Myths Realities
Myths Realities
As Samway and McKeon (2007) have noted, “a body of myths” or “urban legends” have
been associated with ELLs and their education. They have identified fifty-eight myths about
ELLs that fall into ten categories: demographics, enrollment, native language instruction,
second language acquisition, literacy, placement, assessment, programming, staffing and
staff development, and parent and community involvement. Espinosa (2008) has focused
on six “commonly held beliefs” about young ELLs or “dual-language learners.” Other
researchers have discussed myths related to language acquisition (McLaughlin, 1992),
second language learning (Snow, 1992), and bilingual education (Crawford, 1998). NCTE
(2008) has also highlighted several myths about ELLs in a research brief. While it is beyond
the scope of this project to attempt to identify and debunk every possible myth or
misperception about ELLs, it is possible to try to summarize and dispel some of the most
common misconceptions noted in the literature that are associated with the ELL
population (Crawford, 1998; Espinosa, 2008; McLaughlin, 1992; NCTE, 2008; Samway &
McKeon, 2007; Snow, 1992).
Myth #1: The number of ELL students is decreasing and most of them are foreign-
born and recent arrivals.
Reality: As evidenced by data from the U.S. 2000 Census and other sources (cited in
Samway & McKeon, 2007), the number of ELLs is rapidly increasing in the United States.
Also, more than 50% of ELLs have been born in this country, 24% of PreK-5 children are
foreign-born, and the vast majority of ELLs are from Spanish-speaking backgrounds
(Center for Public Policy, 2007; Matthews & Ewen, 2006). ELL children may be immigrants,
refugees, undocumented immigrants, members of indigenous Native American tribes, or
citizens of U.S. territories (i.e., American Samoa, Guam, Puerto Rico).
Myth #2: Students who are not legal residents cannot be enrolled in school.
Reality: Equal access to public education has been mandated by law for undocumented
immigrant children since a Supreme Court decision in1982 (Plyer v. Doe, 457 U.S. 202)
under the Equal Protection Clause of the Fourteenth Amendment. The U.S. Department of
Education-Office of Civil Rights (2000), as well as every state, has provided specific
guidelines regarding the access to education for ELLs, including undocumented immigrants,
and other underrepresented groups. These students are also required to attend primary
and secondary schools until they reach a mandated age.
Myth #3: ELLs should not use their native languages in the home, social settings, or
the classroom because this will impact their ability to learn English effectively.
Commissioned by the Pennsylvania Departments of Education and Public Welfare, Office of Child Development and Early Learning,
June 2009. Adapted by Lisa C. Buenaventura, Ed.D.
Reality: Research on bilingualism suggests that use of the native or primary language (L1)
can mediate content-area instruction in English (L2), when L1 is used for cognitive and
academic skill development (Cummins & Swain, 1996; Hakuta, 1986; Handscombe, 1994;
Ovando & Collier, 1985). Note that the use of dual or bilingual approaches assumes that the
learner has acquired proficiency in speaking, reading, writing, and listening in L1. If the
learner does not have L1 proficiency in reading and writing, then achieving L2 (second
language) proficiency will be much more challenging (Cummins, 2000).
Myth #4: Once ELLs are able to speak fluently, they have successfully acquired
English.
Myth #5: Reading and writing instruction should be delayed until ELLs are proficient
in English.
Reality: Exposure to meaningful literacy practices in reading and writing can facilitate the
development of English language skills. Several studies have shown that ELLs can read and
write before achieving oral fluency (Hans & Ernst-Slavin, 1999; Samway, 1993; Taylor,
1990), and that reading and writing can support aural and oral language development
(Samway & Taylor, 1993). At the same time, reading and writing abilities can be supported
through authentic oral language experiences (Samway & Whang, 1996; Uzria, 1987).
Myth #6: ELLs should be placed in Special Education programs for language services.
Commissioned by the Pennsylvania Departments of Education and Public Welfare, Office of Child Development and Early Learning,
June 2009. Adapted by Lisa C. Buenaventura, Ed.D.
been conducted in the native language, per Public Law 94-142 (Education for All
Handicapped Children Act), now amended as the Individuals with Disabilities Act (IDEA).
Research shows that there has been disproportionate over-representation as well as
under-representation of ELLs in special and gifted education programs (Donovan & Cross,
2002). IDEA has provided specific requirements for gathering and analyzing statewide data
about the representation of ELLs in Special Education. In addition, the Council for
Exceptional Children (CEC), U.S. Office of Special Education Programs, and the National
Association for Bilingual Education (NABE) have guidelines available for educators about
special education placement and assessment (Samway & McKeon, 2007).
Myth #7: No Child Left Behind (NCLB) does not require ELLs to be tested for Annual
Yearly Progress (AYP) because they do not speak English proficiently.
Reality: Schools, districts, and states are required by NCLB (2001) to include ELLs in state
testing and AYP reporting. They must be tested for listening, speaking, reading, and writing.
