of an upcoming object. Regions in orange are consistently activated when
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
attention is reoriented to an unexpected but behaviorally relevant object. (Bottom panel) Model
for the interaction of dorsal (blue) and ventral (orange) networks during stimulus-driven
reorienting. Dorsal network regions FEF and IPS send top-down biases to visual areas and via
MFG to the ventral network (filtering signal), restricting ventral activation to behaviorally
important stimuli. IPS-FEF are also important for exogenous orienting. Overall, the dorsal
network coordinates stimulus-response selection. Conversely, when a salient stimulus occurs
during stimulus-driven reorienting, the ventral network sends a reorienting signal to the dorsal
network through MFG.
(A) Exogenous orienting does not activate TPJ. Subjects saw a color singleton (exogenous cue)
or a heterogeneous colored array (neutral cue) followed by a target (cue and cue period in light
blue). Behavioral performance was speeded when the target location matched the singleton
location, even though the cue and target locations were random. The time course of the BOLD
signal shown in the graph indicates that R FEF (see A in the surface-rendered brain) showed
a larger response for exogenous than neutral cues. In contrast, SMG showed a small
deactivation during the cue period, followed by a small activation when the cue period ended
(Kincade et al., 2005).
(B) Salient irrelevant distracters influence the dorsal, not ventral, system. Subjects categorized
the orientation of a line within a singleton shape (the circle). Salient but irrelevant singleton
color distracters that impaired behavioral performance activated dorsal region SPL (see B
in brain) rather than TPJ (de Fockert et al., 2004).
(C) Unattended stimuli only activate TPJ if they are task relevant, not if they are irrelevant,
even though they have high sensory salience. A task-relevant unattended letter activated
angular gyrus and inferior frontal gyrus (see C in brain), but no responses were seen to the
unattended but highly salient checkerboard. The angular gyrus response may reflect the
combined activation of dorsal (IPS/SPL) and TPJ regions (Indovina and Macaluso, 2007).
(D) Distracters only activate TPJ if they share features with a target, indicating a strong effect
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
of task relevance. Subjects identified red foveal letters while ignoring irrelevant peripheral
letters. Peripheral letters that matched the target color interfered with performance and
activated TPJ, while non-target-colored letters had no effect (D in brain) (Serences et al.,
2005).
distracters in the RSVP experiment (see panel [A]), including dorsal attention regions IPS and
FEF (red/orange in surface-rendered brain) but also regions in anterior insula and anterior
cingulate that form a putative task-control network (Dosenbach et al., 2006). These regions
may send top-down signals (see arrows) to the ventral network, which showed sustained
deactivations during search (blue/green in surface-rendered brain), restricting its input to task-
relevant objects (Shulman et al., 2003).
Figure 7. Common Activation of TPJ during Reorienting, Task Transitions, and Social Cognition
(A) Regions in green show transient activity at the transition between a rest period and the
onset of an extended block of trials (see time course in inset, indicating that both onsets and
offsets are often observed). Start cue activity is observed within some ventral and dorsal
regions, indicating the involvement of both networks in task transitions (Dosenbach et al.,
2006).
(B) A meta-analysis of activations across studies measuring reorienting and various aspects of
social cognition. Largely similar TPJ activity is observed across paradigms, with perhaps a
more posterior extension of activity in the social cognition paradigms (Decety and Lamm,
2007).
(C) A within-subject comparison of reorienting and ToM paradigms revealed that both
activated very similar TPJ regions (Mitchell, 2007). The bar graph shows the magnitude of the
TPJ activation in the two paradigms. A large deactivation was observed in the control false-
photograph condition with a significantly smaller deactivation in the experimental ToM
false-beliefs condition. The reorienting paradigm yielded event-related activations that were
larger during trials with invalid than valid cues. The blue and orange regions are taken from
the meta-analysis of the dorsal and ventral networks in Figure 2.
(D) Gaze perception activates superior temporal regions. The graph shows the time course of
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
activity when another person makes or averts eye contact with the observer. Mutual gaze
enhances the activation (Pelphrey et al., 2004).
networks are activated along with other regions) (Shulman et al., 2003). The bottom panel
shows spiking activity in monkey locus coeruleus neurons during analogous periods: an
inattentive period in which a task is poorly performed and tonic activity is high, an attentive
period in which the task is performed well and tonic activity is decreased, and target detection,
which produces a phasic increase in activity (Usher et al., 1999). The inset trace shows event-
related potentials recorded from the scalp of a human when a target is detected in a completely
separate experiment, with the large positive deflection indicating the P300.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Table 1
Ventral Attention Network/P300/LC Activity
Output