A Universal Grammar For Visual Composition (Art)
A Universal Grammar For Visual Composition (Art)
The MIT Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Leonardo.
http://www.jstor.org
THEORETICAL
A
for
PERSPECTIVE
Universal
Visual
Grammar
Composition?
ABSTRACT
PeterD.Stebbing
MY AIM
My aim is to identify the core components of a visual organization
grammar ("composition") for the
art and design curriculum. Criticisms of formalism are as irrelevant
as they would be for all grammars,
The author
hasidentified
four
fundamental
organizational
common
to both
principles
formandthecreation
of
organic
visualcomposition.
Theauthor
thatourperceptual
proposes
to respond
systemhasevolved
to theseprinciples
(perceptual
dueto thenecessity
primitives)
of recognizing
thediversity
of
formsonwhichour
organic
survival
our
depended
during
earlier
evolution.
Theevidence
showsthatthesefourprinciples
occurwidely
huthroughout
mankind's
aesthetic
expression
indifferent
cultures,
epochs,art
formsandmedia.Applying
von
Humboldt's
theauthor
principle,
thattheselimited
proposes
meansprovide
unlimited
possibilitiesfordevelopingstudent
ifitweretaughtas a
creativity
coherent
grammar.
Fig. 1. A summary of the results of the frequency of the most-mentioned terms from the tables of contents of 50 books on visual composition.
? 2004 ISAST
LEONARDO,
2004
63
ANALYTICALINVESTIGATIONPROCEDURE
Authors present the major components
and structure of their books in tables of
contents, and so I created a "mega-glossary"of all the words from the contents
pages of 50 books [10] on visual composition from various art and design subjects.
Analysis 1
Acton,M., 1997
Allen,A.B.,1938
Anderson,D.M.,1961
R., 1974
Arnheim,
Barratt,K., 1980
Birren,F., 1961
Bugg,S., 1997, 1998
Dondis,D.A.,1984
Dow,A.W.,1997
Garrett,L., 1967
Graves,M., 1951
Harlan,C., 1970
Hesselgren,S., 1969
Hofmann,
A., 1965
Howard,C.H.,1926
Itten,J., 1975
Itten,J., 1980
W.,1979
Kandinsky,
Kepes,G., 1995
Kleint,B., 1969
Kunstgewerbeschule,
Zurich,1968
Lauer,D.A.,1979
Lowry,B., 1961
J.J. de,
Lucio-Meyer,
1975
Malins,F., 1980
Mante,H., 1969
J.M.,1993
Matthaei,
Meiss,P.,von, 1996
Owen,P., 1970
Palmer,F., 1988
Pearce,C., 1947
Peters,H., 1994
Popham,J.K.,1954
Read,H., 1963
Rowland,K., 1965,
vols. 1-3
Rowland,K., 1976
Ruder,E., 1981
Ruegg,R, and
Fr6hlich,
G., 1972
de Sausmarez,M.,1980
Schodek,D.L.,1993
Scott, R.G.,1951
Smith,P.F.,1987
Sneum,G., 1965
Taylor,J.F.A.,1964
Thiel,P., 1981
Thompson,T., 1950
Wagner,F.C.,1981
Weber,J., 1975
Wong,W.,1993
Zelanski,P., and
Fisher,M.P.,1988
Subtotals
*
*
*
U
?
U
**
*
*
*
-
2
4
5
1
1
U
U
U
* _
*
*
1
2
2
U
*
*
*
U
*
*
U
U
U
U
U
U
*
*
*
*
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
*
U
4
5
2
U
U
U
U
1
2
2
2
4
0
1
4
3
4
3
2
0
4
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
3
0
1
5
1
2
3
2
4
3
2
5
23
19
3
-120/50 = 2.4
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
U
27
?
31
20
U
U
3
0
0
1
5
5
2
Results
The results revealed the most frequently
occurring terms and supported my hypothesis that CRBP (and their synonyms)
were indeed the terms most commonly
used for visual composition. In addition,
two other words that I had not anticipated appeared prominently: "movement" and "expression." However, these
64
Analysis 2
Shape
Oz\
Form
0~\^
06
Orientation
Size
/-O
Tone
_L]
Dimenion
Transpency
Etc.,etc.,...
components.