They are required to take language arts and math achievement tests, although they can be
excused for language arts if they have been in the country for less than a year; tests can be
conducted in their native languages, if available. Specific statewide assessments are used to
measure the academic progress of ELLs and to meet federal Annual Measures of
Achievement Objectives (AMAOs). Guidelines are provided by the U.S. Department of
Education.
Reality: There is no one specific program or approach that works with all ELLs, as reflected
in the range of ESL and bilingual programming identified by the National Center for English
Language Acquisition (NCELA). Researchers have proposed a variety of strategies,
practices, and programs to address the complex needs of ELLs in educational environments
(Cummins, 1986; Gollnick & Chinn, 2006; Haynes, 2007; Kim, Roehler, & Pearson, 2009;
Lachat, 2004; Nieto, 2000; Nilles & Rios, 2009; Pranksy, 2008; Robles de Melendez & Beck,
2007; Rothstein-Fisch & Trumbull, 2008; Swain, 1986). There are several indicators of
effective ELL programming: high expectations, integrated language and content-area
instruction; concept development in L1; extensive professional development; supportive
school environment; and school leadership (Samway & McKeon, 2007). NCELA (n.d.) has
developed a chart, which reflects a continuum of ELL programming and describes each
type of English-as-a-Second-Language (ESL) or bilingual program model that might be
used. Some models incorporate strategies that emphasize a pull-out approach, while other
models integrate ELLs within the mainstream classroom setting (http://ncela.gwu.edu)
[Note: These models are described in more detail later in this document.]
Commissioned by the Pennsylvania Departments of Education and Public Welfare, Office of Child Development and Early Learning,
June 2009. Adapted by Lisa C. Buenaventura, Ed.D.
Reality: Speaking another language may be helpful, but not necessary because the
language of instruction is English, Nationally, many (if not most) teachers of ELLs are not
bilingual. Fluency and proficiency in another language, whether it is the teacher or a
classroom aide, is an asset. There are early caregivers and educators who are bilingual, but
as the research has shown, the majority of early learning professionals tend to be White
and monolingual (Matthews & Jang, 2006). In any case, it is important that all educational
professionals have ESL preparation and knowledge of culturally responsive pedagogical
practices, considering dramatically increasing numbers of ELLs.
Myth #10: Immigrant and refugee parents and families do not respond to invitations
to participate because they just do not care.
Reality: ELL parents and families may be reluctant to participate because they themselves
lack effective English skills, do not understand the culture of American schools, or may
simply have additional job or family responsibilities that preclude involvement (Samway &
McKeon, 2007). Alternative approaches, which are more culturally sensitive to the needs of
these families, such as using a cultural liaison or sending information home in the native
language (Trumbell, Rothstein-Fisch, Greenfield & Quiroz, 2001), may produce better
participation.
Myth #11: Younger ELLs absorb a second language (L2) more quickly and more
easily than older ELLs.
Reality: While younger ELLs may appear to learn a second language (L2) more easily,
much of their learning activities involve concrete representations of language or multi-
sensory cues in, what Cummins (1981a, 1981b) has called, “context-embedded” settings.
Cummins (1986, 2000) refers to the initial communication skills of beginning ELLs as Basic
Interpersonal Communication Skills (BICS). BICS provide the basic language structure for
communication and reflect the type of social language heard on the playground, at l unch,
and in other social situations. This level of communication involves simple words, phrases,
and sentences, and may appear to suggest that young ELLs have achieved language
proficiency, when that is not the case. Citing several European and Canadian studies, Snow
(1992) has reported that older children demonstrated better L2 acquisition and
proficiency in school immersion programs than younger children, except in the area of
pronunciation, where younger children sounded more like native speakers. Effective L2
acquisition and proficiency reflect what Cummins has labeled as Cognitive Academic
Language Proficiency (CALP), or the English used for instruction, textbooks, academic
writing, higher order thinking, and more abstract and complex forms of linguistic
communication (Cummins, 1986, 2000).
Commissioned by the Pennsylvania Departments of Education and Public Welfare, Office of Child Development and Early Learning,
June 2009. Adapted by Lisa C. Buenaventura, Ed.D.
Myth #12: Second language (L2) acquisition is the same for all children.
Reality: Snow (1992) has suggested several factors that influence L2 acquisition and
learning, including social class, peer or sibling influence, culture, home and school
environments, and instructional practices. Cummins (1986, 2000) has theorized that there
are two forms of proficiency: BICS--social language and CALP--cognitive academic
language. In addition, other researchers have proposed five stages of second language
acquisition (Krashen & Terrell, 1981) through which ELLs proceed, some earlier stages
which last a few months to other later stages which may last several years. At the very
least, researchers in the field of second language acquisition estimate that it takes at least
five to seven years, at a minimum, to attain English language proficiency.
Commissioned by the Pennsylvania Departments of Education and Public Welfare, Office of Child Development and Early Learning,
June 2009. Adapted by Lisa C. Buenaventura, Ed.D.