Theories of Mathematic
Theories of Mathematic
During that same time there existed other views of knowledge and
learning. In 1916 Dewey said that "It is that reconstruction or
reorganization of experience which adds to the meaning of experience,
and which increases ability to direct the course of subsequent experience"
(p.89). In another occasion Dewey (1938) wrote that "I use the word
understanding rather than knowledge because ...knowledge to so many
people means 'information.' Information is knowledge about things (it is
static), and there is no guarantee that understanding-the spring of
Children are natural learners and the environment both social and physical
offers them many opportunities to acquire notions of quantity. Even in very
poor or diverse cultures, races or classes, children have the opportunity to
acquire quantitative notions (Gelman 1980; Ginsburg, Posner &
Russel1,1981; Ginsburg & Russell, 1981).
Each healthy human brain, no matter the age, sex, race or culture, comes
equipped with a set of unique features: the ability to detect patterns and
to make approximations, a capacity for various types of memory, the
ability to self-correct and learn from experience and external data and self
reflection, and an great capacity to create (Came & Caine 1994). Because
of this predisposition of the brain, children and adults constantly search for
ways to make sense and make connections. This can be translated into a
search for common patterns and relationships as Hiebert and Carpenter
(1992) propose.
Caine and Caine (1994) argue that brain research confirms that multiple
complex and concrete experiences are essential for meaningful learning
and teaching. They add that the brain is designed as a "pattern detector"
and that the function of educators should be to provide students with the
kind of experiences that enable them to perceive "the patterns that
connect."
Children from a very young age are sensitive to quantity. They perceive
differences in number; they see correlation among different numbers of
events; their actions contain quantity and they use words referring to
basic mathematical events (Gelman, 1980; Ginsburg, 1989). Various
studies (Ginsburg & Baron, 1993; Starkey & Cooper, 1980; Van de Walle &
Watkins, 1993) have pointed out the importance of taking into account
children's informal mathematical connections as building block for formal
mathematics. Ginsburg (1989) suggests that students need to learn that it
is acceptable, "even desirable", for them to connect conventional
arithmetic with their own informal knowledge, intuition and invented
procedures.
In a study by Carpenter, Ansell, Franke, Fennema and Weisbeck (1993),
the results suggest that children can solve a wide range of problems,
including problems involving multiplication and division, much earlier than
is generally presumed. With only a few exceptions, children's strategies
could be characterized as representing or modeling the action or
relationships described in the problem. These researchers conclude that
young children's problem-solving abilities have been seriously
underestimated. They suggest that if from an early age children are
motivated to approach problem solving as an effort to make sense out of
problem situations, they may come to believe that learning and doing
mathematics involves solving problems in a way that always makes sense.
Ross and Kurtz (1993) offers the following suggestions when planning a
lesson involving the use of manipulatives. He suggests that the
mathematics teacher should be certain that:
In the last few years there have been studies about the idea of students'
constructing their own mathematical knowledge rather than receiving it in
finished form from the teacher or a textbook (Carpenter, Ansell, Franke,
Fennema, Weisbeck, 1993; Markovits & Sowder, 1994). A crucial aspect of
students' constructive processes is their inventiveness (Piaget, 1973).
Children continually invent ways of dealing with the world. Many of the
errors they make can be
suggests a similar program in the Netherlands and Baker & Baker (1991)
in Australia.
Various studies have been made in the area of invented strategies. Cook
and Dossey's (1982) findings show that children learn number facts easily
and quickly and recall them better when using a strategy approach than
when using a learned algorithm, drill or practice approach. Browne (1906);
Howe and Ceci (1979); Kouba (1989); Rathmill (1978);
Sowder and Wheeler (1989) have done studies on strategies used for
calculation. Carpenter and Moser (1984) found that children in the United
States ordinarily invent a series of abbreviated and abstract strategies to
solve addition and subtraction problems during their first four years in
school. Romberg and Collis (1987) found that even though some children
are limited by their capacity to handle information, most are able to solve
a variety of problems by inventing strategies that have not been taught.
English (1991) observed that in a study of young children's combinatoric
strategies, a series of six increasingly sophisticated solution strategies
were identified. A significant number of children independently adopted
more efficient procedures as they progressed on the task.
Sowder and Schappelle (1994) suggest that there are common elements
found in classrooms that help children acquire good number sense:
1.
Sense-making is emphasized in all aspects of mathematical learning
and instruction.
2.
The classroom climate is conducive to sensemaking. open
discussions about mathematics occurs both in small groups and with the
whole class.
3.
Mathematics is viewed as the shared learning of an intellectual
practice. This is more than simply the acquisition of skills and information.
Children learn how to make and defend mathematical conjectures, how to
reason mathematically and what it means to solve a problem.