CRBP IN OTHERMEDIA
AND MODALITIES
into the occurrence
2. Pattemwith contrastingpauses...
3. Pate
withcont
asing
n
MJ
...........
1. Repetitionor translation
2. Rotation
3. Reflection
4. Glide Reflection
OCCURRENCE OF CRBP
IN OTHER CULTURES
tion is perceived while simultaneously indicating that CRBP and Harmony are
composition's most commonly identified
My investigations
Simplerhythmorpatte........
Number
_E
of
HYPOTHESES
The evidence I provide here suggests that
CRBP may also be universal, since I have
found them in both different media and
different cultures. I therefore developed
the hypothesis that our aesthetic behavior in creating and appreciating visual
compositions has evolved (exapted) from
our innate ability to recognize the diversity of organic forms through the basic organizing principles of Contrast, Rhythm
65
;r
7
a131)8
r'
.B
K
l
?:J
,Sl?'ay.
?,
I .?
L'
"t
nr
I
III
+$^$
>
a"\^L/
fw
wvX
f^TKwt
r ?YL
?I
possessed an organic-form-recognition
system that emancipated them from their
dependence on the food organisms in
the niche of their origin. Clearly,it would
have been biologically inefficient for
them to have evolved feature detectors
for every organism they eat; therefore,
our ancestors' brains must have evolved
an economical (i.e. grammatical) system
for recognizing any kind of organism. Recently published work on word recognition [34] supports the proposition that
the recognition of complex form takes
place through the perception of simple
patterns consistent with the hypotheses
being proposed here.
Although as J.Z. Young has pointed
out, "the genes have to perform a task
analogous to prediction" [35], the evolution of a perceptual-grammatical system enhances this capability by
extending prediction towards novelty. I
propose that the evolution of a CRBP
perceptual-grammaticalsystem gave early
humans the ability to recognize organic
forms, including those never seen before.
(This case parallels that of speech, in
which we are able to understand sentences we have never previously heard
and, furthermore, recognize as patterns
patterns that we have never previously
seen.) This capabilitymade possible both
(1) the earliest migrations, by freeing our
ancestors from their dependence on the
food species of their original niche, and
(2) (later) our aesthetic capability.
our species, then we must ask two questions (among others [29]). Firstly, why
do we possess a sense of visual composition? Secondly, from what ability did this
Fig. 7. Example of the organic occurrence of visual rhythm. Schematic diagrams illustrating
sense evolve?
the assembly and growth of the bacterial flagellar filament. The cap (A) at the growing end is
in
lives
an
Every organism
ecological essential for the assembly of the filament. Monomers travel within the hollow filament to
niche to which it is adapted and con- reach the end and are added sequentially to the filament's end underneath the rotating cap.
fined. The earliest primates, the prosimi- Side views of the assembly are shown in B and schematic views of the exact assembly process
ans, were insectivores, possessing a shown from above and the side in C show "rotation and axial translation of the cap plate and
accompanied rearrangement of the legs upon every incorporation of a flagellin subunit
perceptual system equipped with "bug (from left to right)" [58]. Reprinted with permission from K. Yonekura et al., "The Bacterial
detector" [30] perceptual primitives that Flagellar Cap as the Rotary Promoter of Flagellin Self-Assembly," Science290 (15 December
stimulated feeding behavior; these were 2000) pp. 2,148-2,152. (? 2000 American Association for the Advancement of Science)
(1) a small dark spot that (2) moves.
4
C 2
A
8
However, primate evolution from the
lemur to the chimpanzee is characterized
by the consumption of an increasing 2
_4 1-_^
4t'4
range of food species [31]. Furthermore,
the increase in the body size of the genus 8
Homo[32] supports the hypothesis for an
.
increase in the variety of diet during the
. ..
I
evolution of Homo from the australopithecines. Our ancestors became non2
specialist feeders par excellence, a
development that was probably an adaptation to climatic changes forcing early
3
humans to seek (new) food organisms in
new environments. Today, humans are
omnivores with an unspecialized diet
rf_D
[33].
When humankind's ancestors spread
6
around the world, they must have already
Stebbing,A Universal Grammar
67
risk of
predation
sexual
attractiveness
/%
greatest
greatest
Chancesof
survival
Reproductive
potential
least
\
small
size
least
large
elephant
1000_
bull
boar: ii;
.
man .:;:; cow
chimpanzemei8.'
woman
.:t:i
s doo
'"
at
100...
ing
10-
macaque
hen
henis.Nrabbit
marmot
dove .,J:i:'
1- small
birds .:
.*
rat
:," mouse
0.1
0.01
0.1
10
100
Body mass (kg)
.d
starfish
size
rough
seas
/A
Fig. 8. Twoexamplesof the organicoccurrence of balance.(a, top) A balancebetween
contrastingselectionpressuresdetermines
the lengthof the Arguspheasant'stail.The
lengthof the tailis a balancebetweentwo
selectivepressures:(1) long tailsmakethe
pheasantan easierpreyfor the fox to catch
and so selectiondue to predationresultsin
pheasantswithshortertails.However,(2)
the femaleis attractedto maleswithlong
tales and thereforesexualselectionencourages longertailsin the population(see
Lorenz[59]). (b, bottom)Similarly,a balance betweencontrastingselectionpressures
determinesthe fecundityof seashoreanimals. Organismsare usuallymore fecund
the largertheyare;however,on the
seashore,largerorganismsare in dangerof
beingsweptaway.Therefore,a successful
seashoreorganismbalancesthese contrasting pressuresby growinglargeenoughto be
as fecund as possiblewhileremainingsmall
enoughto withstandthe strongestwavesit is
most like to experience.Starfishfecundityis
a balancebetweenat least twopressures:
stormstrengthand frequency,and their
increasingsize (drawnafterDenny[60]).
Mithen has proposed that several different kinds of intelligence evolved in our
ancestors (including social, technical and
natural-history intelligences adapted to
specific behaviors). These intelligences
became more interactive and subsequently merged to facilitate the development of "cognitive fluidity," eventually,
Mithen suggests, enabling the creation of
"art"[36]. I propose that a similar cognitive fluidity underlies our ability not only
to recognize organisms not previously
seen with variable constitutions of CRBP
but also to aesthetically use CRBP across
different modalities and media.
The ability to recognize organic form
also requires an aesthetic/emotional reward system, because, as Boyden states:
A feature of hunting and foodgathering... is the fact thatthese activitiesappearto be generallyenjoyedbythe
Thisis to be expected,since
participants.
in nature all animalsenjoy behaviours
68
GRAMMATICAL PRINCIPLES,
NATURAL SYNTAX AND
(d
P.
a a
10,000
2000
CREATIVITY
I posit that von Humboldt's observation
with regard to language, that limited
means provide unlimited possibilities
[40], is a principle of natural creative and
organic systems, of which language is a
special case. These generative systems all
display the characteristics of limited
means for either the creation or perception of unlimited variety, e.g.:
* there are only 4 basic forces in the
universe
* there are 12 particles in the Standard
Model
* there are only 4 bases in DNA to code
for the 20 amino acids required to
create organisms
* there are only 4 symmetryoperations
from which all patterns are generated
* higher plants have only 3 possible
leaf growth patterns.
Our own perceptual systems of color
and taste also employ limited means.
Pinker [41] cites Chomsky concerning
our linguistic ability to understand sentences never previously heard. Similarly,
we can also recognize novel patterns as
patterns. It is when we consider the ubiquity of von Humboldt's principle that we
can recognize the natural grammatical
strategy of using limited means (e.g.
CRBP) to recognize organic form, which
has also provided us with our generative
aesthetic grammar.
As one of nature's products ourselves,
we cannot doubt its creativity despite or
rather because of the rules with which it
operates. The art of the last century [421,
characterized by the idea of "artas freedom," may not appear to have been very
creative when compared with nature's
products, produced by rule-based strategies. Many have recognized the creative
significance of limitation, including Arnheim [43], Braque [44], Riley [45],
Welles [46], Goethe [47], Rowlands [48],
da Vinci [49] and finally Westrup, who
1000
I
-
i!iiiii;
.