Mental Computation
According to Boulware (1950) mental arithmetic has its origin during the
second quarter of the nineteenth century. The idea of building a broader
foundation of meaning and understanding in arithmetic gave rise to
Mental Arithmetic as it was known in the middle of the nineteenth century
with Warren Colburn (1841) considered as pioneer in the field of mental
arithmetic. Before his time, arithmetic had reached a point of extreme
abstraction according to Boulware. The second half of the century
witnessed the decline in interest and understanding of the purpose of
mental arithmetic. With the coming of more writing paper, cheap pencils,
with the rise of industry and its accompanying needs for persons skilled in
computation, the practical or computational phase of arithmetic took on
importance around the turn of the century. The emphasis in arithmetic at
that time was the teaching of isolated facts, followed by drill upon these
facts. High among the purposes stated for the study of arithmetic many
authors of the time placed speed, memory and accuracy by mechanical
rules. There was an emphasis in arithmetic on drill for perfection and
automatic response at the expense of meaning and understanding. In
1950, a dissertation by Boulware is representative of the quest for the
development of "meaning" in mental computation stirred by Brownwell
(1935), who urged that meaning and seeing sense in what is being learned
should be the central focus of arithmetic instruction. Boulware's
conception of mental computation is as follows: Mental arithmetic deals
with number as a unified, consistent system, and not as an aggregate of
unrelated facts. [It] consists of methods of dealing with numerical
situations whereby a clear concept of the number system may be
conceived and utilized in quantitative thinking. It proceeds to the analysis
of number combinations by processes of meaningful experiences with
concrete numbers, reflective thinking in number situations, seeing
relationships, and discovery of new facts as an outgrowth of known facts
(pp.7-8). In 1960, in an article by Sister Josefina there seems to begin
interest in mental computation and in the 1978 NCTM yearbook on
computational skills there appears an article by Trafton (1978) where the
need for including proficiency with estimation and mental arithmetic as
goals for the study of computation is presented. A good number of studies
and articles about mental computation appeared in the period of the
1980s (e.g. Reys, R.E., 1984, 1985; Reys, B. J., 1985a, 1985b; Madell,
1985; Hope, 1985, 1986, 1987; Reys & Reys, 1986; Langford, 1986;
Markovits and Sowder, 1988; Baroody, 1984, 1985, 1986, 1987, 1989 and
others) leading up to the statement of the inclusion of mental computation
as an area where increased attention is needed in school mathematics by
the NCTM (1989). With the increase of studies in cognitive skills and
number sense (e.g.Simon, 1979; Resnick, 1986; Silver, 1987; Schoenfeld,
1987; Greeno, 1980; Sowder, 1988) and more recent studies mentioned in
this chapter, mental computation is suggested to be related to number
sense, needed for computational estimation skills and considered a higher
order thinking skill.
In a study by Reys, Reys and Hope (1993) they argued that the low mental
computation performance reported in this study most likely reflected
students' lack of opportunity to use mental techniques they constructed
based on their own mathematical knowledge. The study of Reys, Reys,
Nohda and Emori (1995) assessed attitude and computational preferences
and mental computation performance of Japanese students in grades 2, 4,
6, and 8. A wide range of performance on mental computation was found
with respect to all types of numbers and operations at each grade level.
The mode of presentation (visual or oral) was found to significantly affect
performance levels, with visual items generally producing higher
performance. The strategies used to do mental computation were limited,
with most subjects using frequently a mental version of a learned
algorithm.
70 + 12 = 82
Beishuizen's analysis indicates that N10 strategies are more powerful, but
that many weaker students used only 1010 strategies. The study's
findings also suggest that instruction involving the hundreds board can
have a positive influence on a student's acquisition of N10 strategies.
Fuson and Briars (1990) and others have also identified these strategies.
According to Reys et al. (1995) there have been many studies that suggest
the benefit of developing mental computation strategies. Mental
computation has also been highlighted in the Curriculum and Evaluation
Standards for School Mathematics (NCTM, 1989).
significantly less time than did a group using concrete materials in grades
2 and 3.
Fuson and Fuson (1992) found that in all of the groups studied, children
were accurate and fast at counting up for subtraction as at counting on for
addition. This contrasts with the usual finding that subtraction is much
more difficult than addition over the whole range of development of
addition and subtraction solution strategies. Sequence counting on and
counting up according to Fuson and Fuson are abbreviated counting
strategies in which the number words represent the addends and the sum.
In both strategies the counting begins by saying the number word of the
first addend. For example: 7 + 5, a child would say 7 pause 8, 9, 10, 11,
12 (up to five numbers, the last number of the sequence is the answer)
and 12 - 5 would be, 5, pause 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12, seven numbers were
counted, which is the answer. Thornton's (1990) study provides evidence
that children who were given an opportunity to learn a counting up
meaning for subtraction as well as counting down (counting back from
minuend), preferred the counting up meaning.
games can be effective for more than drill and practice and for more than
low level learning of skills and concepts,
games can be used along with other instructional methods to teach higher
level content such as problem solving,
games should probably be used relatively soon before or after instruction
planned by the teacher for the same material,
the use of more challenge, fantasy or curiosity might enhance the
effectiveness of instructional games.
Hestad (1991) found that the use of a card game was effective for third
grade students in introducing new mathematical concepts and maintaining
skills.
We can learn more about how invisible components in the teaching and
learning situation can contribute to or detract from the quality of the
lessons, problem sets, and topics, although topics not included in the texts
were only occasionally added to the instructional program.
Research suggests that teachers are "gatekeepers" (Thornton, 1991) who
make their own decisions about which parts of a textbook to use and
which ways to use them (Barr & Sadow, 1989) and such decisions may not
necessarily lead according Brophy (1982) to close adherence to the
textbook material.
what is best for their students and within the limits of their own
knowledge, time and energy." (p.15)
Koehler and Grouws (In Grouws, 1992) have suggested that teachers'
behavior is influenced by their knowledge of: the mathematics content
being taught, how students might learn or understand that particular
content and of the methods of teaching of that particular content. Also
influencing teachers' behaviors are teachers' attitudes and beliefs about
teaching and mathematics.
[Pgina Anterior]
[Previous page]