.... :.~..~:~11ii!ii1i
1000
.........
l
l
4o
0
10
15
20
30
40 50
70
100
C. Harlan, Visionand Invention,A Coursein Art Fundamentals (Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall,
1970).
CONCLUSION
I conclude that our species shares a basic
sense of composition (possibly with some
other primates [54]), exapted from our
innate ability to recognize organic form.
The adaptation of our perceptual system
to the recognition of the principles of organic organization (as perceptual primitives) has resulted in our favoring the
same principles for aesthetic composition. This knowledge provides a strategy
for art and design educators, because
what has appeal for us has been determined by our evolutionary past. We have
apparently evolved a cognitive fluidity for
responding to organic novelty based on
permutations of universal principlesContrast, Rhythm, Balance and Proportion-that are simultaneously embedded
in our own biology. If we ignore the constants of the human condition and follow
only fashions and technology, are we in
danger of losing ourselves in a meaningless texture of background noise? I profoundly believe that human creative
potential has more to offer than that.
Contrast, Rhythm (pattern), Balance
(symmetry) and Proportion are embedded in the laws of nature, which has
shown us that they can also provide us
with an aesthetic meta-grammarwith similarly unlimited creative possibilities.
C.H. Howard, Design (London: John Lane the Bodley Head, 1926).
J. Itten, Design and Form(London: Thames & Hudson, 1978).
P. Zelanski and M.P. Fisher, TheArt of Seeing(Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1988).
F. Malins, UnderstandingPaintings:
TheElements
of Composition (London: Phaidon, 1980).
3. P. Ball, "Science in Culture," a review of the exhibition More Than Meets the Eye, at the Victoria and
Albert Museum, 6-30 September 2000, Nature407,
No. 6800 (7 September 2000) p. 20.
H. Peters, Bildnerische
Grundlehre
(Stuttgart,Germany:
Enke Verlag, 1994).
F.C.Wagner, GrundlagenderGestaltung-plastischeund
rdumlicheDarstellungsmittel(Stuttgart, Germany: Verlag W. Kohlhammer, 1981).
APPENDIX A
69
15. G. Rowley, Principlesof ChinesePainting (Princeton, NJ: Princeton Univ. Press, 1970).
16. Dong-sung Cho, "Four Stages of Design Revolution: Expansion of the Design Domain and the Development of Grand Principles of Design," in
Exploring EmergingDesign Paradigm, Proceedingsof
ICSID2001 Seoul Conference(Oullim), (Seoul: ICSID,
2001) pp. 84-152.
17. P. Stevens, Handbookof Regular Patterns (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1984).
18. D.K. Washburnand D.W. Crowe, Symmetries
of Culture (Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press,
1992).
19. S.J. Gould and E. Vrba, "Exaptation: A Missing
Term in the Science of Form," Paleobiology8 (1981)
pp. 4-15.
20. D.G. Stork, "Preadaptation and Principles of Organization in Organisms," in Principlesof Organization in Organisms,Proceedings of the Workshop on
Principles of Organisation
70
Glossary
The working definitions that I have adopted are deliberately simple, pragmatic and holistic.
balance and symmetry-two or more elements or
forces set against (opposing) each other so that they
equalize or neutralize their tensions, often resulting
in a symmetry of form (see Fig. 5).
contrast-I use Bateson's definition [55] of a contrast as a difference that makes a difference and can
be identified by any of our senses (see Fig. 2).
exaptation or preadaptation-"used to describe the
process by which an organ, behavior, neural structure, etc., which evolved to solve one set of tasks is
later utilized to solve a different set of tasks" [56].
proportion-a ratio composed of two or more contrasting quantities used repeatedly in either the same
and/or different measures in a design (see Fig. 6).
rhythm or pattern-a repetition of a contrast also occurring in one or a combination of the four basic
symmetry operations (see Figs 3 and 4).